Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

27
RESEARCH ARTICLE Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with Synaptic Plasticity Sadra Sadeh 1,2 * ¤ , Claudia Clopath 2, Stefan Rotter 11 Bernstein Center Freiburg & Faculty of Biology, University of Freiburg, Freiburg im Breisgau, Germany, 2 Bioengineering Department, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom ¤ Current address: Department of Neuroscience, Physiology and Pharmacology, University College London, London, United Kingdom These authors are joint senior authors on this work. * [email protected] Abstract In rodent visual cortex, synaptic connections between orientation-selective neurons are un- specific at the time of eye opening, and become to some degree functionally specific only later during development. An explanation for this two-stage process was proposed in terms of Hebbian plasticity based on visual experience that would eventually enhance connec- tions between neurons with similar response features. For this to work, however, two condi- tions must be satisfied: First, orientation selective neuronal responses must exist before specific recurrent synaptic connections can be established. Second, Hebbian learning must be compatible with the recurrent network dynamics contributing to orientation selectivity, and the resulting specific connectivity must remain stable for unspecific background activity. Previous studies have mainly focused on very simple models, where the receptive fields of neurons were essentially determined by feedforward mechanisms, and where the recurrent network was small, lacking the complex recurrent dynamics of large-scale networks of excit- atory and inhibitory neurons. Here we studied the emergence of functionally specific con- nectivity in large-scale recurrent networks with synaptic plasticity. Our results show that balanced random networks, which already exhibit highly selective responses at eye open- ing, can develop feature-specific connectivity if appropriate rules of synaptic plasticity are in- voked within and between excitatory and inhibitory populations. If these conditions are met, the initial orientation selectivity guides the process of Hebbian learning and, as a result, functionally specific and a surplus of bidirectional connections emerge. Our results thus demonstrate the cooperation of synaptic plasticity and recurrent dynamics in large-scale functional networks with realistic receptive fields, highlight the role of inhibition as a critical element in this process, and paves the road for further computational studies of sensory processing in neocortical network models equipped with synaptic plasticity. PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 1 / 27 a11111 OPEN ACCESS Citation: Sadeh S, Clopath C, Rotter S (2015) Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with Synaptic Plasticity. PLoS Comput Biol 11(6): e1004307. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 Editor: Boris S. Gutkin, École Normale Supérieure, College de France, CNRS, FRANCE Received: October 16, 2014 Accepted: April 30, 2015 Published: June 19, 2015 Copyright: © 2015 Sadeh et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files. Funding: Funded by the German Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF, grant BFNT 01GQ0830) and the German Research Foundation (DFG, grant EXC 1086). The article processing charge was covered by the open access publication fund of the University of Freiburg. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Transcript of Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

Page 1: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Emergence of Functional Specificity inBalanced Networks with Synaptic PlasticitySadra Sadeh1,2*¤, Claudia Clopath2‡, Stefan Rotter1‡

1 Bernstein Center Freiburg & Faculty of Biology, University of Freiburg, Freiburg im Breisgau, Germany,2 Bioengineering Department, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom

¤ Current address: Department of Neuroscience, Physiology and Pharmacology, University College London,London, United Kingdom‡ These authors are joint senior authors on this work.* [email protected]

AbstractIn rodent visual cortex, synaptic connections between orientation-selective neurons are un-

specific at the time of eye opening, and become to some degree functionally specific only

later during development. An explanation for this two-stage process was proposed in terms

of Hebbian plasticity based on visual experience that would eventually enhance connec-

tions between neurons with similar response features. For this to work, however, two condi-

tions must be satisfied: First, orientation selective neuronal responses must exist before

specific recurrent synaptic connections can be established. Second, Hebbian learning must

be compatible with the recurrent network dynamics contributing to orientation selectivity,

and the resulting specific connectivity must remain stable for unspecific background activity.

Previous studies have mainly focused on very simple models, where the receptive fields of

neurons were essentially determined by feedforward mechanisms, and where the recurrent

network was small, lacking the complex recurrent dynamics of large-scale networks of excit-

atory and inhibitory neurons. Here we studied the emergence of functionally specific con-

nectivity in large-scale recurrent networks with synaptic plasticity. Our results show that

balanced random networks, which already exhibit highly selective responses at eye open-

ing, can develop feature-specific connectivity if appropriate rules of synaptic plasticity are in-

voked within and between excitatory and inhibitory populations. If these conditions are met,

the initial orientation selectivity guides the process of Hebbian learning and, as a result,

functionally specific and a surplus of bidirectional connections emerge. Our results thus

demonstrate the cooperation of synaptic plasticity and recurrent dynamics in large-scale

functional networks with realistic receptive fields, highlight the role of inhibition as a critical

element in this process, and paves the road for further computational studies of sensory

processing in neocortical network models equipped with synaptic plasticity.

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 1 / 27

a11111

OPEN ACCESS

Citation: Sadeh S, Clopath C, Rotter S (2015)Emergence of Functional Specificity in BalancedNetworks with Synaptic Plasticity. PLoS Comput Biol11(6): e1004307. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307

Editor: Boris S. Gutkin, École Normale Supérieure,College de France, CNRS, FRANCE

Received: October 16, 2014

Accepted: April 30, 2015

Published: June 19, 2015

Copyright: © 2015 Sadeh et al. This is an openaccess article distributed under the terms of theCreative Commons Attribution License, which permitsunrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in anymedium, provided the original author and source arecredited.

Data Availability Statement: All relevant data arewithin the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding: Funded by the German Ministry ofEducation and Research (BMBF, grant BFNT01GQ0830) and the German Research Foundation(DFG, grant EXC 1086). The article processingcharge was covered by the open access publicationfund of the University of Freiburg. The funders had norole in study design, data collection and analysis,decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing Interests: The authors have declaredthat no competing interests exist.

Page 2: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

Author Summary

In primary visual cortex of mammals, neurons are selective to the orientation of contrastedges. In some species, as cats and monkeys, neurons preferring similar orientations areadjacent on the cortical surface, leading to smooth orientation maps. In rodents, in con-trast, such spatial orientation maps do not exist, and neurons of different specificities aremixed in a salt-and-pepper fashion. During development, however, a “functional”map oforientation selectivity emerges, where connections between neurons of similar preferredorientations are selectively enhanced. Here we show how such feature-specific connectivitycan arise in realistic neocortical networks of excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Our resultsdemonstrate how recurrent dynamics can work in cooperation with synaptic plasticity toform networks where neurons preferring similar stimulus features connect more stronglytogether. Such networks, in turn, are known to enhance the specificity of neuronal re-sponses to a stimulus. Our study thus reveals how self-organizing connectivity in neuronalnetworks enable them to achieve new or enhanced functions, and it underlines the essen-tial role of recurrent inhibition and plasticity in this process.

IntroductionAlthough lacking an orderly map of orientation selectivity (OS) [1–3], an increased connectivi-ty between neurons with similar preferred orientations (POs) has been reported in the visualcortex of adult mice [4–7]. However, such specific connectivity is conspicuously lacking imme-diately after eye opening [6], suggesting that it might be a result of experience-dependent plas-ticity during development. Simulations have indeed corroborated that synaptic plasticity canlead to the formation of functionally specific networks [6].

The insight into the neuronal mechanisms that can be derived from those numerical studiesis, however, still somewhat limited: First, the results were reported only for very small ensem-bles of neurons that lacked the rich recurrent dynamics of realistic cortical networks. Second,the receptive fields and OS properties were mainly driven by feedforward inputs, and the effectof large recurrent network dynamics in shaping orientation selective responses of neurons wasnot taken into consideration. It remains, therefore, unclear if feature-specific connectivity canat all emerge in more realistic neuronal networks, and whether the recurrent dynamics possiblycompromises the stability of the learned weights.

Here we study this scenario in large-scale recurrent networks of excitatory and inhibitoryneurons. These networks are known for their rich and biologically realistic repertoire of the dy-namics they can exhibit [8, 9]. As a result, it is not clear if the connectivity structure observedexperimentally in adult animals [6] can also be obtained in simulations under these more real-istic conditions. Collective dynamics of a strongly recurrent network of excitatory and inhibito-ry neurons may enhance, but could also impede Hebbian learning and self-organization [10,11]. The dynamics of large plastic networks is difficult to characterize in general [10, 12–22],and there is conflicting evidence whether functional subnetworks can emerge in them under bi-ological conditions. It remains, therefore, unclear if functionally specific connectivity can de-velop in such networks.

Even more challenging is the question whether specific connectivity can be obtained in bal-anced networks of spiking neurons with realistic receptive fields. It has been recently demon-strated that balanced random networks can show highly selective neuronal responses despitereceiving only weakly tuned inputs [23–25]. Here we report that both highly selective neuronalresponses and functionally specific connectivity emerge simultaneously in balanced random

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 2 / 27

Page 3: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

networks with synaptic plasticity of excitatory-excitatory and excitatory-inhibitory recurrentconnections. In addition, the resulting connection specificity is sensitive to the statistics of thestimuli, and an over-representation of one particular orientation leads to an over-representa-tion of functionally specific weights among those neurons that prefer this orientation. Interest-ingly, the emerging functional subnetworks remain stable for spontaneously active (non-stimulated) networks, and the activity of the network in absence of a visual stimulus does notsubstantially change the learned weights.

ResultsOur point of departure is a recurrent network of excitatory and inhibitory neurons, with sparserandom connectivity (Fig 1A). Both the connection probability and the synaptic weights aretaken to be independent of the orientation preference of pre- or post-synaptic neurons. Wenow ask the question whether such a network with plastic recurrent synapses can, through vi-sual experience, develop specific connectivity (Fig 1B), where neurons with similar preferredorientations are more likely and/or more strongly connected than others. To simplify our sim-ulations and analyses, we admit only changes in the strength of synapses here. Structural plas-ticity is not considered here and, hence, the multiplicity of connections is fixed in the networksconsidered (see Materials and Methods). Feedforward plasticity is also not considered, and weassume a fixed weight for the feedforward connections in all our simulations (see Discussion).

Activity-dependent modification of network responsesIn agreement with experiments in mice demonstrating that neurons show orientation selectiveresponses already at eye opening [6], neurons in our networks exhibit selective responses evenbefore synaptic plasticity is turned on in the network, and despite receiving only weakly tunedinputs [6, 23–25]. This is demonstrated here with the example of network activity in responseto a stimulus orientation of 90° (Fig 1C).

We then studied the consequences of turning on synaptic plasticity in our networks. We im-plemented a voltage-based spike-timing dependent plasticity (STDP) rule [26] for excitatory-to-excitatory (E! E), excitatory-to-inhibitory (E! I), and inhibitory-to-excitatory (I! E)connections, while keeping inhibitory-to-inhibitory (I! I) connections non-plastic (see Mate-rials and Methods). This particular learning rule has been shown to faithfully reproduce abroad range of experimental results on pair-based spike-timing-dependent plasticity, STDP[27, 28], voltage-clamp experiments [29, 30]), frequency dependency [31], pair and triplet ex-periments [32], and triplet and quadruplet experiments [33] (see [34] for details). We thenstimulated the plastic network with a random sequence of different orientations, reflectingsome visual experience of this circuit of total duration 80 seconds. The response of the networkat the beginning and at the end of the learning period are depicted in Fig 1D and 1E, respective-ly. The raster plots and histograms of population activity show that plasticity-induced changescause sparser activity in the network in the excitatory population. Individual neurons are selec-tive before and after learning, but after learning they respond with a lower number of spikes,and to a reduced range of stimulus orientations. This is most evident when we freeze the finalweights after learning and stimulate the network with exactly the same input as before learning(compare Fig 1C and 1F). The overall response of the network activity (as reflected by the “net-work tuning curve”) is still selective, but fewer spikes are emitted, and a smaller set of neuronsis now active.

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 3 / 27

Page 4: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

Fig 1. Simulating the effect of synaptic plasticity in balanced random networks. (A) In random networks of excitatory (E; triangles) and inhibitory (I;circles) neurons, synaptic connections are established disregarding the stimulus selectivities (preferred orientation) of pre- and post-synaptic neurons. (B) Inspecific networks, synapses between neurons of similar preferred orientations are stronger, while dissimilar feature selectivity of pre- and post-synapticneurons imply weaker synapses between them. (C–F) Stimulus-induced response of a network before, during and after learning. The middle of each panelshows the raster plot of two seconds of stimulation. Spikes of excitatory and inhibitory neurons are displayed in red and blue, respectively. Within eachpopulation, neurons are sorted according to the preferred orientations of their weakly tuned inputs. Average firing rates of individual neurons during the periodof stimulation are shown in the histogram on the right. The average firing rate of each subpopulation is indicated to the right of it, in the corresponding color.The lower panel depicts the time-resolved histograms of population activity for excitatory (red) and inhibitory (blue) neurons, respectively. Population spikecounts are extracted from bins of size 10 ms. The colored bar at the bottom of the main panel shows the sequence of stimulus orientations applied during thesimulation (color code is indicated between panels (D) and (E)). For the simulations before (C) and after (F) learning, the initial or final weights are frozen,respectively, and network activity is simulated with static weights in response to a stimulus of orientation 90°. During learning, a network with plasticsynapses is stimulated with 40 batches of oriented bar stimuli. Each batch consists of a random sequence of 20 different stimulus orientations, each stimuluslasting for 100 ms. Therefore, the “visual experience” lasts 20 × 20 × 0.1 s = 40 s in total. The responses to the first and the last batch are shown in (D) and(E), respectively.

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g001

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 4 / 27

Page 5: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

Individual tuning curves before and after experience-dependentplasticityChanges in neuronal responses as a result of learning can be further illustrated by individualtuning curves (Fig 2). We obtained such tuning curves by stimulating the network before andafter learning (Fig 1C and 1F, respectively), for 8 different stimulus orientations. Tuning curveswere extracted from the average firing rates of neurons in response to each orientation. Shownare a sample excitatory and a sample inhibitory neuron in Fig 2A and 2B, respectively, and theaverage tuning curves of the respective subpopulations in Fig 2C and 2D, respectively. Such

Fig 2. Orientation selectivity before and after learning. (A) Tuning curves of a sample excitatory neuron from the network before and after learning. Themean firing rate of a neuron for different stimulus orientations was extracted from simulations of network activity for two seconds, as explained for Fig 1C and1F. This was repeated 10 times for each orientation, and the mean and standard deviation over trials of the firing rates are depicted by solid lines andcorresponding shadings, respectively. (B) Same as (A) for a sample inhibitory neuron. (C) All tuning curves of excitatory neurons, similar to the tuning curvein (A), were aligned to the preferred orientation of the input, and the average tuning curves (over 180 bins) before and after learning were computed fromthem. The solid line indicates the mean value in each bin, and the shading is ± one standard deviation. The orientation selectivity index indicated in the plot(OSI, see Materials and Methods) was computed from the average tuning curves, respectively. (D) Same as (C) for the inhibitory population. (E) The OSI wascomputed for each individual tuning curve, and its distribution for the entire excitatory population is shown for a network before (solid) and after (outlined)learning. The average OSI in the network is indicated for each case. (F) Same as (E), for the inhibitory population.

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g002

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 5 / 27

Page 6: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

average tuning curves are obtained from averaging over individual tuning curves each alignedto its respective input preferred orientation.

Overall, tuning curves show a slight enhancement of selectivity, as quantified by the orienta-tion selectivity index, OSI (see Materials and Methods). The excitatory population respondswith less firing rate, consistent with our observation above that plasticity leads to sparser andmore selective network responses in this case. Enhancement of orientation selectivity, for indi-vidual cells, also shows up in the distribution of OSI of individual tuning curves before andafter learning (Fig 2D and 2E, for excitatory and inhibitory populations, respectively).

This enhancement of tuning in excitatory neurons is consistent with the results reported byKo et al. [6], who demonstrated a slight enhancement of OSI after eye opening (an increasefrom 0.62 to 0.68, see supplementary Figure 2 therein). Another experimental study [35] has infact reported a larger (almost two-fold) enhancement of OSI (from� 0.4 to 0.8, see Figure 4Btherein) during development. Note, however, that a different measure of orientation selectivity(i.e. (Rpref − Rorth)/(Rpref + Rorth), where Rpref and Rorth are firing rates at preferred and orthogo-nal orientations, respectively) were employed in these two experimental studies, as opposed tothe “global”measure of orientation selectivity (1−circular variance, see Materials and Methods)that we have used here [36, 37]. A very recent study [38] has in fact computed both measuresof orientation selectivity and found a significant enhancement of OSI for both measures.

Emergence of feature-specific connectivity through learningThe synaptic learning rule considered here allows for changes in the weights of connections.Comparing the initial random connectivity matrix (Fig 3A) with the connectivity matrix afterlearning reveals some interesting structure within its subpopulations (Fig 3B). This pattern be-comes more salient when the accumulated changes of weights are plotted directly (Fig 3C).One observes an unspecific potentiation (increase in amplitude) of E! I and (I! E) connec-tions (column numbers and row numbers 401 to 500, respectively). This explains the observedsparser responses in the network as the result of a more intense recruitment of inhibitoryneurons.

More noteworthy, however, is the increase in some E! E connections (diagonal band inthe matrix (B) and (C)). As neuron indices are sorted according to the preferred orientation ofthe input to neurons, an increase around the main diagonal indicates a potentiation of connec-tions between neurons with similar preferred orientations. The obvious symmetry of the E!E block implies an over-representation of bidirectional connectivity between these neurons, aswas also shown experimentally [6]. This can be quantified by a normalized measure of weight-ed bidirectional connectivity (WBInorm, see Materials and Methods), which has been increasedhere from 1 (bidirectional connections expected from a random distribution of weights) beforeplasticity to 1.38 (38% over-representation of bidirectional weights compared to random distri-bution) after plasticity. As was noted in a previous small network model [6, 26], with an archi-tecture that was mainly driven by feedforward links, this increase in bidirectional connectivityis a consequence of the employed plasticity rule and would also hold for other, similar nonline-ar rules [39–42]. Symmetric pair-based STDP models [14, 43], however, would not support bi-directional connectivity.

Fig 3D–3E show the distribution of synaptic weights after learning, in relation to the differ-ence in preferred orientation of pre- and post-synaptic neurons (dPO). The clear tuning ofweights for E! E connections (Fig 3D) demonstrates the emergence of specific connections(black) in the network from initial random connections (red). In the case of E! I (Fig 3E) andI! E connections (Fig 3F) the specific enhancement of weights is generally much weaker, re-flecting the aforementioned essentially unspecific potentiation of all connections. The reason

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 6 / 27

Page 7: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

for this is the weaker tuning of neuronal responses in the inhibitory population (Fig 2A–2D),as was also observed experimentally [4, 44, 45]. In our simulations, this is in turn a result of aweaker tuning of feedforward input to inhibitory neurons (see Materials and Methods).

The sharp tuning of E! E weights as a function of the difference between preferred orien-tations (dPO) of the pre- and post-synaptic neurons leads to an over-emphasis of connectionsbetween neurons with similar preferred orientations. If pre-post pairs are split according to thedifference of their preferred orientations to similar (dPO< 30°), indifferent (30°< dPO< 60°)and dissimilar groups (60°< dPO< 90°), a clear trend is now observed (Fig 3G). In contrast torandom networks with a flat profile of average connectivity (before learning, red), after the

Fig 3. Emergence of specific connectivity as a result of synaptic plasticity. (A) Initial connectivity matrix of the random network. Each excitatory neuronis connected to a random sample of 30% of all post-synaptic neurons, with a weight EPSP = 0.5 mV. Inhibitory neurons form synapses of weight IPSP = −4mV. Neurons are sorted according to their preferred orientations (POs) within each population (1–400: excitatory, 401–500: inhibitory). (B)Matrix of synapticweights in the network, after learning. (C) Change in the weights as a result of plasticity and “sensory experience”. The overall weight increase on thediagonal shows the specific potentiation of synapses between pairs of excitatory neurons with similar POs. (D–F) The final weights (black) are plotted againstthe difference in the preferred orientations (dPO) of pre-synaptic and post-synaptic neurons, for E! E(D), E! I (E) and I! E (F) connections, respectively.The initial weights are shown in red, for comparison. (G)Mean of the weight distributions are shown for all synapses between neurons with similar(dPO < 30°), indifferent (30° < dPO < 60°) and dissimilar POs (dPO > 60°). The unspecific distribution of initial weights (red) has now changed to an over-emphasis of connections between neurons with similar POs, indicating the emergence of specific connectivity in the network through synaptic plasticity andvisual experience.

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g003

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 7 / 27

Page 8: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

plastic period in the balanced network pairs of excitatory neurons with similar POs increase,and pairs with dissimilar POs decrease the strength of a synapse between them (after learning,black), fully consistent with experiments [5, 6].

In the results presented above, we considered networks with a connection probability of30% for both E! E and E! I connections. An even higher connection probability for E! Ihas in fact been reported in an experimental study [4]. We therefore studied how such a differ-ence in connectivity affects our results. We found that all our main results, including emer-gence of feature-specific connectivity and sparse orientation selective responses after learning,also hold for the alternative connectivity parameters (S1 Fig). Our results are therefore alsoconsistent with a more experimentally constrained network connectivity. Note, however, thatmatching the parameters of our model networks to real cortical networks might not bestraightforward, as relatively small networks of 500 neurons were considered to speed up thesimulations, and the exact scaling of the connectivity parameters for larger networks might bedifferent. Our conclusions, however, hold for a range of connectivity parameters as long as thenetwork is inhibition-dominated and operates in a balanced activity regime.

Balanced activity regime before and after learningThe balanced change of synaptic weights ensures that balanced regime of activity is maintainedin the network also after learning (Fig 4). In either case, a large excitatory input to the neuronsis counter-balanced by recurrent inhibition of comparable strength, which keeps individualneurons away from saturation, and prevents runaway activity to occur in the network. This isdemonstrated for a sample neuron in response to its preferred orientation before and afterlearning (Fig 4A and 4D, respectively). A 500 ms trace of the actual membrane potential of theneuron is shown, along with the excitatory and inhibitory components of its (spike free) mem-brane potential.

As a result of balanced input to the neuron, its membrane potential remains on averagebelow threshold, and spikes are generated by positive-going fluctuations. The tuning curve ofthe average membrane potential of a sample neuron before and after learning (Fig 4B and 4E,respectively), as well as the average (over network) tuning curves of the membrane potential ofall neurons in the network also reveal the same behavior (Fig 4C and 4F). Both networks, be-fore and after learning, operate in the balanced activity regime. The only difference is that thenetwork after learning shows more hyper-polarized membrane potentials (Fig 4F). This is ex-plained by the potentiated inhibition shown in Fig 4D, as compared to the network beforelearning. This is also fully consistent with sparser and more selective responses of networksafter activity-dependent plasticity.

Orientation selectivity that emerges in balanced networks is very pronounced, even if the se-lectivity of the feedforward input is extremely weak. This is because the untuned component ofthe input is strongly suppressed by balanced networks [23], as a result of selective attenuationof the common-mode due to dominant recurrent inhibition, while the same mechanism doesnot affect the tuned component [25]. This processing is fully contrast-invariant: If the tunedcomponent of the input remains the same while only the untuned component increases (lead-ing to a decrease of the relative modulation depth of the input), balanced networks still main-tain their output selectivity.

This is exactly the scenario when the typical connectivity in the network, C = �N (corre-sponding to the average number of connections each neuron receives in the network, see Mate-rials and Methods), increases. In this case, the untuned component scales with C, while the

modulation scales withffiffiffiffiCp

. As a consequence, the relative modulation amplitude of the input

tuning (denoted by μ in our model, see Materials and Methods), also scales with 1=ffiffiffiffiCp

[23].

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 8 / 27

Page 9: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

We therefore investigated the response of our networks for different input selectivities in thismanner, to see if our networks invariantly respond to more weakly tuned inputs.

To study this situation, we decreased the relative modulation of input to excitatory neurons,μexc, in our networks, while scaling the strength of input such that the absolute modulation am-plitude in the input remained the same (Fig 5). The tuning curves of a sample cell are, in fact,almost the same before and after learning (Fig 5A and 5B). Output selectivity is maintained de-spite a two-fold decrease in input selectivity.

This effect can be better seen, on the level of the entire population, by plotting the averagetuning curve of neurons in the network (Fig 5C and 5D), and the distribution of OSI of individ-ual tuning curves (Fig 5E and 5F). Robust output selectivity is again observed for both net-works, before and after learning. However, although there are some subtle differences amongdifferent tuning curves before learning, the network after learning shows completely identicalresponses to different inputs. Plasticity of excitatory and inhibitory weights, therefore, also ex-tends the invariance of balanced network responses to more weakly tuned inputs.

Dynamics and stability of learning in the networkNext we looked at the dynamics of plasticity in our networks (Fig 6). Following the time evolu-tion of the synaptic weights (averaged over synapses and average per batch) indicates that the

Fig 4. Balanced activity regime before and after learning. (A) The membrane potential trace of the same excitatory neuron shown in Fig 2A in response toits preferred orientation is depicted here for 500 ms. The black trace is its actual membrane potential, with the thin vertical lines denoting spikes when themembrane potential crosses the spiking threshold (the dashed line). The red and the blue traces reflect the excitatory and inhibitory components of the freemembrane potential (by not allowing the neuron to spike), respectively. (B) Temporal average of the membrane potential, hui, over two seconds ofstimulation is extracted for each stimulus orientation and plotted as a tuning curve. The tuning curve of the free membrane potential, ufree, is also computed,by correcting for the total reset voltage induced as a result of spiking, i.e. ufree = u + τm uth r, where r is the mean firing rate of the neuron. (C) The tuningcurves introduced in (B) for a single cell are now computed for the whole network, by aligning the individual tuning curves and computing the mean andstandard deviation of values in each 180 discrete bins (similar to Fig 2C). (D–F) Same as (A–C), respectively, for the network after learning.

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g004

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 9 / 27

Page 10: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

connection weights converge smoothly to their equilibrium values during the period of evokedactivity of the network (Fig 6A). Importantly, the learned weights remains stable during spon-taneous activity: We continued the stimulation with an input that lacks feedforward tuningand has a weaker baseline intensity (reflecting the fact that in the spontaneous state the feedfor-ward input and hence the untuned input induced by it would be weaker). The results showedthat the weights learned in the stimulus-induced state now remain stable during untunedinput, when the network exhibits only sparse spontaneous activity (Fig 6A). We can thereforeconclude that the learned weights generally remain stable even when the corresponding visualstimuli are not present, and govern the evoked responses in future stimulations. Note that thisphenomenon could not have been studied in the small network model mentioned before [6], as

Fig 5. Orientation selectivity of network responses to more weakly tuned inputs. (A) Simulations of the network in Fig 2 (before learning) are repeatedwith the following changes in the parameters: sb (see Eq 2 in Materials and Methods) is scaled by a connectivity factor (C = 1, 2, 4), which emulates aneffective increase in connectivity. The relative modulation of the feedforward input to excitatory neurons, μexc (Eq 2), is reduced by a factor 1=

ffiffiffiffiCp

. Finally, tokeep the absolute size of modulation the same, feedforward synaptic strength is scaled by a factor 1=

ffiffiffiffiCp

. The output tuning curve of the sample neuron in Fig2A for the three different values of C is then plotted. (B) Same as (A) for the network after learning. (C) Average tuning curves of all the cells in the network(extracted similarly to Fig 2C) are plotted for different values of C, along with the OSI computed for each average tuning curve. (D) Same as (C) for thenetwork after learning. (E, F)Distribution of OSI of individual tuning curves (similar to Fig 2E and 2F) for the networks before and after learning, respectively.

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g005

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 10 / 27

Page 11: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

is the networks studied there were mainly driven by feedforward, stimulus-induced input, andwere lacking the realistic recurrent dynamics of large-scale networks we study here.

The convergence of synaptic weights we observed in Fig 6A was for the average weightsover synapses. To obtain more insight on the evolution of synaptic weights on the systemslevel, it is important to also look at the distribution of weights, rather than only on their meanvalues (Fig 6B and 6C). The distribution of weights before and after learning in the evokedstate (first column) and during spontaneous activity (second column) is plotted for E! E, E! I and I! E connections, respectively (Fig 6B). A more complete account of the evolution ofweight distributions can be obtained by inspecting the changes in weights after each batch oflearning (Fig 6C). Both analyses corroborate the previous results on the convergence of themean weights, and the stability of weights under spontaneous activity.

However, in the case of E! E weights, as opposed to other connections, a bimodal weightdistribution emerges out of the initial distribution of weights. This is due to the fact that the

Fig 6. Dynamics and stability of learned weights. (A) Evolution of synaptic weights in the network during plasticity. After each batch of learning, weightsare frozen and compared to the weights in the previous batch. The difference in the weights is averaged over populations, for all synapses, respectively. Onaverage, weight changes decrease over batches, indicating the convergence of the learning process. The weights established under evoked activity are alsostable for spontaneous activity, in absence of any specific stimulation (batch 41 and later, after the vertical dashed line). Here, the plasticity period iscontinued for extra 10 batches, where neurons are only receiving an untuned background input (with an input rate of sb/2 and μ = 0, see Materials andMethods). (B) Distribution of weights are shown separately for E! E, E! I and I! E weights, for evoked and spontaneous states, respectively. In eachcase, two distributions are shown, one sampling the beginning and one sampling the end of the corresponding learning phase. (C) Distribution of weightsextracted at the end of each trial batch (every 2 s) is plotted for all plastic connections in a pseudo-color map. Note the logarithmic scale of the color code.

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g006

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 11 / 27

Page 12: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

learning rule considered here is hyper-competitive. Therefore the weights are typically pushedeither to very high or to very low values. Similar bimodal distributions have previously been re-ported in several other studies [14, 46, 47]. We did not observe such a bimodal distributionwhen we chose the initial (non-zero) excitatory weights from a normal distribution about Jexc(S2 Fig) (for a similar observation, see [14]).

In both cases (Fig 6 and S2 Fig), however, imposing hard bounds on E! E weights seemsnecessary: in Fig 6C, the emergent bimodal distribution is not stable and smaller and largerweights are still changing (decreasing and increasing, respectively). As a result, if learning iscontinued for a longer time, smaller weights converge to zero and larger weights converge tothe maximum weight imposed on the connections. Such a bimodal distribution at wmin andwmax is already visible in S2 Fig.

To further study the issue of dynamics of learning in our networks, we can also investigatewhether the network is sensitive to the statistics of stimulation (Fig 7). To test that, we pre-sented cardinal orientations more frequently to the network such that half of stimuli corre-sponded to either 0° or 90°. We observed that, as a result, the subnetworks corresponding tothese orientations became over-represented in the network, as reflected by its connectivity ma-trix (Fig 7A). Although neurons with similar preferred orientations were still connected morestrongly, the enhancement of connectivity is more pronounced for subnetworks preferringover-represented orientations, here corresponding to the cardinal orientations (Fig 7B). Thus,the Hebbian learning leads to the emergence of feature-specific connectivity, while it remainssensitive to the statistics of stimulation.

Fig 7. Stimulus statistics is reflected in the learned weights. (A) Sensitivity of the final weights to an over-representation of certain stimulus orientations.When the stimulation is repeated with cardinal orientations being more frequent than others (0° and 90° stimuli represent 50% of all stimuli), the final weightsalso reflect this over-representation of the corresponding subnetworks. (B) Total weight change of each presynaptic neuron is plotted vs. its initial preferredorientation (PO). Specifically for the excitatory population, neurons with POs close to 0° and 90° show the highest increase in their synaptic weights.

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g007

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 12 / 27

Page 13: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

Joint emergence of feature selectivity and selective connectivityOur results so far revealed a critical role of inhibition for the initial establishment of orientationselectivity and the ensuing emergence of feature-specific connectivity. The former was gov-erned by the network dynamics alone, and specifically was a result of inhibition-dominance inour networks. For the latter plasticity of certain recurrent synaptic connections was needed.Next we asked the question what would happen if the initial selectivity was not as strong as wehad assumed so far in our simulations underlying Figs 1–7.

To answer this question, we first decreased the dominance of inhibition by a factor of 2(from g = 8 to g = 4). This lead to a network that was not any more dominated by inhibition,rather excitation and inhibition were of equal strength. Under these conditions, the output se-lectivity of the network response was not as strong as before (Fig 8A), and the response wasgenerally less sparse. During learning, however, the responses became gradually sparser andhighly selective (Fig 8B and 8C), and eventually a degree of selectivity was obtained comparableto the case g = 8 considered before (Fig 8D). As a result of synaptic learning in the network, in-hibition became more dominant, which ensured highly selective output responses. Notably,feature-specific connectivity was found to result from this procedure at the same time (Fig 8E–8K). Thus, enhancement of feature selectivity and emergence of feature-specific connectivityjointly result from plasticity in randomly connected recurrent networks, even if they are not, oronly weakly dominated by inhibition.

To study how feature-specific connectivity is related to initial orientation selectivity in ournetworks, we systematically changed the inhibition dominance ratio, g, and measured the selec-tivity of E! E weights after learning in each network (Fig 9). The more dominant inhibitionwas in the beginning, the more selective were the initial network tuning curves (Fig 9A). In allnetworks, some degree of feature-specific connectivity emerged after learning (Fig 9B). Howev-er, the post-learning connections were more specific in networks with initially more selectiveresponses, as revealed by quantifying the selectivity of initial network responses and specificityof final network connections after a similar period of learning (Fig 9C).

Robustness of the resultsThe above result shows that the emergence of feature-specific connectivity is robust with re-gard to the most important parameter of network dynamics, namely inhibition-dominance.We have shown before that the emergence of orientation selectivity is in turn robust to otherparameters of network connectivity (specifically, spatial extent of excitation and inhibition inlocally connected networks) [48], network dynamics (specifically, operating in asynchronousvs. synchronous or mean-driven vs. fluctuation-driven regimes of activity) and single cell prop-erties (specifically, linear or non-linear neuronal transfer functions) [49]. We will thereforeonly focus on the parameters of plasticity for the remainder of the paper and test whether ourresults are also robust to changes in these parameters.

To do this, we changed (either decreased or increased) the major parameters of plasticity inour networks (see Materials and Methods) by 10% of their respective values, and ran the simu-lations again (Fig 10). In all cases, feature-specific connectivity still emerges in the network, asillustrated by the tuning curve of connection weights as a function of the difference betweenpreferred orientations of pre- and post-synaptic neurons, dPO (Fig 10A). This is further quan-tified by computing a selectivity index of this tuning curve, which reveals similar values as forsimulations with the default values for all plasticity parameters (Fig 10B). We conclude that theemergence of feature-specific connectivity is a robust property of our networks, as it is essen-tially invariant to wide changes in network parameters.

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 13 / 27

Page 14: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

Fig 8. Emergence of feature selectivity and feature-specific connectivity. The format of (A–D) is identical to Fig 2, (C–F), and (E–K) identical to Fig 3,respectively, for a network with initial random connectivity and strongly reduced inhibition dominance, g. Instead of g = 8, the inhibition-dominance ratio isnow decreased to g = 4 for the network “before plasticity”. As before, the learning was run for 20 batches.

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g008

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 14 / 27

Page 15: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

We also checked specifically whether our results depend on the learning rates of excitatoryand inhibitory weights, and found that these are again not crucial for our results (S3 and S4Figs). We increased the strength of synaptic potentiation (ALTP; see Materials and Methods) by20% for either excitatory (S3 Fig) or inhibitory (S4 Fig) synapses, while keeping ALTP fixed forthe other type of synapses. This lead to similar results, and we found that the emergence of fea-ture-specific connectivity in our networks is not compromised by different learning rates. Theonly difference was a slightly increased or decreased feature-specificity of connections when

Fig 9. Relation between orientation selectivity and feature-specific connectivity induced by learning. (A) Initial (before learning) response selectivityfor networks with different amounts of inhibition dominance, g. (B) Each network is simulated for 20 batches of learning, and the final selectivity of E! Eweights is illustrated by plotting the weights vs. the difference in the PO of pre- and post-synaptic neurons (pre-post dPO). (C) The selectivity of the initialnetwork tuning curve (A) and the final weight tuning (B) is quantified by computing their OSI (see Materials and Methods) for each value of g. The moreselective the initial orientation selectivity is, the more feature-specificity is observed in the final connectivity.

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g009

Fig 10. Robustness of the results to changes in the plasticity parameters. (A, B) The robustness of the results in the default configuration of the networkis studied by decreasing (A) or increasing (B) 5 parameters of plasticity by 10%. The parameters are ALTD, ALTP, θ−, θ+ and u2

ref , respectively. The learningphase for each network is again organized in 20 batches. (C) Selectivity of the final weights is quantified (blue and red; similar to Fig 9) and compared with theselectivity in the default case (black).

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.g010

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 15 / 27

Page 16: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

the learning rate was larger for excitatory and inhibitory synapses, respectively (S3 and S4 Figs,respectively).

Finally, our results were also robust with respect to the learning rule governing I! E con-nections. Although for simplicity, we have assumed the same plasticity rule governing all theplastic synapses (i.e. E! E, E! I and I! E), employing another plasticity rule for I! E con-nections [50] did not compromise our results (S5 Fig). We implemented this rule in S5 Fig, in-stead of our voltage-based rule before, and choose the parameters such that inhibitoryplasticity ensures an average firing rate of 5 Hz in the excitatory population. Moreover, weused the denser connectivity of E! I connections corresponding to experimental data [4], in-stead of the default parameter. Note that given our parameters before, the connection frequen-cy is now fully constrained by the experimental data (E! E and E! I connections accordingto [4] and I! E and I! I connections according to [51, 52]). To be more realistic, we alsodraw E! E connections from a normal distribution, similar to S2 Fig.

Similar results as before were also obtained in this case (S5 Fig). Initial orientation selectiveresponses became sparser as a result of learning (S5A–S5D Fig), and feature-specific connectiv-ity emerged in E! E connections (S5E–S5K Fig). Synaptic weights showed stable and conver-gent evolution under evoked state as well as spontaneous activity (S5L Fig), while change in thestatistics of stimulus presentation was reflected in the statistics of feature-specific connectivity(S5M Fig). Note that neurons with similar preferred orientations are connected together withthe strongest weights (S5H Fig), consistent with a recent experimental report [53]. This, how-ever, does not render the network fragmenting into disconnected parts, as neurons with similarpreferred orientations are connected together in a continuous manner. Thus, we can concludethat our main results hold in realistic balanced networks fully constrained with experimentallyreported connectivity, and that they are robust to the main parameters of network dynamicsand plasticity, including the I! E plasticity rule.

DiscussionWe analyzed biologically realistic models of sensory cortical networks, which are strongly driv-en by recurrent activity, and the recurrent connections of which undergo synaptic plasticity.We showed that functionally specific and stable connections can arise from an initially randomstructure. Specifically, we could demonstrate that Hebbian synaptic plasticity in balanced ran-dom networks can lead to the emergence of specific and bidirectional connectivity in orienta-tion-selective networks. The initial response selectivity generated by such random networksguides the Hebbian learning process to enhance the connectivity between neurons with similarevoked responses.

If the initial selectivity is absent, or if it is very weak, specific connections, do not emerge, orremain very weak (Fig 9). Therefore, if the recurrent coupling is weak, or if the network is notinhibition-dominated, significant specific connectivity does not emerge. This is an immediateconsequence of too weak initial tuning of output tuning curves. This sequence of events in theemergence of orientation selectivity and specific connectivity during learning is indeed fullyconsistent with the developmental processes, where neurons first show orientation selective re-sponses, and only later functional subnetworks emerge [6].

The plastic mechanisms in our networks would also lead to the joint emergence of feature-specific neuronal responses and feature-specific synaptic connectivity, if the initial featurespecificity was weak. Concretely, if not enough inhibition was provided at the beginning to en-sure sharp and sparse output responses, a potentiation of inhibition in our networks lead to anenhancement of neuronal response selectivity during development. This was indeed accompa-nied by the simultaneous emergence of feature-specific synaptic connectivity in the network,

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 16 / 27

Page 17: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

demonstrating that self-regulation in a randomly connected balanced network via synapticplasticity is capable of adjusting the network to work in the “right” regime, dominated byinhibition.

In fact, in absence of such a dynamic self-regulation, plasticity of excitatory connections canlead to a network, where only neurons with very similar orientation selectivity are connectedtogether. A similar issue was recently raised by [3], where it is argued that Hebbian plasticity,naively applied, would drive networks to become overly specific, with only very few excitatoryconnections between neurons preferring different orientations. First, this scenario is inconsis-tent with the experimental results which show a heterogeneous functional connectivity inmouse visual cortex [54]. Second, an overrepresentation of connections between functionallysimilar neurons can lead to an instability of network dynamics, with pathological states of high-ly correlated activity (unpublished observation). None of these phenomena were observed inthe simulations we report here, demonstrating that in plastic balanced networks self-regulatorymechanisms emerge that work against such consequences.

We found that the emergence of functionally specific connectivity was a robust process thatalways converged to a stable equilibrium in our simulations. However, the network was sensi-tive to the stimulus statistics, and an overrepresentation of specific stimulus orientations even-tually increased the number of neurons that selectively responded to that orientation.Moreover, the synaptic weights in the equilibrium state after learning remained stable for aspontaneously active network, in absence of visual stimulation.

Our results suggested that synaptic plasticity can enhance orientation selectivity. Consistentwith experimental studies [6, 35, 38], we observed an enhancement of orientation selectivity intuning after learning, as indicated by the OSI of tuning curves. The overall responses of ournetworks became sparser, with fewer neurons in the network responding to each stimulus ori-entation. This is consistent with experimental results demonstrating a sparsification of neuro-nal activity in visual cortex after eye opening [55], and a general decrease in cortical activitybetween eye opening (postnatal day 13) and the peak of the critical period for rodent visual cor-tical plasticity (postnatal day 28) [56]. Also, consistent with our findings on the role of inhibi-tion and stimulus statistics in refining both connectivity and function during development, arecent experimental study has found an enhancement of surround-suppression during normalvision for natural surround stimuli [57].

The importance of inhibition-dominance and inhibitory plasticity for our results brings upa question with regard to recent experimental studies, which argue that lateral inhibition is nota prerequisite for the emergence of orientation selectivity and its sharpening, and that the non-linearity induced by the spiking threshold is enough [58–61]. First, in our networks, too, a non-linearity induced by the spiking threshold was necessary for sharpening of tuning curves, as wehave analyzed and discussed in a recent study [49]. In fact, if such a nonlinearity in the transferfunction of single neurons was absent, no sharpening would appear, as a linear operation of theneuronal network would linearly transform the feedforward tuning to output tuning curvesand map input cosine curves to output cosine curves [25, 49]. This nonlinearity, however, isnot acting alone in our networks, as the interplay between recurrent (excitatory and inhibitory)inputs from the network and the nonlinearities of single cell transfer functions eventually de-termines the output orientation selectivity and its sharpening.

This notion is supported by recent experimental results in mouse visual cortex [62, 63],where broad inhibitory input underlies a developmental [63] and a contrast-dependent [62]sharpening of tuning curves. These results, along with recent optogenetic experiments on thecontribution of inhibitory neurons to orientation selectivity [64, 65], provide direct evidencefor the functional role of inhibition in this process. However, we cannot rule out the possibilityof species-specific differences regarding the contribution of intracortical inhibition, especially

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 17 / 27

Page 18: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

between species with and without orientation maps (e.g. cats and macaque monkeys vs. miceand rats) [1, 66–68], although clear evidence for the contribution of untuned inhibition to ori-entation selectivity has also been provided for the former species [69, 70]. Further experimentalstudies are thus needed to resolve this issue, and to elucidate the relative importance of eachmechanism.

The voltage triplet plasticity rule we used here was a non-linear variant with a symmetriccomponent. It was constructed to account for experiments [31], which show that if both neu-rons fire at high rate (about 15 Hz), pre-before-post and post-before-pre both lead to LTP. Inthat case, two neurons with the same orientation preference tend to fire together at high rate,and therefore are bound to develop a bidirectional connection. An asymmetrical STDP rule, incontrast, would tend to remove bidirectional connections (see also [14]). Hence, the learningrule we used here is well suited to generate the connectivity pattern recently observed experi-mentally in visual cortex [5, 6, 71].

The form of the plasticity rule from E! E, E! I, and I! E in our work was assumed tobe identical for the sake of simplicity. There are some recent data on inhibitory plasticity (e.g.[72, 73], see also [74] for a review), but the experimental evidence is rather confusing. Theexact rule seems to be highly dependent on the protocol, the inhibitory neuron subtype, andthe experimental preparation. Therefore, we chose the simplest possible approach and used thesame rule for our main results. Specifically, our I! E plasticity rule was different from theplasticity rule employed in a previous study [50], which set out to reproduce excitatory and in-hibitory co-tuning in auditory cortex [75], where inhibitory neurons are sharply tuned. De-tailed balance resulted from this rule. In the visual cortex, although orientation selectivity ofinhibitory neurons and the specificity of their wiring is controversial in the experimental litera-ture [45, 76], mostly a non-specific connectivity of inhibitory neurons for both major subtypes(i.e. PV+ and SOM+ interneurons) has been reported (see e.g. [64]). We therefore confined ourmodeling to PV-expressing interneurons with broad tuning and non-specific connectivity toconform better to the currently available experimental data. Nevertheless, we were able to showthat the same results can be obtained using the I! E plasticity rule of [50] (S5 Fig), or, in fact,even without plasticity of I! E connections (S1 Fig).

We assumed non-plastic feedforward connections in our study. The rationale behind thisdecision came from experimental results [6], which showed that orientation selectivity is al-ready in place at eye opening, and only later on, presumably thanks to experience-dependentplasticity, there is a refinement of intra-cortical connections. Our present work puts a focus onmodeling the second step of the development, i.e. what happens after eye-opening. We thinkthat, even though the feedforward weights are still plastic during that time, they do not changeorientation selectivity much anymore. Note that feedforward plasticity was studied in a previ-ous model [6, 26], but the model did not have large recurrent and complex dynamics of excita-tion and inhibition. Here, instead, we focused on the cooperation of recurrent dynamics andrecurrent plasticity, and we showed how functional networks can operate in balanced regimesof activity and develop feature-specific connectivity.

It has been shown that while Hebbian learning can extract the first principal component[77], including inhibition allows us to extract other components as well [78]. Inclusion of re-current inhibition in realistic network models thus paves the road to study this phenomenon.It can also cast light on the formation of receptive fields: a non-linear spike-timing-dependentplasticity (STDP) rule has been recently shown [79] to reduce to a rule similar to the Bienen-stock-Cooper-Munro (BCM) rule, which can form receptive fields [80]. The excitatory plastici-ty model (voltage-triplet rule, i.e. a non-linear variant of STDP) used in this paper allows toextract three point correlation structures in the inputs, as shown in [79]. If this rule is appliedto patches of images that are whitened, receptive fields emerge that are Gabor filters, similar to

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 18 / 27

Page 19: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

orientation selectivity [26]. In contrast, using standard linear STDP learning, no Gabor filterscan develop. Lateral inhibition can be used to make sure that neurons become selective to dif-ferent orientations [26] so that the network can recover other components and represent allthe different orientations.

A study using a pair-based STDP rule has shown that plasticity of recurrent excitatory con-nections may lead a group of neurons to take over the whole network, but including recurrent(nonplastic) inhibition prevents that and can lead to the emergence of maps [46]. The latter re-sult is consistent with our network model here, where the balance of excitation and inhibitionkeeps the network in a dynamically stable regime, which in turn ensures a linear representationof the feedforward input [48, 49]. It is, however, difficult to replicate the same winner-take-alltype of behavior reported in [46] in our networks for the following reasons. First, the activity inour networks is dominated by recurrent input to neurons. Hence, the networks show unstableactivity in absence of recurrent inhibition, since the excitatory recurrent feedback is large. Sec-ond, the networks are dominated by recurrent plasticity. As a result, when recurrent inhibitionis not present, the output selectivity of responses is very weak and the output selectivity cannotbe enhanced by feedforward plasticity. Because the activity is very weakly selective, the selectivecomponent of the responses cannot dominate in a winner-take-all type of dynamics. The net-work response, instead, ends up in a pathological state of nonselective activity, where all neu-rons are active (not shown).

This was not the case, however, for smaller, feedforward-driven networks where recurrentdynamics did not lead to runaway excitation in absence of inhibition, and where the feedfor-ward input was dominant such that the receptive fields of neurons were mainly determined byit (networks developed in [6, 26]). The same winner-take-all behavior as reported in [46] wasobserved in absence of inhibition, where all neurons developed the same selectivity. It will beinteresting to see in the future how including feedforward plasticity can change the behavior ofour network model in this respect. This might be particularly pertinent to experimental find-ings on brain plasticity after lesion, and useful for understanding the mechanisms of brain re-pair in certain brain diseases.

In conclusion, our study demonstrates how functional balanced networks with realistic re-current dynamics can work in tandem with Hebbian synaptic plasticity to understand and ex-plain findings recently reported in experimental studies. It sheds light on the process ofrefinement of network responses during development, and elucidates the conditions for theemergence of feature-specific connectivity in the network. Our results underline the significantrole of inhibition for network function [81], both for the emergence of feature selectivity andfeature-specific connectivity. This work in turn, triggers another important question: Whichfunctional properties result from feature-specific connectivity in the network [82]? Further the-oretical and experimental studies are definitely needed to address these questions, but our pres-ent study indicates some promising directions to further explore large-scale neuronal networkswith realistic recurrent dynamics and synaptic plasticity.

Materials and Methods

Network modelThe technical details of network model are described elsewhere [25]. Briefly, the model consistsof a recurrent network of N = 500 leaky integrate-and-fire (LIF) neurons, of which f = 80% areexcitatory and 20% are inhibitory [83]. The sub-threshold dynamics of the membrane potentialuk(t) of neuron k is given by the leaky-integrator equation

t _ukðtÞ þ ukðtÞ ¼ RIkðtÞ: ð1Þ

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 19 / 27

Page 20: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

The current Ik(t) represents the total input to neuron k, the (leaky) integration of which is gov-erned by the leak resistance R, and the membrane time constant τ = 20 ms. When the voltagereaches the threshold at uth = 20 mV, a spike is generated and transmitted to all post-synapticneurons, and the membrane potential is reset to the resting potential at u0 = 0 mV.

Each excitatory neuron projects to a randomly sampled population comprising �exc = 30%of all neurons in the network. Inhibitory neurons project to all neurons in the network (�inh =100%) [51, 52]. In addition, inhibitory post-synaptic potentials have a g = 8 times larger ampli-tude than excitatory ones [9, 25]. This choice of parameters is motivated by the dense connec-tivity of inhibitory neurons reported in different areas of cortex [4, 51, 84]. Post-synapticcurrents are modeled as δ-functions, where the total charge Q is delivered instantaneously tothe post-synaptic neuron after the arrival of a spike, without a synaptic delay. Synaptic couplingstrength is measured in terms of the amplitude of the resulting post-synaptic potential (PSP), J= QR/τ. Unless stated otherwise, the amplitude of excitatory connections in the local networkis Jexc = 0.5 mV, and that of inhibitory connections Jinh = −gJexc = −4 mV.

Upon presentation of the visual stimulus, an elongated bar with some fixed orientation,feedforward input is driving cortical neurons. We lump all feedforward synapses to a givenneuron into one single synapse, and model the feedforward input as a single channel with anuntuned and a tuned component. The untuned baseline firing rate of this feedforward input issb = 2 kHz, and the amplitude of the input synapses is Jffw = 1 mV. The tuned component ofthe input is modulated depending on the orientation of the stimulus, θ, and the preferred ori-entation (PO) of the neuron, θ�, according to a cosine function

sðy; y�Þ ¼ sb½1þ m cos ð2ðy� y�ÞÞ�: ð2Þ

The parameter μ is the relative modulation amplitude of the input tuning curve, which is set toμexc = 20% for excitatory neurons, and μinh = 2% for inhibitory neurons, consistent with weakertuning of inhibitory neurons reported in the cortex [4, 44, 45].

In our simulations, the feedforward input is represented by a stationary Poisson process ofrate s. To obtain individual tuning curves, as shown in Fig 2, the stimulation of the network isrepeated with 8 different orientations, 0°, 22.5°, 45°, . . ., 157.5°. Selectivity of responses, includ-ing individual tuning curves, is quantified by a global selectivity index, OSI = 1−circular vari-ance [25, 36].

The implementation of the LIF model is based on a numerical method known as “exact inte-gration” [85, 86]. Numerical integration of network dynamics was performed using a time stepof dt = 1 ms. We repeated some of the simulations with a smaller time step (dt = 0.1 ms) to ver-ify the accuracy of our results.

The simulation code will be published on the freely-available repository ModelDB (http://senselab.med.yale.edu/modeldb/) after publication.

Plasticity modelThe plasticity model is explained in detail elsewhere [6, 26]. Here, it applies only to recurrentweights, and describes changes in the synaptic amplitude of synapse i (wi) by the equation

ddt

wi ¼ �ALTDð��u ÞXiðtÞ ½u�ðtÞ � y��þ þ ALTP �xiðtÞ ½uðtÞ � yþ�þ ½uþðtÞ � y��þ; ð3Þ

combined with hard bounds wmin� wi� wmax. wmin and wmax are 0 mV and 2 mV for excit-atory synapses, and −5 mV and 0 mV for inhibitory synapses, respectively.

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 20 / 27

Page 21: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

The quantity u�ðtÞ is a low-pass filtered version of the postsynaptic membrane potential u(t), assuming an exponential kernel with time constant τ−:

t�ddt

u�ðtÞ ¼ �u�ðtÞ þ uðtÞ: ð4Þ

The brackets [.]+ denote half-wave rectification ([x]+ = x for x> 0, and [x]+ = 0 otherwise) andreflect experimental findings showing that postsynaptic depolarization induces a depression ofthe synapse only if it exceeds a certain threshold value θ− [29]. The presynaptic spike train isrepresented by a sequence of impulses at times tni , XiðtÞ ¼

Pndðt � tni Þ, where i is the index of

the synapse and n is an index that counts the spike. ALTDð��uÞ is an amplitude parameter that isunder the control of a homeostatic processes [87]. Here it depends on ��u, which is the mean de-

polarization of the postsynaptic neuron, averaged over a time window of size 0.1 s: ALTDð��uÞ ¼ALTD

��u 2

u2refwhere u2

ref is a reference value. The variable ��u is used to control the homeostasis, and

therefore the window is longer than all the other time constants. uref defines the target depolari-zation that the homeostasis is reaching, whereas ALTD is the amplitude for depression of therule. See [26] for more details.

For the LTP part, we assume that each presynaptic spike arriving at the synapse wi increasesa synapse-specific trace �xiðtÞ of some biophysical quantity by some fixed value. The trace �xiðtÞdecays exponentially with a time constant τx in the absence of presynaptic spikes, as describedin previous work [43, 88]. The temporal evolution of �xiðtÞ is described by a linear differentialequation

txddt

�xiðtÞ ¼ ��xiðtÞ þ XiðtÞ; ð5Þ

where Xi is the input spike train defined above. ALTP is a free parameter inferred from experi-mental data, and �uþðtÞ is another lowpass filtered version of u(t) similar to �u�ðtÞ, but with asomewhat shorter time constant τ+. Thus positive weight changes can occur if the momentaryvoltage u(t) surpasses a threshold θ+ and, at the same time, the average value �uþðtÞ is above θ−.

The two time constants τ+ and τ− were fitted to several experimental data (see [26]). With-out the filtering, it is neither possible to reproduce the frequency dependency demonstrated in[31], nor the classical spike-timing dependent plasticity learning window. The voltage is veryfast during a spike and therefore needs to be filtered to detect the pre-post coincidence. Thisrule shows a nice trade-off between the number of parameters and the robustness at describingthe experimental phenomenology [34].

The values of the plasticity parameters in our simulations are: τ− = 10 ms, τ+ = 7 ms, τx = 15

ms, ALTD = 14 × 10−5, ALTP = 8 × 10−5, θ− = −20 mV, θ+ = 7.5 mV, u2ref ¼ 70mV2. Structural

plasticity is not allowed: only synapses that were initially established undergo potentiation anddepression. Feedforward weights are not plastic. For a justification of network and plasticityparameters, see [25, 26], respectively. All the parameters are listed in Table 1.

To quantify the amount of bidirectional connectivity emerging as a result of plasticity (as ine.g. Fig 3) we used a weighted bidirectionality index (WBI). As we did not allow for structuralplasticity in our networks, we quantified this by computing the product of bidirectional weightsbetween each pair of neurons (wij wji for neurons i and j). If only one neuron is connected tothe other, and the bidirectional connection is completely missing (wij = 0 or wji = 0), the prod-uct is zero. To obtain WBI of a network, we average over all neuronal pairs in the network. Acompletely unidirectionally connected network would therefore return a WBI of zero. For net-works with bidirectional connections, WBI would be larger than zero. For a specific pair of

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 21 / 27

Page 22: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

neurons with a similar sum of weights, wij + wji = constant, WBI would be highest if weightsare fully bidirectional, i.e. wij = wji.

To account for the fact that some bidirectional connectivity exists by chance in random net-works, we shuffle the elements of weight matrix for each network and compute its WBI asWBIrandom. The normalized WBI, WBInorm, is then obtained by dividing the WBI of the actualweight matrix of the network by its shuffled WBI: WBInorm =WBI/WBIrandom.

Supporting InformationS1 Fig. Emergence of specific connectivity in a network with a different initial connectivity.Results are illustrated in the same fashion as in Fig 8, for a network with the following parame-ters of connectivity: probability of an I! E connection = 80%, J = 0.1, g = 4. I! E weights arenot plastic. Other parameters are the same as the default values. The learning phase is orga-nized in 20 batches.(TIF)

Table 1. Table of parameters. Default parameters are given, and only the exceptions are noted for figures with parameters different from the default values.

Parameters of Default values Fig 8 Fig 9 Fig 10 S1 Fig S2 Fig S3 Fig S4 Fig S5 Fig

Single Neurons

membrane time constant τm = 20 ms

resting potential Vrest = 0 mV

threshold voltage Vth = 20 mV

refractory period tref = 0 ms

Network Simulations

# neurons N = 500

fraction of Exc neurons f = 80%

fraction of Inh neurons 1−f = 20%

Exc to Exc conn. prob. �E ! E = 30%

Exc to Inh conn. prob. �E ! I = 30% 80% 80%

Inh to Exc conn. prob. �I ! E = 100%

Inh to Inh conn. prob. �I ! I = 100%

recurrent EPSP (mV) Jexc = 0.5 0.1 N(0.5, 0.5) N(0.1, 0.1)

recurrent IPSP: Jinh = −gJexc g = 8 4 (1, . . ., 8) 4 2

feedforward EPSP (mV) Jffw = 1

feedforward baseline rate νffw = 2 kHz

modulation ratio of Exc input μexc = 20%

modulation ratio of Inh input μinh = 2%

Synaptic Plasticity

θ+ 7.5 mV ±10%

θ−

−20 mV ±10%

ALTP 8 × 10−5 ±10% +20% for Exc +20% for Inh

ALTD 14 × 10−5 ±10%

τ−

10 ms

τ+ 7 ms

τx 15 ms

plasticity of I! E [26] [50]

doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307.t001

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 22 / 27

Page 23: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

S2 Fig. Effect of the initial distribution of excitatory weights. Same as Fig 6 for the same net-work, but with a different initial weight matrix. Instead of excitatory weights being either 0 orJexc, as in Fig 6, here the amplitudes of non-zero initial connections are drawn from a Gaussiandistribution with mean Jexc and standard deviation Jexc. Rarely occurring negative values are setto zero.(TIF)

S3 Fig. Effect of an altered potentiation rate of excitatory connections. The default networkwith the same parameters as the network in Fig 1, except for a potentiation rate of excitatory

(to both excitatory and inhibitory) synapses increased by 20%: AexcLTP ¼ 9:6� 10�5. Panels and

conventions are the same as in Fig 8.(TIF)

S4 Fig. Effect of an altered potentiation rate of inhibitory connections. The default networkwith the same parameters as the network in Fig 1, except for a potentiation rate of inhibitory

(to excitatory) synapses increased by 20%: AinhLTP ¼ 9:6� 10�5. Panels and conventions are the

same as in Fig 8.(TIF)

S5 Fig. Emergence and evolution of specific connectivity in a network with denser connec-tivity of E! I and a different plasticity rule for I! E connections. Probability of an I! Econnection is 80% [4], Jexc = 0.1, g = 2. Similar to to S2 Fig, the amplitudes of non-zero initialE! E connections are drawn from a Gaussian distribution with mean Jexc and standard devia-tion Jexc. I! E connections are plastic according to the plasticity rule described in [50]. Briefly,the synaptic weight wij from a pre-synaptic inhibitory neuron j to a post-synaptic excitatoryneuron i is updated at each time step according to the following rule:wij wij þ Zð�xi � aÞ forpre-synaptic spikes, and wij ! wij þ Z�xj for post-synaptic spikes [50]. Here, �xj and �xi are

traces obtained by low-pass filtering (similar to Eq 5 with the same time constant τx) spikesemitted in the pre- and post-synaptic neurons j and i, respectively. η = 0.1 is a learning rate andα = 0.01 is a depression factor. The parameters are chosen to ensure an output post-synapticfiring rate of 5 Hz. Other parameters are the same as the default values. The learning phase isorganized in 40 batches. For the spontaneous activity, the network is stimulated with an un-tuned input with sb. Panels and conventions are the same as in Figs 6–8 of the main text withthe following correspondence: panels (A–K) correspond to Fig 8, A–k; panel (L) correspondsto Fig 6A; and panel (M) corresponds to Fig 7A.(TIF)

Author ContributionsConceived and designed the experiments: SS CC SR. Performed the experiments: SS. Analyzedthe data: SS CC SR. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SS CC SR. Wrote the paper:SS CC SR.

References1. Ohki K, Chung S, Ch’ng YH, Kara P, Reid RC (2005) Functional imaging with cellular resolution reveals

precise micro-architecture in visual cortex. Nature 433: 597–603. doi: 10.1038/nature03274 PMID:15660108

2. Ohki K, Reid RC (2007) Specificity and randomness in the visual cortex. Current opinion in neurobiolo-gy 17: 401–7. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.007 PMID: 17720489

3. Kaschube M (2014) Neural maps versus salt-and-pepper organization in visual cortex. Current opinionin neurobiology 24: 95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.08.017 PMID: 24492085

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 23 / 27

Page 24: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

4. Hofer SB, Ko H, Pichler B, Vogelstein J, Ros H, et al. (2011) Differential connectivity and response dy-namics of excitatory and inhibitory neurons in visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience 14: 1045–52. doi:10.1038/nn.2876 PMID: 21765421

5. Ko H, Hofer SB, Pichler B, Buchanan KA, Sjöström PJ, et al. (2011) Functional specificity of local syn-aptic connections in neocortical networks. Nature 473: 87–91. doi: 10.1038/nature09880 PMID:21478872

6. Ko H, Cossell L, Baragli C, Antolik J, Clopath C, et al. (2013) The emergence of functional microcircuitsin visual cortex. Nature 496: 96–100. doi: 10.1038/nature12015 PMID: 23552948

7. Ko H, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Hofer SB (2014) Emergence of Feature-Specific Connectivity in Cortical Micro-circuits in the Absence of Visual Experience. Journal of Neuroscience 34: 9812–9816. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0875-14.2014 PMID: 25031418

8. van Vreeswijk C, Sompolinsky H (1996) Chaos in neuronal networks with balanced excitatory and in-hibitory activity. Science 274: 1724–6. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5293.1724 PMID: 8939866

9. Brunel N (2000) Dynamics of sparsely connected networks of excitatory and inhibitory spiking neurons.Journal of Computational Neuroscience 8: 183–208. doi: 10.1023/A:1008925309027 PMID: 10809012

10. Morrison A, Aertsen A, Diesmann M (2007) Spike-timing dependent plasticity in balanced random net-works. Neural Computation 19: 1437–1467. doi: 10.1162/neco.2007.19.6.1437 PMID: 17444756

11. Kunkel S, Diesmann M, Morrison A (2011) Limits to the development of feed-forward structures in largerecurrent neuronal networks. Frontiers in computational neuroscience 4: 160. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00160 PMID: 21415913

12. Roberts PD, Bell CC (2000) Computational consequences of temporally asymmetric learning rules: II.Sensory image cancellation. Computational Neuroscience 9: 67–83. doi: 10.1023/A:1008938428112

13. Mehta M, Quirk M, Wilson M (2000) Experience-dependent asymmetric shape of hippocampal recep-tive fields. Neuron 25: 707–715. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81072-7 PMID: 10774737

14. Song S, Miller KD, Abbott LF (2000) Competitive Hebbian learning through spike-timing-dependentsynaptic plasticity. Nature neuroscience 3: 919–26. doi: 10.1038/78829 PMID: 10966623

15. Izhikevich EM (2004) Which model to use for cortical spiking neurons? IEEE Transactions on NeuralNetworks 15: 1063–1070. doi: 10.1109/TNN.2004.832719 PMID: 15484883

16. Legenstein R, Naeger C, MaassW (2005) What can a neuron learn with spike-timing dependent plas-ticity. Neural Computation 17: 2337–2382. doi: 10.1162/0899766054796888 PMID: 16156932

17. Guyonneau R, VanRullen R, Thorpe S (2005) Neurons tune to the earliest spikes through stdp. NeuralComputation 17: 859–879. doi: 10.1162/0899766053429390 PMID: 15829092

18. Iglesiasa J, Erikssonb J, Grize F, Tomassini M, Villa A (2005) Dynamics of pruning in simulated large-scale spiking neural networks. Biosystems 79(1–3): 11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2004.09.016

19. Izhikevich EM, Edelman GM (2008) Large-scale model of mammalian thalamocortical systems. Pro-ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 3593–3598. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0712231105

20. Kozloski J, Cecchi GA (2008) Topological effects of spike timing-dependent plasticity. arxivorg abs:0810.0029.

21. Babadi B, Abbott LF (2013) Pairwise analysis can account for network structures arising from spike-tim-ing dependent plasticity. PLoS Comput Biol 9: e1002906. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002906 PMID:23436986

22. Klampfl S, MaassW (2013) Emergence of dynamic memory traces in cortical microcircuit modelsthrough STDP. J Neurosci 33: 11515–11529. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5044-12.2013 PMID:23843522

23. Hansel D, van Vreeswijk C (2012) The Mechanism of Orientation Selectivity in Primary Visual Cortexwithout a Functional Map. The Journal of Neuroscience 32: 4049–4064. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6284-11.2012 PMID: 22442071

24. Pehlevan C, Sompolinsky H (2014) Selectivity and Sparseness in Randomly Connected Balanced Net-works. PLoS ONE 9: e89992. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089992 PMID: 24587172

25. Sadeh S, Cardanobile S, Rotter S (2014) Mean-field analysis of orientation selectivity in inhibition-domi-nated networks of spiking neurons. SpringerPlus 3: 148. doi: 10.1186/2193-1801-3-148 PMID:24790806

26. Clopath C, Büsing L, Vasilaki E, Gerstner W (2010) Connectivity reflects coding: a model of voltage-based STDP with homeostasis. Nature Neuroscience 13: 344–52. doi: 10.1038/nn.2479 PMID:20098420

27. Markram H, Lübke J, Frotscher M, Sakmann B (1997) Regulation of synaptic efficacy by coincidence ofpostysnaptic AP and EPSP. Science 275: 213–215. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5297.213 PMID:8985014

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 24 / 27

Page 25: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

28. Bi G, Poo M (1998) Synaptic modifications in cultured hippocampal neurons: dependence on spike tim-ing, synaptic strength, and postsynaptic cell type. J Neurosci 18: 10464–10472. PMID: 9852584

29. Artola A, Bröcher S, Singer W (1990) Different voltage dependent thresholds for inducing long-term de-pression and long-term potentiation in slices of rat visual cortex. Nature 347: 69–72. doi: 10.1038/347069a0 PMID: 1975639

30. Ngezahayo A, Schachner M, Artola A (2000) Synaptic activation modulates the induction of bidirection-al synaptic changes in adult mouse hippocamus. J Neuroscience 20: 2451–2458.

31. Sjöström P, Turrigiano G, Nelson S (2001) Rate, timing, and cooperativity jointly determine cortical syn-aptic plasticity. Neuron 32: 1149–1164. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00542-6 PMID: 11754844

32. Froemke R, Dan Y (2002) Spike-timing dependent plasticity induced by natural spike trains. Nature416: 433–438. doi: 10.1038/416433a PMID: 11919633

33. Wang H, Gerkin R, Nauen D, Bi G (2005) Coactivation and timing-dependent integration of synaptic po-tentiation and depression. Nature Neuroscience 8: 187–193. doi: 10.1038/nn1387 PMID: 15657596

34. Clopath C, Gerstner W (2010) Voltage and Spike Timing Interact in STDP—A Unified Model. Frontiersin synaptic neuroscience 2: 25. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00025 PMID: 21423511

35. Rochefort NL, Narushima M, Grienberger C, Marandi N, Hill DN, et al. (2011) Development of directionselectivity in mouse cortical neurons. Neuron 71: 425–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.013 PMID:21835340

36. Ringach DL, Shapley RM, Hawken MJ (2002) Orientation Selectivity in Macaque V1: Diversity andLaminar Dependence. The Journal of Neuroscience 22: 5639–5651. PMID: 12097515

37. Niell CM, Stryker MP (2008) Highly selective receptive fields in mouse visual cortex. The Journal ofNeuroscience 28: 7520–36. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0623-08.2008 PMID: 18650330

38. Hoy JL, Niell CM (2015) Layer-Specific Refinement of Visual Cortex Function after Eye Opening in theAwake Mouse. Journal of Neuroscience 35: 3370–3383. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3174-14.2015PMID: 25716837

39. SennW, Tsodyks M, Markram H (2001) An algorithm for modifying neurotransmitter release probabilitybased on pre- and postsynaptic spike timing. Neural Computation 13: 35–67. doi: 10.1162/089976601300014628 PMID: 11177427

40. Shouval HZ, Bear MF, Cooper LN (2002) A unified model of nmda receptor dependent bidirectionalsynaptic plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99: 10831–10836. doi: 10.1073/pnas.152343099 PMID:12136127

41. Pfister JP, Toyoizumi T, Barber D, Gerstner W (2006) Optimal spike-timing dependent plasticity for pre-cise action potential firing in supervised learning. Neural Computation 18: 1309–1339. doi: 10.1162/neco.2006.18.6.1318

42. Graupner M, Brunel N (2012) Calcium-based plasticity model explains sensitivity of synaptic changesto spike pattern, rate, and dendritic location. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109: 3991–3996. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1109359109 PMID: 22357758

43. Gerstner W, Kempter R, van Hemmen J, Wagner H (1996) A neuronal learning rule for sub-millisecondtemporal coding. Nature 383: 76–78. doi: 10.1038/383076a0 PMID: 8779718

44. Bock DD, LeeWCA, Kerlin AM, AndermannML, Hood G, et al. (2011) Network anatomy and in vivophysiology of visual cortical neurons. Nature 471: 177–82. doi: 10.1038/nature09802 PMID: 21390124

45. Kerlin AM, Andermann ML, Berezovskii VK, Reid RC (2010) Broadly tuned response properties of di-verse inhibitory neuron subtypes in mouse visual cortex. Neuron 67: 858–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.002 PMID: 20826316

46. Song S, Abbott L (2001) Cortical Development and Remapping through Spike Timing-Dependent Plas-ticity. Neuron 32: 339–350. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00451-2 PMID: 11684002

47. Gütig R, Aharonov R, Rotter S, Sompolinsky H (2003) Learning input correlations through nonlineartemporally asymmetric Hebbian plasticity. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Socie-ty for Neuroscience 23: 3697–714.

48. Sadeh S, Rotter S (2014) Distribution of orientation selectivity in recurrent networks of spiking neuronswith different random topologies. PloS one 9: e114237. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114237 PMID:25469704

49. Sadeh S, Rotter S (2015) Orientation Selectivity in Inhibition-Dominated Networks of Spiking Neurons:Effect of Single Neuron Properties and Network Dynamics. PLoS computational biology 11: e1004045.doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004045 PMID: 25569445

50. Vogels TP, Sprekeler H, Zenke F, Clopath C, Gerstner W (2011) Inhibitory plasticity balances excitationand inhibition in sensory pathways and memory networks. Science 334: 1569–73. doi: 10.1126/science.1211095 PMID: 22075724

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 25 / 27

Page 26: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

51. Packer AM, Yuste R (2011) Dense, unspecific connectivity of neocortical parvalbumin-positive inter-neurons: a canonical microcircuit for inhibition? The Journal of Neuroscience 31: 13260–71. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3131-11.2011 PMID: 21917809

52. Pfeffer CK, Xue M, He M, Huang ZJ, Scanziani M (2013) Inhibition of inhibition in visual cortex: the logicof connections between molecularly distinct interneurons. Nature neuroscience 16: 1068–76. doi: 10.1038/nn.3446

53. Cossell L, Iacaruso MF, Muir DR, Houlton R, Sader EN, et al. (2015) Functional organization of excit-atory synaptic strength in primary visual cortex. Nature 518: 399–403. doi: 10.1038/nature14182PMID: 25652823

54. Jia H, Rochefort NL, Chen X, Konnerth A (2010) Dendritic organization of sensory input to cortical neu-rons in vivo. Nature 464: 1307–12. doi: 10.1038/nature08947 PMID: 20428163

55. Rochefort NL, Garaschuk O, Milos RI, Narushima M, Marandi N, et al. (2009) Sparsification of neuronalactivity in the visual cortex at eye-opening. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106: 15049–54. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907660106 PMID: 19706480

56. Griffen TC, Wang L, Fontanini A, Maffei A (2012) Developmental regulation of spatio-temporal patternsof cortical circuit activation. Frontiers in cellular neuroscience 6: 65. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2012.00065PMID: 23316135

57. Pecka M, Han Y, Sader E, Mrsic-Flogel T (2014) Experience-Dependent Specialization of ReceptiveField Surround for Selective Coding of Natural Scenes. Neuron 84: 457–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.010 PMID: 25263755

58. Finn IM, Priebe NJ, Ferster D (2007) The emergence of contrast-invariant orientation tuning in simplecells of cat visual cortex. Neuron 54: 137–152. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.029 PMID: 17408583

59. Priebe NJ, Ferster D (2008) Inhibition, spike threshold, and stimulus selectivity in primary visual cortex.Neuron 57: 482–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.005 PMID: 18304479

60. Priebe NJ, Ferster D (2012) Mechanisms of Neuronal Computation in Mammalian Visual Cortex. Neu-ron 75: 194–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.011 PMID: 22841306

61. Sadagopan S, Ferster D (2012) Feedforward origins of response variability underlying contrast invari-ant orientation tuning in cat visual cortex. Neuron 74: 911–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.007PMID: 22681694

62. Li YT, MaWP, Li LY, Ibrahim LA, Wang SZ, et al. (2012) Broadening of Inhibitory Tuning UnderliesContrast-Dependent Sharpening of Orientation Selectivity in Mouse Visual Cortex. Journal of Neurosci-ence 32: 16466–16477. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3221-12.2012 PMID: 23152629

63. Li YT, MaWP, Pan CJ, Zhang LI, Tao HW (2012) Broadening of cortical inhibition mediates develop-mental sharpening of orientation selectivity. The Journal of Neuroscience 32: 3981–91. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5514-11.2012 PMID: 22442065

64. Wilson NR, Runyan CA, Wang FL, Sur M (2012) Division and subtraction by distinct cortical inhibitorynetworks in vivo. Nature 488: 343–8. doi: 10.1038/nature11347 PMID: 22878717

65. Lee SH, Kwan AC, Zhang S, Phoumthipphavong V, Flannery JG, et al. (2012) Activation of specific in-terneurons improves V1 feature selectivity and visual perception. Nature 488: 379–83. doi: 10.1038/nature11312 PMID: 22878719

66. Blasdel GG, Salama G (1986) Voltage-sensitive dyes reveal a modular organization in monkey striatecortex. Nature 321: 579–85. doi: 10.1038/321579a0 PMID: 3713842

67. Bonhoeffer T, Grinvald A (1991) Iso-orientation domains in cat visual cortex are arranged in pinwheel-like patterns. Nature 353: 429–31. doi: 10.1038/353429a0 PMID: 1896085

68. Ohki K, Chung S, Kara P, Hübener M, Bonhoeffer T, et al. (2006) Highly ordered arrangement of singleneurons in orientation pinwheels. Nature 442: 925–8. doi: 10.1038/nature05019 PMID: 16906137

69. Shapley R, Hawken M, Ringach DL (2003) Dynamics of orientation selectivity in the primary visual cor-tex and the importance of cortical inhibition. Neuron 38: 689–99. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00332-5PMID: 12797955

70. Xing D, Ringach DL, Hawken MJ, Shapley RM (2011) Untuned Suppression Makes a Major Contribu-tion to the Enhancement of Orientation Selectivity in Macaque V1. The Journal of Neuroscience 31:15972–15982. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2245-11.2011 PMID: 22049440

71. Song S, Sjöström PJ, Reigl M, Nelson S, Chklovskii DB (2005) Highly Nonrandom Features of SynapticConnectivity in Local Cortical Circuits. PLoS Biology 3: e68. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030068 PMID:15737062

72. Woodin MA, Ganguly K, Poo M (2003) Coincident pre- and postsynaptic activity modifies GABAergicsynapses by postsynaptic changes in Cl- transporter activity. Neuron 39: 807–20. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00507-5 PMID: 12948447

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 26 / 27

Page 27: Emergence of Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks with ...

73. Haas JS, Nowotny T, Abarbanel HDI (2006) Spike-timing-dependent plasticity of inhibitory synapses inthe entorhinal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 96: 3305–13. doi: 10.1152/jn.00551.2006 PMID:16928795

74. Vogels TP, Froemke RC, Doyon N, Gilson M, Haas JS, et al. (2013) Inhibitory synaptic plasticity: spiketiming-dependence and putative network function. Frontiers in neural circuits 7: 119. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00119 PMID: 23882186

75. Froemke RC, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE (2007) A synaptic memory trace for cortical receptive fieldplasticity. Nature 450: 425–9. doi: 10.1038/nature06289 PMID: 18004384

76. MaW, Liu B, Li Y, Huang ZJ, Zhang LI, et al. (2010) Visual representations by cortical somatostatin in-hibitory neurons-selective but with weak and delayed responses. The Journal of Neuroscience 30:14371–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3248-10.2010 PMID: 20980594

77. Oja E (1982) Simplified neuron model as a principal component analyzer. Journal of Mathematical Biol-ogy 15: 267–273. doi: 10.1007/BF00275687 PMID: 7153672

78. Sanger TD (1989) Optimal unsupervised learning in a single-layer linear feedforward neural network.Neural Networks 2: 459–473. doi: 10.1016/0893-6080(89)90044-0

79. Gjorgjieva J, Clopath C, Audet J, Pfister JP (2011) A triplet spike-timing-dependent plasticity modelgeneralizes the Bienenstock-Cooper-Munro rule to higher-order spatiotemporal correlations. Proceed-ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108: 19383–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105933108 PMID: 22080608

80. Bienenstock E, Cooper L, Munro P (1982) Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientationspecificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex. J Neurosci 2: 32–48. PMID: 7054394

81. Isaacson JS, Scanziani M (2011) How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron 72: 231–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027 PMID: 22017986

82. Wolf F, Engelken R, Puelma-Touzel M, Weidinger JDF, Neef A (2014) Dynamical models of cortical cir-cuits. Current opinion in neurobiology 25: 228–36. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.01.017 PMID: 24658059

83. Braitenberg V, Schüz A (1998) Cortex: Statistics and Geometry of Neuronal Connectivity. 81. Springer.

84. Fino E, Yuste R (2011) Dense inhibitory connectivity in neocortex. Neuron 69: 1188–203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.025 PMID: 21435562

85. Diesmann M RSAAGewaltig M-O (2001) State space analysis of synchronous spiking in cortical net-works. Neurocomputing: 565–571. doi: 10.1016/S0925-2312(01)00409-X

86. Rotter S, Diesmann M (1999) Exact digital simulation of time-invariant linear systems with applicationsto neuronal modeling. Biological cybernetics 81: 381–402. doi: 10.1007/s004220050570 PMID:10592015

87. Turrigiano G, Nelson S (2004) Homeostatic plasticity in the developing nervous system. Nature Re-views Neuroscience 5: 97–107. doi: 10.1038/nrn1327 PMID: 14735113

88. Pfister JP, Gerstner W (2006) Triplets of spikes in a model of spike timing-dependent plasticity. J Neu-roscience 26: 9673–9682. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1425-06.2006

Functional Specificity in Balanced Networks

PLOS Computational Biology | DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004307 June 19, 2015 27 / 27