Duff Fiona J

download Duff Fiona J

of 20

Transcript of Duff Fiona J

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    1/20

    Evaluating the effectiveness of a phonologically

    based reading intervention for struggling readerswith varying language profiles

    Fiona J. Duff Marianna E. Hayiou-Thomas

    Charles Hulme

    Published online: 6 January 2011 Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2011

    Abstract This study evaluates Reading Interventiona 10-week supplementary

    reading programme emphasising the link between phonological awareness and

    readingwhen delivered in a realistic educational setting. Twenty-nine 6-year-olds

    with reading difficulties participated in Reading Intervention and their progress and

    attainments were compared with those of a representative control group from the

    same classes, matched on age and gender. Language profiles were also explored.

    Children with reading difficulties showed weaknesses in phonological awarenessand literacy as well as nonphonological oral language skills and nonverbal rea-

    soning. During the intervention, the intervention group made significantly greater

    progress than the control group in early word reading, phoneme awareness and

    phonetic spelling. Over a 6-month follow-up period, the intervention group main-

    tained its gains but during this time made significantly less progress on single word

    reading, phoneme awareness and phonetic spelling than the control group. These

    findings provide evidence that reading interventions can be delivered effectively in

    standard educational settings. We argue that a better understanding of how to

    manage withdrawal of intervention and how to address poor readers additional orallanguage weaknesses is needed.

    Keywords Reading disability Intervention Phonology Oral language

    Introduction

    Extensive research has investigated the most effective methods for ameliorating

    word-level reading difficulties. A general consensus is that approaches whichinvolve training phonological awareness and lettersound relationships, while

    F. J. Duff (&) M. E. Hayiou-Thomas C. Hulme

    Psychology Department, University of York, York YO10 5DD, UK

    e-mail: [email protected]

    1 3

    Read Writ (2012) 25:621640

    DOI 10.1007/s11145-010-9291-6

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    2/20

    explicitly linking these skills to the task of reading, are effective for the majority of

    children with reading difficulties (for reviews, see Bus & van IJzendoorn, 1999;

    NICHD,2000; Torgerson, Brooks, & Hall, 2006; Torgesen, 2005).

    Reading Intervention, as developed by Hatcher, Hulme, and Ellis (1994), is an

    example of an evidence-based intervention that honours best practice. Its efficacyhas been demonstrated in a number of different studies, across various populations.

    In the original study (Hatcher et al., 1994), Reading Intervention (incorporating

    phonological awareness linked to reading) was delivered to children aged 7.5 years

    who were experiencing reading difficulties. Children received 40 30-min sessions of

    individual instruction over 20 weeks. While the Reading Intervention group made

    greater progress than the control group on all measures of reading and spelling, the

    progress of the other two treatment groups (reading alone and phonology alone) was

    akin to that of the control group on all but one measure. The superiority in reading

    for the Reading Intervention group was maintained at a 9-month follow-up.Hatcher et al. (1994) provided good evidence for the Reading Intervention

    approach, making clear that explicit linkage between phonological awareness and

    reading is important, and that training either skill in isolation is not particularly

    effective. Findings from two subsequent meta-analyses (Bus & van IJzendoorn,

    1999; NICHD, 2000) support this view given that the mean effect size of

    phonological awareness training on reading is significantly increased when it is

    combined with training in letter knowledge (ds = 0.18 or 0.38, respectively, for

    phonological awareness in isolation; ds = 0.66 or 0.67, respectively, with letter

    knowledge included).Further rigorous evidence for the effectiveness of Reading Intervention comes

    from a randomised control trial (RCT) conducted by Hatcher et al. (2006b). In this

    study, Reading Intervention was delivered to children aged 56 years, who

    represented the bottom 8% of the population in terms of reading development.

    Children were randomly assigned to either a 20-week intervention group or a

    10-week (waiting list) intervention group. Reading Intervention was found to

    produce improvements in reading, letter knowledge and phoneme awareness

    (ds = 0.69, 0.94 and 0.46, respectively). An 11-month follow up showed that gains

    in reading had been maintained.

    Carefully controlled studies, like those above, are critical for demonstrating that

    gains in reading can be attributed to a particular intervention programme. Such

    tested programmes can then be passed confidently from researchers to practitioners.

    However, it is important to check that the programme continues to produce

    educationally significant results, to validate its feasibility and ongoing use. Here, we

    report a novel approach to evaluating intervention, which takes the Reading

    Intervention programme and assesses its effectiveness when it is initiated and

    implemented not by researchers, but by schools as a routine part of their educational

    practice.

    The timing of this evaluation is also important, owing to significant shifts in

    educational policy related to the teaching of literacy in the UK. The Independent

    Review of the Teaching of Early Reading (Rose, 2006) recommends that for all

    young children, The knowledge, skills, and understanding that constitute high

    quality phonic work should be taught as the prime approach to learning to decode

    622 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    3/20

    (to read) and encode (write/spell) print (p. 70). Therefore, the key principles of

    teaching reading that underlie Reading Intervention (e.g. promoting phonological

    awareness and phonic decoding) are more familiar elements of whole-class literacy

    instruction than they were when Reading Intervention was first developed (Hatcher

    et al., 1994), or last evaluated (Hatcher et al., 2006b). Thus, the major aim of thepresent study was to evaluate Reading Intervention, when co-ordinated by schools,

    by assessing its efficacy against the evidence-based instructional background of

    contemporary literacy education.

    Evaluations of interventions usually consider children with diagnosed specific

    reading problems. Here, we are interested instead in children that schools deem to

    be in need of reading intervention. Preliminary research suggests that such children

    might have weaknesses not only in phonological skills but also nonphonological

    oral language skills including vocabulary and grammar (Duff et al., 2008). Thus, a

    second aim of our work is to explore the linguistic profiles of children identified byschools as needing reading intervention.

    To achieve our aims, children with weaknesses in reading participated in Reading

    Intervention, and their literacy progress was compared to that of a representative

    untreated control group drawn from the same classrooms. This comparison makes

    for a conservative evaluation of Reading Intervention: The progress of children with

    weaknesses in reading is being compared to that of children whose reading levels

    are age-appropriate; and, in the wake of policy shifts, the baseline instructional

    environment is one that includes many of the same key elements of the intervention.

    Method

    A 10-week reading intervention programme was implemented for a group of children

    with weaknesses in word reading. Their progress during the intervention period was

    compared to that of a representative group of untreated classmates. Children were

    tested individually at school on several occasions, following informed parental

    consent. A comprehensive test battery assessing literacy, language and cognitive

    skills was administered to all children before the intervention began (t1, mean

    age = 6;02). Immediately after the intervention finished, relevant literacy-based

    skills were re-assessed (t2, mean age = 6;06). The post-test battery was repeated at a

    6-month follow-up (t3,mean age =7;00).

    Participants

    Participants were drawn from eight primary schools in the UK county of North

    Yorkshire that were running Reading Intervention in the second and third years of

    formal schooling. Children in the intervention group were selected by professionals

    within schools, and were identified as children with poor word reading skills who

    would likely benefit from Reading Intervention. A representative peer-comparison

    group served as the control group. This included children from the same classes

    matched individually on gender and age to the intervention children. One

    Reading intervention 623

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    4/20

    intervention child was withdrawn part-way through the intervention programme due

    to poor attendance. This resulted in a total of 30 participants in the control group

    and 29 in the intervention group. Due to changing schools, only 26 children in the

    intervention group were assessed at follow up. Further demographic information is

    shown in Table1.

    Assessment battery

    Literacy

    Letter-sound knowledge (t1, t2, t3)

    Children were shown all 26 letters of the alphabet in a random but fixed order. They

    were required to produce the associated sound of each letter.

    Spelling (t1, t2, t3)

    Children were asked to name pictures of 10 items and to write down each associated

    word. A raw score was computed. Spellings were also scored according to how

    closely the orthographic representations of the consonants matched the target

    consonants: 4 points were awarded for each correct letter; 3 points for a letter that

    differed from the target consonant by only one phonetic feature (place, manner or

    voicing); 2 points for a partially represented digraph, or for a correct letter in the

    wrong position; and 1 point for a letter more than one phonetic feature away from

    the target consonant. Vowels were not scored to avoid any confounds owing to

    regional variations in accents.

    Single word reading (t1, t2, t3)

    The BAS II Word Reading subtest (Elliot, Smith, & McCulloch, 1997) was

    administered as a standardised test of single word reading. Children were also

    administered a 43-item version of the Early Word Reading test (EWRSnowlinget al., 2009), which assessed childrens ability to read words that are frequently

    encountered during the early stages of learning to read. A 50-item version of this

    test was devised to avoid potential ceiling effects by adding seven more difficult

    items from the BAS II Word Reading subtest.

    Table 1 Demographics of the control and intervention groups, assessed pre-intervention

    Group N Gender Mean age (SD) Range Diagnoses

    (M:F) (years;months) (years;months) Dyslexia ADHD SLD

    Control 30 15:15 6;02 (0;06) 5;037;03 0 0 3

    Intervention 29 14:15 6;02 (0;07) 5;037;02 0 0 7

    SLD speech/language difficulties, based on teacher report

    624 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    5/20

    Phonological awareness

    Phoneme awareness (t1, t2, t3)

    Three subscales from theSound Linkage Test of Phonological Awareness (Hatcher,2000) provided a measure of ability to manipulate phonemes. Children were first

    required to blend phonemes, for example upon hearing /t////p/, they were to

    produce tap. Next, they were asked to segment phonemes, for example, break up

    pet into its constituent phonemes /p/ /e/ /t/. Finally, the children were required to

    delete phonemes from the beginning, middle, or end of a word, for example, /g/

    from gone makes on.

    Nonword repetition (t1)

    The Childrens Test of Nonword Repetition (CNRepGathercole & Baddeley,

    1996) was administered. Children were required to repeat 40 nonwords which

    ranged in length from two to five syllables (e.g. glistow, versatrationist).

    Rapid automatized naming (RAN) (t1)

    To assess childrens rapid naming ability, the Naming Speed Test from the

    Phonological Assessment Battery (PhABFrederickson, Frith, & Reason, 1997)

    was administered. Children were first required to name the five relevant objects toensure that they had access to the requisite semantic labels. They were then

    presented with a grid containing these five objects repeated at random to create a

    series of 50 pictures. Their task was to name these objects in order as quickly as

    possible. The task was repeated for a second grid containing the same items but

    presented in a different order. Childrens performance was judged according to the

    total time in which it took them to complete both trials.

    Oral language

    Receptive vocabulary (t1)

    Comprehension of single words was measured using the British Picture Vocabulary

    Scale II(BPVS IIDunn, Dunn, Whetton, & Burley,1997). For a variety of spoken

    words, children were required to indicate which of four pictures most accurately

    represented each target word.

    Expressive vocabulary (t1)

    Production of single words was assessed using a confrontation naming testthe

    Assessment of Comprehension and Expression 6-11 (ACEAdams, Cooke,

    Crutchley, Hesketh, & Reeves, 2001). Children were required to name a series of

    pictures that represented a set of 25 nouns gradually increasing in level of difficulty.

    Reading intervention 625

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    6/20

    Expressive grammar (t1)

    Two tests were administered to assess expressive grammar. In the Recalling

    Sentences subtest of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals III(CELF

    IIISemel, Wiig, & Secord, 1995), children were asked to repeat back a series ofsentences, which increased in complexity and length. The Past Tense Probe from

    theTest of Early Grammatical Impairment (TEGIRice & Wexler,2001) was also

    administered. On each trial, a sentence relating to a picture was modelled in the

    present continuous tense, and the child was then required to talk about the same

    picture in the past tense.

    Receptive grammar (t1)

    The ability to understand grammar in spoken language was measured by theTest forReception of Grammar 2 (TROG 2Bishop, 2003). Upon hearing a sentence,

    children had to point to one of four pictures that represented the grammatical

    relations between the elements in the sentence (e.g. The elephant chases the duck).

    Various grammatical constructions were tested.

    Nonverbal ability (t1)

    Nonverbal reasoning was assessed using the Pattern Constructionsubtest from theBAS II. Children were required to manipulate two-coloured squares and two-

    coloured cubes to replicate abstract patterns which increased in complexity.

    Successful replications were credited with points, with additional marks for faster

    completion.

    Behaviour (t1)

    Each childs class teacher completed the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire

    (SDQGoodman, 1997) as a means of assessing childrens behavioural profiles.

    Two outcomes are reported: total social deviance (a sum of scores from 4

    subscaleshyperactivity, emotional symptoms, conduct problems and peer prob-

    lems), and hyperactivity as an independent subscale. Lower scores reflect more

    socially appropriate behaviour.

    Inhibition (t1)

    An estimate of childrens attention skills was provided by the Inhibitionsubscale ofthe Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning (BRIEFGioia, Isquith,

    Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000). Class teachers rated the extent to which a list of

    behaviours typically indicating lack of inhibition (or presence of perseveration),

    were evident in each child. Lower scores reflect better attention/inhibition.

    626 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    7/20

    Teaching procedures

    The intervention programme implemented in this study was Reading Intervention

    (Hatcher et al.,1994,2004,2006a,b). Ten teaching assistants (TAs) were involved

    in delivering the intervention, all of whom had received 4 days of training (from thelocal education authority), had previous experience of delivering the programme,

    and whose instructional delivery had recently been observed in a quality assurance

    exercise. The intervention is designed to run for 10 weeks, alternating between daily

    group (n = 3) and individual sessions, lasting 20 min each. All sessions follow a

    standard procedure, within which it is possible to tailor the level and focus of

    teaching content to individual childrens needs.

    Six of the children in the present evaluation received intervention of the

    same content but always administered on an individual basisa pragmatic

    decision made by the school. Analyses of variance showed that these childrendid not differ significantly from the remaining children in the intervention group

    on pre-intervention word reading (Fs(1,27) =0.230.65, ps = .636.429, gp2s =

    0.01). Nor did they differ in gains made on most outcome measures during the

    intervention (Fs(1,27) = 0.003.62, ps = .979.068, gp2s = 0.000.12). This indi-

    vidually tutored group made significantly less progress than the rest of the inter-

    vention group on Early Word Reading (F(1,27) =9.91, p = .004, gp2

    = 0.27).

    Though this may appear contradictory, the NICHD (2000) report is not conclusive

    on whether individual instruction is more effective than small-group instruction. In

    terms of phonological awareness training, the effects on reading are significantlygreater when instruction occurs in small groups of 27 children (d =0.83)

    compared with on a one-to-one basis (d = 0.45). However, there is no significant

    difference concerning phonics instruction (small groups, d = 0.43; individually,

    d = 0.57).

    Group sessions

    The group sessions begin with letter and word identification training (6 min)

    letters and sight words are introduced and reinforced through multi-sensory

    activities (e.g. writing on whiteboards, using tactile letters). The following 8 min are

    occupied by phonological awareness activities. Predominately with reference to

    Sound Linkage (Hatcher, 2000), children practise blending, segmenting, deleting

    and transposing both syllables and (preferentially) phonemes. The link between

    sounds and letters is also demonstrated, for example, through simple word-building

    activities. Finally, the group narrate a sentence or short story together (e.g. by

    describing a picture or event) and each child writes a section from it, in line with

    their writing capabilities (6 min). Here, children are explicitly encouraged to deploy

    their sound linkage (grapheme-phoneme correspondence) skills to aid spelling. The

    TA scaffolds this activity according to each childs needs (e.g. the TA may spell

    difficult words or allow children to practise writing a word on a whiteboard before

    incorporating it in the final sentence/story).

    Reading intervention 627

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    8/20

    Individual sessions

    The first activity in the individual sessions involves the child reading an easy book

    (4 min)a book that can be read with above 94% accuracy. The childs ability to

    read an instructional level book (9094% accuracy) is then formally assessed(5 min); TAs take a running record (miscue analysis) in which they detail reading

    accuracy on a word-by-word basis, and categorise the nature of any errors made.

    A further 2 min are allowed for making specific teaching points related to the childs

    reading strategies (e.g. introducing new sight words, encouraging phrasing and

    fluency). Finally, the child is introduced to a new book at the instructional level and

    has a first attempt at reading this book alone, followed by a repeated reading with the

    TA employing a scaffolding approach to shared reading (9 min). During all reading

    activities, children are encouraged to use phonic decoding skills when they encounter

    unknown words. For regular words, this might involve the child providing the soundfor every letter in a word and blending those sounds, all through the word, to read it

    (Rose,2006). Other strategies, such as the use of contextual and visual cues, are also

    taught.

    Treatment fidelity

    To monitor the fidelity with which the intervention was delivered, all TAs had one

    teaching session observed. TAs were rated on the quality with which they delivered

    key teaching components of the intervention (1 =

    poor; 2 =

    satisfactory; 3 =

    good).The overall quality (across activities within individual and group sessions) was

    deemed good, with an average rating of 2.71 (0.37) and a range of 13. Sessions were

    also timed and on average lasted 19.09 (5.94) min (target = 20 min). As these TAs

    were experienced in delivering Reading Intervention,and had recently undergone an

    independent observation for quality assurance, this suggests that the intervention was

    delivered with integrity.

    Classroom instruction

    Information was obtained from teachers regarding literacy instruction in the

    classroom, in which children from both the control and intervention groups

    participated. Questionnaires were returned from 6 out of 8 schools (representing 9

    classrooms in total). In these schools, phonics was taught daily for around 15 min.

    Throughout the week, children generally participated in guided reading as well as

    independent or shared reading. More details are given in Table 2.

    Results

    Evaluation of intervention

    The performance of both groups on various outcome measures at t1, t2 and t3 is

    shown in Table 3together with Cohensdas a measure of the size of the difference

    628 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    9/20

    between groups at each time of testing. We wished to assess the extent to which the

    intervention group made greater progress between pre-test (t1) and the immediate

    (t2) and delayed (t3) post-tests, compared to the representative untreated control

    group. A series of one-way analyses of co-variance (ANCOVAs) was carried out for

    this purpose. Short-term effects were assessed by testing for group differences on

    raw scores of various outcome measures immediately after the intervention finished

    (t2), controlling for corresponding t1 scores as the covariates. The results of theseanalyses for the intervention period are summarised by the grey bars in Fig.1,

    which plots the relative gain in raw score (at the mean of the covariate) for the

    intervention group over the control group. Any score greater than 0 represents

    an advantage for the intervention group over the control group; where the

    Table 2 Nature and frequency of reading instruction in the different classrooms

    Classroom Phonics Guided reading Independent

    reading

    Shared

    reading

    Paired

    reading

    Reading cues

    encouraged

    1 15 mina day

    20 min a day Twice aweek

    Phonics, pictures,context, sight

    word

    recognition

    2 20 min

    a day

    3 sessions in

    small groups

    every week

    1 or 2

    sessions

    a week

    Every day Occasionally

    3 1520 min

    a day

    20 min 3 times

    a week

    Phonics, pictures,

    context, sight

    word

    recognition

    4 10 mina day

    1520 minonce a week

    10 mintwice

    a week

    Phonics, pictures,sight word

    recognition

    5 1015 min

    a day

    Once a week 1 or 2

    sessions

    a week

    Range of methods

    6 15 min

    a day

    30 min twice

    a week

    30 min

    twice

    a week

    1520 min

    3 times a

    week

    Phonics, pictures,

    context

    7 15 min

    a day

    15 min twice

    a week

    5 min once

    a week

    Every day 20 min a

    week for

    some

    children

    Phonics, pictures,

    context, sight

    word

    recognition

    8 15 min

    a day

    30 min twice

    a week

    Every day

    9 Regularly 20 min

    twice a

    week

    Guided readingan adult working with a small group of children, grouped by ability, to teach strategies

    for reading with meaning; independent readingchild reading to himself, to encourage fluency and a

    reading habit; paired readingone child reading with one adult or peer; shared readingan adult reading

    with a group of children to model and scaffold fluent and expressive reading (with reference to PrimaryNational Strategy,2003)

    Reading intervention 629

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    10/20

    95% confidence interval does not cross the x-axis, this represents a statistically

    significant effect.

    Figure1 shows that the intervention group made significantly greater progress than

    the control group on three main outcome measures: early word reading, phonetic

    spelling and phoneme awareness. The intervention and control groups made similar

    progress in letter knowledge, word reading and absolute spelling. In terms of

    individual variation in response to intervention, only 5 out of 29 children in the

    intervention group (17%) failed to increase their reading standard scores (where a gain

    in standard score represents progress beyond the expected rate). One child dropped

    5 standard score points, 1 child dropped 1 point, and 3 children remained stationary

    (i.e. showed an expected rate of progress). While there was a general trend for these5 children to perform at a slightly lower level on most measures at t1, the only

    significant difference was on phonetic spelling (F(1,25) = 5.84,p = .023, gp2

    = 0.19).

    The use of ANCOVA to evaluate the effects of intervention relies on the

    assumption of equivalent regression slopes between groups. We assessed this in a

    Table 3 Means (standard deviations) and effect sizes on outcome measures at pre-intervention (t1), post-

    intervention (t2), and 6-month follow-up (t3) for the control and intervention groups

    Measure Test point Max. Control Intervention Effect

    size (d)

    Letter knowledge t1 26 24.57 (3.19) 24.21 (2.06) 0.13

    t2 25.13 (2.36) 25.38 (1.18) 0.13

    t3 25.77 (0.82) 25.85 (0.46) 0.16

    Early word reading t1 50 25.47 (14.54) 11.03 (8.70) 1.21***

    t2 32.37 (14.98) 22.31 (12.12) 0.74**

    t3 38.63 (12.29) 29.38 (11.81) 0.77**

    Word reading, raw t1 90 20.43 (14.95) 8.10 (7.72) 1.04***

    t2 29.67 (17.61) 16.17 (11.36) 0.91**

    t3 39.40 (16.89) 22.19 (13.25) 1.13***

    Word reading, standard t1 101.73 (15.29) 87.72 (9.73) 1.09***

    t2 106.43 (15.77) 94.45 (9.71) 0.91**

    t3 109.03 (13.73) 93.62 (10.77) 1.25***

    Spelling, raw t1 10 2.90 (1.84) 1.03 (1.27) 1.18***

    t2 4.17 (2.48) 2.52 (1.74) 0.77**

    t3 5.33 (2.25) 3.31 (2.31) 0.89**

    Phonetic spelling t1 92 76.70 (18.99) 60.41 (20.27) 0.83**

    t2 81.17 (15.25) 76.86 (9.60) 0.34

    t3 86.77 (6.50) 80.19 (10.06) 0.78**Phoneme awareness t1 18 10.50 (4.23) 5.97 (4.78) 1.00***

    t2 11.17 (4.09) 9.83 (4.25) 0.32

    t3 13.07 (2.85) 10.65 (4.24) 0.67*

    N.B. Group differences tested with one-way analyses of variance: * significant at p\ .05; ** significant

    at p\ .01; *** significant at p\ .001 At t1 and t2, control group N = 30, intervention group N = 29; at t3, control group N = 30, inter-

    vention group N = 26

    630 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    11/20

    series of regression models in which we entered the Covariate, Group (coded 0,1),

    and the Group 9Covariate interaction term as predictors of outcome. In all cases,

    except for phonetic spelling, the Group 9 Covariate interaction term was nonsig-

    nificant (bs =

    0.001.20, ts =

    0.021.57, ps =

    .121.983), which supports the useof an ANCOVA model with fixed slopes across groups. For phonetic spelling the

    Group 9 Covariate interaction was significant indicating non-equivalent slopes

    between groups [b = -0.34,t = -3.95,p\ .001; the slope relatingt2 scores tot1

    scores was steeper in the control group (b = 0.90) than in the intervention group

    (b = 0.80)]. In this model, however, there was a highly significant difference in

    marginal means scores between the groups at the mean level of the covariate, of

    4.58 points in favour of the intervention group (v2 = 6.37, p = .012). This

    difference became progressively larger at lower levels of the covariate (initial

    phonetic spelling score). The results of this analysis confirm that there is a reliable

    effect of the intervention on phonetic spelling for children at the mean of the

    covariate and below, but that the effects of the intervention reduce to nonsignificant

    levels for children who start out with higher phonetic spelling scores.

    Long-term effects of the intervention were evaluated in a similar way, this time

    testing for group differences 6-months after the intervention finished (t3), with

    corresponding t2 scores as the covariates. The results of these ANCOVAs for the

    maintenance period are represented by the white bars on Fig. 1. Any score less than

    0 represents a relative advantage for the control group, over the intervention group;

    where the 95% confidence interval bars cross the x-axis, this represents a

    statistically significant effect.The ANCOVAs show that after the intervention had ceased, the intervention

    groups progress on tests of letter knowledge, early word reading and absolute

    spelling did not differ from that of the control groups, and that its progress on

    phoneme awareness, phonetic spelling and word reading was significantly slower.

    -12

    -10

    -8

    -6

    -4

    -2

    0

    2

    4

    6

    8

    10

    lette

    rkno

    wle

    dge

    phon

    emeaw

    aren

    ess

    early

    wordreadin

    g

    wordreadin

    g

    spellin

    g

    phon

    etic

    spellin

    g

    lette

    rkno

    wle

    dge

    phon

    emea

    wareness

    early

    wordreadin

    g

    wordreadin

    g

    spellin

    g

    phon

    etic

    spellin

    g

    Relativeraws

    corega

    in

    0.22

    0.71

    0.37

    0.10

    0.13

    0.61

    -0.13 -0.25

    0.03

    -0.29 -0.19 -0.17

    Fig. 1 Relative advantage in raw scores (with 95% confidence intervals) on outcome variables for

    the intervention group over the control group across the intervention period (in grey bars) and the

    maintenance period (in white bars). Effect sizes (Cohens d) hover above the bars, calculated as the

    difference between groups on raw gain scores divided by the pooled standard deviation of the covariate

    Reading intervention 631

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    12/20

    Analyses demonstrated that most variables met the assumption of homogeneity

    of regression slopes (bs =0.082.00, ts =0.75 1.46, ps = .151.455)allowing

    a fixed slopes, varying intercepts ANCOVA. The assumption was not met for

    phonetic spelling [b = 0.44, t = 3.93, p\ .001; the slope relating t3 scores to t2

    scores was steeper in the intervention group (b = 0.79) than in the control group(b =0.70)]. In a model including the Group 9 Covariate interaction, however,

    there was a significant difference in marginal means scores between the groups at

    the mean level of the covariate, of 4.04 points in favour of the control group

    (v2 =7.26, p = .007). This difference became progressively larger at lower levels

    of the covariate (phonetic spelling score at immediate post-test).These results show

    that, for those whose post-test phonetic spelling scores fall at the mean of the

    covariate and below, children in the control group are progressing more than those

    in the intervention group on phonetic spelling during the maintenance period. This

    difference in progress reduces to nonsignificant levels for children who have higherphonetic spelling scores at immediate post-test.

    The measure of phoneme awareness also failed to meet the assumption of

    homogeneity of regression slopes [b = 0.39, t = 2.13, p = .151; the slope relating

    t3 scores to t2 scores was steeper in the intervention group (b = 0.87) than in the

    control group (b = 0.85)]. As with phonetic spelling, in a model including the

    Group 9 Covariate interaction there was a significant difference in marginal means

    between the groups at the mean level of the covariate, of 1.42 points in favour of the

    control group (v2 = 8.12,p = .004). This difference became progressively larger at

    lower levels of the covariate (phoneme awareness at immediate post-test).Thus, forthose whose post-test phoneme awareness scores fall at the mean of the covariate

    and below, children in the control group are progressing more than those in the

    intervention group on phoneme awareness during the maintenance period. Again,

    this difference in progress reduces to nonsignificant levels for children who have

    higher phoneme awareness scores at immediate post-test.

    In sum the intervention was shown to be effective. On three key literacy

    measures (phoneme awareness, reading and spelling) the progress made during the

    intervention period by a group of children selected as having poor reading skills was

    greater than the progress made by a representative group of children who received

    regular classroom teaching (including daily phonics training and reading practice

    for most children). However, the rate of progress of the children in the intervention

    group was not sustained once intervention was withdrawn. During this maintenance

    period, their progress on letter knowledge, early word reading and spelling was not

    different from that of the control groups but their progress on word reading,

    phoneme awareness and phonetic spelling was significantly slower (at least, for the

    latter two variables, for children performing at lower levels at the end of the

    intervention).

    Learner profiles

    To obtain a better understanding of the characteristics of children selected to take

    part in Reading Intervention, their abilities on a range of linguistic, cognitive and

    behavioural measures were compared to those of the control group (see Table 3).

    632 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    13/20

    As expected, the control group performed significantly better than the intervention

    group on all measures of reading and spelling, with large effect sizes. The difference

    in letter knowledge was not significant, though this reflects ceiling effects in both

    groups. Performance on the remaining measures is shown in Table4. The

    intervention group displayed significant deficits in phonological skills, as measured

    by phoneme awareness and rapid object naming, though it did not differ

    significantly from the control group on nonword repetition. The intervention group

    performed significantly worse than the control group on all measures of grammar,

    but both groups showed age-appropriate receptive vocabulary knowledge. Finally,

    the two groups were equated in terms of their behavioural and attentional skills, but

    the intervention group performed significantly worse than the control group on the

    test of nonverbal reasoning.

    Children were selected for Reading Intervention according to relative weaknessesin reading, based on teacher evaluations. Restricting profile analyses to children

    who reach an objective definition of a reading deficit will give a more accurate

    picture of the language profiles of such children. A reading deficit was quantified as

    performance on the Early Word Reading test at least 1.25 SD below the control

    Table 4 Comparing the means (and standard deviations) of the control (N = 30) and intervention

    (N = 29) groups on a variety of cognitive, linguistic, and behavioural measures at t1

    Measure Score Max. Control Intervention Effect

    size (d)

    Nonword repetition Raw 40 25.50 (5.93) 23.14 (7.43) 0.35

    Standarda

    105.03 (14.80) 98.93 (15.39) 0.41

    Rapid naming Seconds 139.70 (32.62) 163.16 (33.04) 0.71**

    Standardb 97.35 (14.16) 80.00 (14.97) 1.19**

    Receptive vocabulary Raw 168 64.27 (13.28) 60.97 (14.77) 0.23

    Standard 102.77 (11.97) 99.55 (12.24) 0.27

    Picture naming Raw 25 8.87 (4.09) 6.72 (4.21) 0.52

    Standardb

    10.40 (2.48) 7.89 (2.89) 0.93**

    Receptive grammar Raw 20 8.07 (3.61) 6.24 (3.18) 0.54*Standard 89.40 (15.59) 80.79 (13.48) 0.59*

    Recalling sentences Raw 78 23.13 (9.24) 16.45 (9.92) 0.70*

    Scaledb

    8.55 (2.87) 5.61 (2.15) 1.16**

    Past tense production Percentc 100 70.79 (12.10) 60.59 (17.19) 0.69*

    Nonverbal reasoning Raw 59 26.60 (9.16) 20.76 (9.66) 0.62*

    T-score 52.60 (9.50) 44.93 (10.02) 0.79**

    Hyperactivity Raw 10 3.73 (2.88) 4.69 (2.98) 0.33

    Total social deviance Raw 40 8.77 (6.55) 9.62 (5.44) 0.14

    Inhibition Raw 30 14.47 (5.08) 14.86 (4.43) 0.08T-score 52.87 (9.68) 54.07 (11.54) 0.11

    a Control group N = 30, intervention group N = 27; b Control group N = 20, intervention group

    N = 18; c Control group N = 28, intervention group N = 27

    * Significant at p\ .05; ** significant at p\ .01; *** Significant at p\ .001; p = .051

    Reading intervention 633

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    14/20

    group mean; this cut-off was chosen so that around 15% of children in the control

    group were characterised with a reading deficitin line with normal distribution. In

    all, 15 children met the inclusion criterion (4 control and 11 intervention children).

    Deficits in aspects of oral language were defined in a similar way (at least 1.25 SD

    below the control mean). Composite z-scores were created for vocabulary (recep-tive and expressive vocabulary) and grammar (receptive grammar and recalling

    sentences). Phonology was represented by phoneme awareness skills. The measure

    of nonverbal reasoning was used to define difficulties beyond language.

    The prevalence of additional deficits in children with or without a reading deficit

    is given in Table5. The frequencies suggest that language deficits are more

    prevalent in children with reading deficit than those without. Indeed, chi-square tests

    revealed that language and reading deficits are non-independent; classification of the

    presence of a language deficit is contingent upon presence of a reading deficit. In

    contrast, nonverbal deficits were independent of reading deficits; children were

    equally likely to have a nonverbal deficit regardless of reading status.

    The co-occurrence of reading and language deficits in individual children were

    then considered. Forty-six percent of children with a reading deficit had an isolated

    phonological deficit (compared with 7% of children without reading deficit).

    However, deficits in phonology also patterned highly with deficits in nonphono-

    logical oral language: 47% of children with a reading deficit had co-occurring

    deficits in phonology and vocabulary or grammarusually both (compared with

    9%). In contrast, isolated difficulties in nonphonological oral language skills in the

    presence of intact phonological skills did not feature in children with a reading

    deficit (compared with 11% of children without a reading deficit).

    Summary of results

    A 10-week reading intervention programme was delivered by trained TAs to 6-year-

    old children who had been identified by their teachers as exhibiting relative

    weaknesses in word reading. The progress of these children was assessed before and

    after the intervention, and compared with a representative sample of age- and

    gender-matched children from the same classrooms. The intervention group made

    significantly greater gains over the course of the intervention than a representativecontrol group from the same classrooms on phoneme awareness, early word reading

    and phonetic spelling. These gains were maintained over a 6-month no-intervention

    maintenance period; though in general children in the intervention group were

    progressing at a slower rate than those in the control group.

    Table 5 Prevalence of

    additional deficits in children

    with or without a reading deficit

    N.B. Critical values ofv2 with 1

    df: 3.84,p = .05; 6.64, p = .01;

    10.83, p = .001

    No reading deficit

    (N = 44)

    Reading deficit

    (N = 15)

    v2 p

    Phonology 7 (16%) 14 (93%) 29.84 .001

    Vocabulary 6 (14%) 6 (40%) 4.80 .05Grammar 4 (9%) 6 (40%) 7.61 .01

    Nonverbal 11 (25%) 5 (33%) 0.39 NS

    634 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    15/20

    Comparisons of the two groups prior to the intervention indicated that children who

    were identified by their teachers as needing Reading Intervention showed relative

    weaknesses in literacy and phonology, as expected, but also wider difficulties in

    nonphonological oral language (vocabulary and grammar) and nonverbal ability. This

    pattern was confirmed in a profile analysis, which demonstrated that deficits in bothphonological and nonphonological aspects of oral language frequently co-occurred

    with reading deficits.

    Discussion

    Previous research has demonstrated that the Reading Intervention programme

    successfully improves the reading abilities of struggling readers. This study aimed

    to build on this evidence base by conducting an educationally realistic evaluation ofReading Intervention, carried out in the wake of recent shifts in educational policy

    relating to general reading instruction (e.g. Rose, 2006). The present study also

    investigated the linguistic profiles of children selected by their teachers to

    participate in Reading Intervention.

    Twenty-nine 6-year-old children with reading difficulties who received Reading

    Intervention underwent a comprehensive cognitivelinguistic assessment, and

    performance on key phonological and literacy tasks was measured before and after

    intervention. The progress of the intervention groups was compared with that of a

    control group (children from the same classes, matched on age and gender). ReadingIntervention was again associated with improvement in key literacy skills. In

    addition to expected deficits in phonology and literacy, some children with reading

    difficulties demonstrated significant weaknesses in nonphonological oral language

    (vocabulary and grammar).

    Efficacy of reading intervention

    The present evaluation of Reading Intervention adds to previous studies which

    demonstrate its effectiveness in improving literacy skills of struggling readers (after

    Hatcher et al., 1994, 2004, 2006a, b). Here, the intervention and control groups

    made significant progress throughout the intervention period. After controlling for

    differences in baseline performance, the groups made statistically equivalent gains

    on a standardised test of word reading, letter knowledge and absolute accuracy of

    spelling attempts. Crucially, the intervention group demonstrated significantly

    greater progress than the control group in early word reading, phoneme awareness

    and phonetic spelling (though for the latter variable, this effect was only significant

    for children who started out with lower levels of phonetic spelling). It must be noted

    that children in the intervention group remained significantly behind their peers in

    single word reading and spelling skills.

    A common metric for comparing intervention studies is the number of standard

    score points in reading gained per hour of intervention (McGuiness, McGuiness, &

    McGuiness,1996). A review of reading interventions which incorporate training in

    phonemic awareness and phonemic decoding (akin to Reading Intervention) reports

    Reading intervention 635

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    16/20

    an average increase of 0.20 standard score points per hour (Torgesen et al., 2001).

    The present implementation of Reading Intervention obtained an average gain of

    0.59 standard score points per hour, which compares very favourably with previous

    studies. Furthermore, only 5 of 29 children in the intervention group (17%) failed to

    show an increase in reading standard score over the intervention period. Thisstatistic also compares well to other studies (e.g. Hatcher et al., 2006b).

    The progress of all children was monitored 6 months after the intervention had

    finished. In both groups, maintenance or even continued progress in literacy skills

    was shown. Strikingly, while the intervention group either equalled or excelled the

    control groups rate of progress during the intervention period, their rate of progress

    lagged behind that of the control group during the maintenance period. Though

    confirming the effectiveness of Reading Intervention, this implies that some poor

    readers may need support for longer than 10 weeks. Future research might pursue

    the hypothesis that children who are gradually phased out of Reading Interventionafter 10 weeks will demonstrate better maintenance of treatment gains than those

    who experience an immediate cessation.

    When drawing conclusions from this study, it must be borne in mind that there was

    a lack of random assignment of children to groups, and that groups were not matched

    on literacy skills at the outset. While we acknowledge that an RCT would have been

    preferable, we nonetheless argue that the results are still meaningful. On the basis of

    previous RCTs (e.g. Hatcher et al., 2006b), we have good reason to suppose a priori

    that Reading Intervention is causal in producing gains in literacy. Furthermore, the

    very design of this study is conservative. Children were selected by their teachers toreceive intervention on the basis that they had weaknesses in reading relative to their

    peers; that is, they had been showing slower progressachieving a lower level of

    literacy compared to their peers, despite the same length of instruction. It is therefore a

    reasonable assumption that the intervention groups progress would have continued to

    be relatively slower, had the intervention not been effective. Yet our results show that

    these poor readers made more progress than a representative group of children who

    themselves were making at least average progress in reading. Finally, the relatively

    slower pace of progress of the intervention group in the no-intervention follow-up

    period confirms this treatment effect.

    Learner profiles

    A second aim of this study was to assess the linguistic profiles of children with

    reading difficulties. Children selected to receive Reading Intervention tended to

    display weaknesses in expressive and receptive grammar and expressive vocabu-

    lary, in addition to their expected weaknesses in phonological skills.

    The children given intervention in this study were selected by their teachers as

    having reading difficulties, relative to their class peers. Consequently, the degree of

    reading difficulty varied across schools, and not all children included in the

    intervention group would have difficulties enough to qualify as reading disabled

    according to typical diagnostic criteria. This observation is in line with other

    research which shows that while teachers are good at identifying the children at-risk

    of dyslexia (good sensitivity), they tend to over-estimate the number of children

    636 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    17/20

    who are truly at-risk (poorer specificity; Snowling, Duff, Petrou, Schiffeldrin, &

    Bailey, in revision).

    To gain a more accurate understanding of the co-occurrence of reading and

    language difficulties, the whole sample was re-categorised according to the presence

    or absence of a reading deficit, as defined relative to the average readingperformance of the control group. Language (but not non-verbal) deficits were

    significantly more likely to occur in children with a reading deficit, compared to

    those without. A large majority of children with reading deficits had a phonological

    deficit (93%). Interestingly, the occurrence of this phonological deficit was split

    across two sub-types of reading disability: 46% of those with reading deficits had a

    concomitant deficit in phonological aspects of oral language alone; another 47% had

    co-occurring deficits in both phonological and nonphonological aspects of oral

    language. Such profiles approximate both classic dyslexia and its broader

    phenotype, respectively (Scarborough, 1990; Snowling, Gallagher, & Frith, 2003;Snowling, Muter, & Carroll,2007). These results mirror the findings of Catts, Fey,

    Zhang, and Tomblin (1999), who tracked the development of a large sample of

    children, categorising them as either good or poor readers in their third year of

    schooling, and retrospectively considered their language skills at school entry. Both

    studies support the conclusion that weaknesses in nonphonological oral language

    feature among a significant proportion of children identified primarily for their

    reading difficulties.

    It is important to consider why broader oral language difficulties are observed in

    children who present primarily with a reading deficit. The most prominent theory ofdyslexia (and by extrapolation, reading disorders) asserts that the main cause is a

    core deficit in phonological processing (Stanovich, 1988; Vellutino, Fletcher,

    Snowling, & Scanlon, 2004). At first blush, this explanation would not anticipate

    concomitant deficits in broader oral language skills; nonetheless, some attempt may

    be made at reconciling these broader language deficits within this theoretical view.

    In terms of vocabulary skills, the children selected for Reading Intervention

    exhibited average receptive skills, with a tendency for weaknesses in expressive

    skills. It is hypothesised that expressive vocabulary is more closely related to word

    reading, as task performance necessitates access to fully specified phonological and

    semantic representations (Chiappe, Chiappe, & Gottardo, 2004; Wise, Sevcik,

    Morris, Lovett, & Wolf, 2007). Children with dyslexia are known to struggle with

    word finding and confrontational naming and these problems are commonly assumed

    to stem from phonological weaknesses, either in the encoding or accessing of

    phonological representations (Nation, Marshall, & Snowling, 2001; Snowling, van

    Wagtendonk, & Stafford, 1988; Swan & Goswami, 1997; see Messer & Dockrell,

    2006 for discussion). The current study also found poor morphological skills in

    children participating in Reading Intervention; it has been argued elsewhere that

    morphological impairments may be caused by a phonological deficit (Bird et al.,

    2003; Chiat,2001; Joanisse & Seidenberg,1999).

    Thus, the observation of expressive vocabulary and morphological deficits in

    children with reading difficulties is not necessarily incompatible with the

    phonological deficit hypothesis. Notwithstanding this, it is less clear how the

    observed deficit in receptive syntax could be accounted for by weaknesses in

    Reading intervention 637

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    18/20

    phonological processing. The precise nature and direction of the relationship

    between the various subdomains of oral language (phonology, morphosyntax and

    semantics) requires further study, and this will have implications for theories of

    reading development and disorders. However, the critical point for present purposes

    is that these subdomains of oral language are likely to interact. Coupling this withthe knowledge that reading disorders are characterised by a primary deficit in

    phonological processing (Vellutino et al., 2004), it becomes unsurprising that

    nonphonological oral language difficulties are observed in children with reading

    disability.

    Clearly, future longitudinal studies need to assess the nature of the relationships

    between intra-domain (phonological and nonphonological oral language) and inter-

    domain (oral and written language) deficits in children at risk of reading difficulties.

    Summary and conclusions

    This study adds to earlier evaluations of Reading Intervention and shows that when

    delivered in an educationally realistic way, it is associated with improvements in

    fundamental literacy skills. The findings support growing evidence that deficits in

    both phonological and nonphonological aspects of oral language co-occur with

    reading difficulties. Such data are in line with the thesis put forward by Snowling

    (2008) that a phonological deficit is the main cause of reading difficulties, but that

    this risk factor may be moderated by weaknesses in broader (nonphonological) orallanguage weaknesses. The finding that weaknesses in nonphonological areas of

    language typify a significant proportion of children with reading difficulties suggests

    that future intervention programmes ought to address both phonological and

    nonphonological aspects of oral language as foundations for literacy development.

    Acknowledgments The present study was funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences

    Research Council. Grateful thanks are extended to Maggie Snowling for her input, to North Yorkshire

    Children and Young Peoples Services for support of this work (particularly Simon Gibbs and Glynnis

    Smith), and to all of the pupils, teaching assistants and schools who participated.

    References

    Adams, C., Cooke, R., Crutchley, A., Hesketh, A., & Reeves, D. (2001). Assessment of comprehension

    and expression 6-11. Windsor: NFER Nelson.

    Bird, H., Lambon Ralph, M. A. L., Seidenberg, M. S., McClelland, J. L., & Patterson, K. (2003). Deficits

    in phonology and past-tense morphology: Whats the connection? Journal of Memory and

    Language, 48, 502526.

    Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Test for reception of Grammar (version 2). London: The Psychological

    Corporation.

    Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysisof experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403414.

    Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading

    disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331361.

    Chiappe, P., Chiappe, D. L., & Gottardo, A. (2004). Vocabulary, context and speech perception among

    good and poor readers. Educational Psychology, 24, 825843.

    638 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    19/20

    Chiat, S. (2001). Mapping theories of developmental language impairment: Premises, predictions and

    evidence.Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 113142.

    Duff, F. J., Fieldsend, E., Bowyer-Crane, C., Hulme, C., Smith, G., Gibbs, S., et al. (2008). Reading with

    vocabulary intervention: Evaluation of an instruction for children with poor response to reading

    intervention.Journal of Reading Research, 31, 319336.

    Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). The British picture vocabulary scale(2nd ed.). Windsor: NFER Nelson.

    Elliot, C. D., Smith, P., & McCulloch, K. (1997). British ability scales II. Windsor: NFER Nelson.

    Frederickson, N., Frith, U., & Reason, R. (1997). Phonological assessment battery. Windsor: NFER-

    Nelson.

    Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). The childrens test of nonword repetition. London:

    The Psychological Corporation.

    Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behaviour rating inventory of executive

    functioning. Florida, USA: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

    Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child

    Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581586.

    Hatcher, P. J. (2000). Sound linkage: An integrated programme for overcoming reading difficulties

    (2nd ed.). London: Whurr.

    Hatcher, P. J., Gotz, K., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., Gibbs, S., & Smith, G. (2006a). Evidence for the

    effectiveness of the early literacy support programme. British Journal of Educational Psychology,

    73, 351367.

    Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., & Ellis, A. W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating

    the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage hypothesis. Child

    Development, 65, 4157.

    Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., Miles, J. N. V., Carroll, J. M., Hatcher, J., Gibbs, S., et al. (2006b). Efficacy of

    small-group reading intervention for beginning readers with reading delay: A randomised control

    trial.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 820827.

    Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Explicit phoneme training combined with phonic

    reading instruction helps young children at risk of reading failure. Journal of Child Psychology andPsychiatry, 45, 338358.

    Joanisse, M., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Impairments in verb morphology following brain injury:

    A connectionist model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 75927597.

    McGuiness, C., McGuiness, D., & McGuiness, G. (1996). Phono-Graphix: A new method for remediating

    reading difficulties. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 7396.

    Messer, D., & Dockrell, J. E. (2006). Childrens naming and word-finding difficulties: Descriptions and

    explanations.Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 49, 309324.

    Nation, K., Marshall, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). Phonological and semantic contributions to childrens

    picture naming skill: Evidence from children with developmental reading disorders. Language and

    Cognitive Processes, 16, 241259.

    National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (2000)Report of the National Reading Panel.

    Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature onreading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups (NIH Publication

    No. 00-4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Primary National Strategy. (2003).Guided reading: Supporting transition from key stage 1 to key stage 2.

    London: Department for Education and Skills (DfES).

    Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (2001). Test of early grammatical impairment. Psychological Corporation.

    Rose, J. (2006). Independent review of the teaching of early reading: Final report (The Rose Review).

    London: Department for Education and Skills (DfES).

    Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61,

    17281734.

    Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (1995). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (3rd ed.).

    London: The Psychological Corporation.Snowling, M. J. (2008). Specific disorders and broader phenotypes: The case of dyslexia. The Quarterly

    Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 142156.

    Snowling, M. J., Duff, F. J., Petrou, A., Schiffeldrin, J., & Bailey, A. M. (in revision). Definition and

    assessment for dyslexia: The validity of teacher ratings. Journal of Research in Reading.

    Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual

    differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358373.

    Reading intervention 639

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Duff Fiona J

    20/20

    Snowling, M. J., Muter, V., & Carroll, J. (2007). Children at family risk of dyslexia: A follow-up in early

    adolescence.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 609618.

    Snowling, M. J., Stothard, S. E., Clarke, P., Bowyer-Crane, C., Harrington, A., Truelove, E., et al. (2009).

    York assessment of reading for comprehension. G L Assessment.

    Snowling, M. J., van Wagtendonk, B., & Stafford, C. (1988). Object-naming deficits in developmental

    dyslexia.Journal of Research in Reading, 11, 6785.Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor

    reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol-

    ogy, 38, 175190.

    Swan, D., & Goswami, U. (1997). Picture naming deficits in developmental dyslexia: The phonological

    representation hypothesis. Brain and Language, 56, 334353.

    Torgerson, C. J., Brooks, G., & Hall, G. (2006).A systematic review of the research literature on the use

    of systematic phonics in the teaching of reading and spelling. Department for Education and Skills.

    Torgesen, J. K. (2005). Recent discoveries on remedial interventions for children with dyslexia.

    In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook(pp. 521537). Oxford:

    Blackwell.

    Torgesen, J. K., Alexander, A. W., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Voeller, K., & Conway, T. (2001).

    Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: Immediate and long-term

    outcomes from two instructional approaches. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 3358, 78.

    Vellutino, F. V., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability

    (dyslexia): What we have learned in the past four decades. Journal of Child Psychology and

    Psychiatry, 45, 240.

    Wise, J. C., Sevcik, R. A., Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., & Wolf, M. (2007). The relationship among

    receptive and expressive vocabulary, listening comprehension, pre-reading skills, word identifica-

    tion skills, and reading comprehension by children with reading disabilities. Journal of Speech-

    Language-Hearing Research, 50, 10931109.

    640 F. J. Duff et al.

    1 3