Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

19
0165–2516/11/0208–0005 Int’l. J. Soc. Lang. 208 (2011), pp. 5–23 © Walter de Gruyter DOI 10.1515/IJSL.2011.010 Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English competence and well-being in Latino children of immigrants in kindergarten BRIAN A. COLLINS, CLAUDIO O. TOPPELBERG, CAROLA SUáREZ- OROZCO, ERIN O’CONNOR, and ALFONSO NIETO-CASTAñON Abstract The aim of this study is to examine within an ecological model the associations of dual language (Spanish/English) competences with the emotional and be- havioral well-being of young children of immigrants in kindergarten. Latino children of immigrants (n = 228) from a public school-based community sam- ple were assessed using standardized, comprehensive measures of Spanish and English oral language competence and normed teacher reports on five dimen- sions of emotional and behavioral well-being and school functioning (inter- personal, intrapersonal, and affective strengths, connection to family, and school functioning). Relevant contextual factors at home (maternal education, poverty, family structure) and school (teacher experience and practices, class- room composition), as well as child factors (non-verbal IQ, gender) were con- sidered. Spanish and English competences accounted for moderate to large portions of variance in all dimensions of well-being. The contributions of child, home, and school variables to well-being were much smaller than lan- guage competence, and in most cases, not significant. Our findings suggest that dual language competence is critically associated with the emotional and behavioral well-being and school functioning of Latino children of immigrants. Keywords: childhood bilingualism and well being; Latino immigrant children; dual language competence; affect and childhood bilingualism. 1. Introduction Children of immigrants comprise close to one quarter of the United States’ child population, and are expected to reach close to one third by 2020, constituting the fastest growing segment of the United States’ child population (Mather 2009; Suárez-Orozco and Suárez-Orozco 2001). The majority of children of

Transcript of Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Page 1: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

0165–2516/11/0208–0005 Int’l. J. Soc. Lang. 208 (2011), pp. 5–23©WalterdeGruyter DOI10.1515/IJSL.2011.010

Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English competence and well-being in Latino

children of immigrants in kindergarten

Brian a. Collins, Claudio o. ToppelBerg, Carola suárez-orozCo, erin o’Connor, and alfonso nieTo-CasTañon

Abstract

The aim of this study is to examine within an ecological model the associations of dual language (Spanish/English) competences with the emotional and be-havioral well-being of young children of immigrants in kindergarten. Latino children of immigrants (n = 228) from a public school-based community sam-ple were assessed using standardized, comprehensive measures of Spanish and English oral language competence and normed teacher reports on five dimen-sions of emotional and behavioral well-being and school functioning (inter-personal, intrapersonal, and affective strengths, connection to family, and school functioning). Relevant contextual factors at home (maternal education, poverty, family structure) and school (teacher experience and practices, class-room composition), as well as child factors (non-verbal IQ, gender) were con-sidered. Spanish and English competences accounted for moderate to large portions of variance in all dimensions of well-being. The contributions of child, home, and school variables to well-being were much smaller than lan-guage competence, and in most cases, not significant. Our findings suggest that dual language competence is critically associated with the emotional and behavioral well-being and school functioning of Latino children of i mmigrants.

Keywords: childhood bilingualism and well being; Latino immigrant children; dual language competence; affect and childhood bilingualism.

1. Introduction

ChildrenofimmigrantscompriseclosetoonequarteroftheUnitedStates’childpopulation,andareexpectedtoreachclosetoonethirdby2020,constitutingthe fastest growing segment of theUnitedStates’ child population (Mather2009;Suárez-OrozcoandSuárez-Orozco2001).Themajorityofchildrenof

Page 2: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

6 B. A. Collins et al.

immigrantsspeakalanguageotherthanEnglishinthehome,whichinmostcases(70percent) isSpanish(U.S.Census2000).The increaseofdual lan-guagechildren(uniquehomeandschoollanguages)overthepastfewdecadeshas had an enormous impact on United States schools. However, the waychildren’scompetenceineachlanguagerelatestotheiremotionalandbehav-ioralwell-beingispoorlyunderstoodandindireneedofconcertedresearchefforts.Latinochildrenofimmigrantsarefacedwithmultipleriskfactorsforemo-

tional,behavioral,andacademicproblems—evenmoresothanotherminorityandwhiteyouthofsimilarsocioeconomicstatus(Arciaetal.1994;Gándaraetal.1998;Steinbergetal.1992).SomeoftheriskfactorsandchallengesLatinochildrenofimmigrantsfaceincludepoverty,lowlevelsofparentaleducation,limitedaccesstohealthcare,anddiscrimination(Hernandezetal.2007).Latinosalsohaveanumberofstrengths,suchasastrongsenseofcommunity,religion,optimism,familysupports,andhighvalueofeducation(Pumariega2009).Muchofthecurrentresearchonthedevelopmentofchildrenofimmigrants

has focused primarily on the risk for emotional, behavioral, and academicproblems.Thereisaneedforstrength-basedmodels,whichintegratepotentialprotectiveattributesofLatinochildrenof immigrants thatmay impact theiremotional, behavioral, and academic well-being. Many Latino children ofimmigrants have shown to be extremely resilient despite risk and adversity(Masten1994). Inaddition to themultiplepersonal, family,andcommunitycharacteristics supportingLatino children, language competence in SpanishandEnglishmaybepositivelyassociatedwiththeiremotionalandbehavioralwell-beingandschoolfunctioning.Ithasbeenwelldocumentedthatlanguagecompetenceisacriticalcontribu-

tortotheemotionalandbehavioraldevelopmentofmonolingualchildren(Be-itchmanetal.1996;ToppelbergandShapiro2000).However,thisassociationhasnotyetbeenfullyinvestigatedamongduallanguagechildren,whooftenhavewidelyvaryingcompetences ineach language.Whilemaintaininganddevelopinglinguisticcompetencesinbothafirstlanguage(L1)andasecondlanguage(L2)cansupporthealthyadaptation,rapidshiftsfromhometoschoollanguage, loss of home language, and inadequate English proficiency mayhavenegativepsychosocialandeducationalconsequences(Snow1991;WongFillmore1991).Thepresentstudyinvestigatestheassociationofduallanguagecompetences

with dimensions of emotional and behavioralwell-being during the criticaldevelopmentalstageofschoolentry—kindergarten.Forchildrenof immi-grants,enteringschooltypicallyrepresentsthefirstmassivecontactwiththeEnglishlanguage,aswellasAmericancultureandsocietyatlarge.Children’scompetencesintheirL1(Spanish)andL2(English)areconsideredinrelationtotheirwell-being(emotionalandbehavioralstrengthsandschoolfunction-

Page 3: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Spanish and English competence of immigrant children 7

ing)withinanecologicalmodelofhomeandschoolfactorsaswellasotherchildcharacteristics.

2. Literaturereview

2.1. Association of language competence and emotional and behavioral well-being

Thesuccessfuldevelopmentoflanguageskillsisacornerstoneofchildhoodmentalhealth(U.S.SurgeonGeneral1999),throughitsdirectrelationshiptothechild’sself-expression,senseofidentity,andabilitytonegotiateandcon-nectwithpeersandfamilymembers.Languageencodesandlabelsemotionsand participates in the regulations of mental states (Aragno and Schlachet1996; Foster 1996). Language competence often underlies positive interac-tionswithpeersandadults,andtheadaptiveexpressionofemotionandappro-priateresponsetostimuli(Farmer1997).Additionally,languagecompetenceiscriticaltotheabilitytocommunicateneedsanddesires(Crittenden1996).So-cial,emotional,andbehavioralcompetencesandstrengths—suchassharing,regulating anger and other emotions, understanding directions and expecta-tions,identifyingfeelings,anddiscussingproblems—areallintricatelycon-nectedtochildren’slanguagecompetence(Ruehlmanetal.1999).Languagecompetenceisparticularlyimportantintheschoolsettinginorderforoptimallearningtooccurandforchildrentofeelemotionallysecure.Socialskills,suchasmakingfriends,displayingasenseofhumor,andrespondingtoandexpress-ingaffectiontoothers,alsorelyheavilyonlanguagecompetence(CoppleandBredekamp2009).The link betweenlanguagecompetenceandemotionalandbehavioralde-

velopment has been evidenced in monolingual children (Beitchman et al.1996),butempiricalresearchfocusingondual languagechildrenis limited.Forexample,childhoodlanguagedisordersarehighlyoverlappedwithchildmentalhealthproblemsinmonolinguals(CantwellandBaker1991;Toppel-bergetal.2002).Yetthisrelationshipinduallanguagechildrenhasbeendocu-mented,toourknowledge,inonlyonestudy(Toppelbergetal.2002).Dual language children are often moving between language contexts

throughouttheday,astheirlifeoftendemandsexclusiveuseofthefirstlan-guageathomeandthesecondlanguageinotherenvironments,suchasschool.Formanychildren,thereisaremarkablecontrastbetweenthesetwosettings.Languageathometendstobecontext-embeddedandcommunicativelybased,withface-to-faceandnon-verbalsupportstoaidinunderstanding.Incontrast,language at school is typically academic and cognitively demanding, oftencontext-reduced and abstract, requiring higher order thinking skills and a

Page 4: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

8 B. A. Collins et al.

relianceonlinguisticdeviceswithoutconnectiontosupportivecontext(Cum-mins1979).Thedevelopmentofchildren’shomelanguagemaybeassociatedwithstrengtheningoffamilycohesionandintimacy,establishingparentalau-thority,andtransmittingculturalnorms,allofwhichcanleadtohealthyadjust-mentandastrongidentificationandinternalizationofthesocialvaluesofthefamily (Fishman 1991; Tseng and Fuligni 2000). The development of theschoolandcommunitylanguageiscrucialforacademicsuccessandlong-termsocialandeconomicwell-being(Bianchi1984;Carhilletal.2008).Children’sabilitytofunctionwithintheschoolcontextinfluencesschoolretention,gradu-ationrates,andcontinuationintohighereducation.Schoolsarealsoanimportantsocialcontextwherethedevelopmentofcrit-

icalemotionalandbehavioralstrengthstakeplace(Bakeretal.2008).Schooliswherechildrenformfriendshipswithsameagepeersaswellasrelationshipswith teachers, which contribute to emotional and behavioral development(O’Connoretal.2011).Withinschoolrelationships,childrenlearntouseinter-personal and intrapersonal strategies (Silver et al. 2005) and form adaptiveemotionalandbehavioralskills(Bakeretal.2008).Atschool,childrenreceiveconstantfeedbackontheirbehavior,onwhatconstitutesageappropriatebe-haviorandcompetences,andonhowtheymeasureuptoothers.Havingade-quatelanguagecompetencetoprocessfeedbackandmeetexpectationsiscrit-icaltopositiveadaptationsanddevelopment.Theabilityofthechildtoadaptto varyingdemandsof the early school years affects both their current andlong-termemotionalandbehavioralwell-beingandacademicsuccess(Bianchi1984).This isparticularly thecase forLatinochildren, forwhomacademicdisparitiesarewelldocumented(Gándaraetal.1998;Steinbergetal.1993).

2.2. Emotional and behavioral well-being of Latinos

The importanceofpositive emotional andbehavioral adjustment forLatinochildrenofimmigrantsiswellunderstoodbyresearchersandeducators.Latinoparentsfrequentlysharethegoalofhavingtheirchildrendevelopinstrumentalcompetencesandofpreservingvaluesrelatedtotheintrapersonal(  personal-ismo),theinterpersonal(respeto),connectionstothefamily(   familismo),theexpression of affection (cariños) and the worth of education (educación)(Suárez-Orozco2002).ThesetypesofstrengthsareanimportantpartofLatinotraditionsandvalues(ChapmanandPerreira2005;Suárez-OrozcoandSuárez-Orozco2001).Amultidimensionalperspectivethatfocusesoninterpersonal,intrapersonal,andaffectivestrengthsaswellasconnectionstothefamilyandschool functioningseemsfundamental ingainingadeeperunderstandingofthewell-beingofLatinochildrenofimmigrants.

Page 5: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Spanish and English competence of immigrant children 9

InterpersonalstrengthsarevaluedinLatinocultureandchildrenareencour-agedtocommunicatewithrespect,defertoauthority,andcooperateandactinapleasantmanner(Flanagan1996).Intrapersonalstrengthsareevidentinchil-dren’sself-reflection,identifyingfeelingsandtheirself-confidence(Ruehlmanetal.1999).Affectivestrengthsmanifestinchildren’sabilitytoregulateemo-tionsandbehaviors.FamilycohesionisapredominantLatinoculturalvalueinvolvingastrongsenseofattachmentandloyaltytofamilyandtheviewofthefamilyasanextensionoftheself(Riveraetal.2008).

2.3. Emotional and behavioral well-being within an ecological model

Understandingchilddevelopmentandwell-beingrequiresconsiderationofthecharacteristicsofthechild,home,andschool,whichareparticularlyimportantwhenconsideringchildrenof immigrants (GarciaColletal.1996;KaoandTienda1998;Ogbu1987;PortesandRumbaut2001).Ecologicalmodelspositthatchildrendevelopwithininterrelatedsystemsthatexistatlevelsproximalanddistaltothechildandactinconcerttoinfluencedevelopment(Bronfen-brenner1979).Ecological factors theoretically linked to both language competence and

well-beingincludecharacteristicsofthechild(genderandcognitivecompe-tence),thehome(maternaleducation,poverty,andthechild-per-adultratio),andtheschool(teacherexperience,transitionpractices,andtheproportionofSpanishspeakers)areconsideredinthepresentstudy.Inregardstochildfactors,PortesandSchauffler(1994)foundthatgirlsare

morelikelytoretaintheirparents’ languageandoftentakeonmorefamily-oriented responsibilities. Cognitive competences act as protective factorsamong high-risk children. High cognitive functioning predicts academicachievement and positive emotional and behavioral outcomes (White et al.1989)andisassociatedwithbilingualproficiencyindual languagechildren(Cummins1977).Athomematernaleducation,familyincome,andstructureplayacriticalroleinchilddevelopment.Childrenwhosefamilieshavelowerincomesand/orwhosemothershavefeweryearsofeducationtendtoevidencehigherlevelsofemotionalandbehavioralproblems,whilechildrenwithbettereducatedmotherstendtobemoremotivatedandcompetent(McLoyd1998).Thenumberofsiblingshasshowntobeinverselyrelatedtoabilityandwell-being(SteelmanandMercy1980).Factorsattheschool,suchastheteacher’slevelofexperienceandthenum-

berofyearsintheprofession,arecloselyrelatedtotheperformanceandwell-beingofchildren.Unfortunately,therehasabeenatrendoftheleastexperi-encedteachersworkinginschoolswiththehighestneed(Darling-Hammond1994).Supportingthetransitionfromhometoschooliscriticalinestablishing

Page 6: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

10 B. A. Collins et al.

competencesandsocialandacademicskills(Piantaetal.1999).Studentsde-velopstrongerinterpersonalrelationshipsinschoolswithproportionatelymorestudentsoftheirownethnicity(Johnsonetal.2001).However,highlysegre-gatedschools tendtobe lessresourcedandlowerperforming(Orfieldetal.2004).

3. Presentstudy

TheoverarchinggoalofthisstudyistoinvestigatetherelationshipbetweentheL1andL2competencesofLatinochildrenofimmigrantsinkindergartenandtheirconcurrentemotionalandbehavioralwell-being.Thepresentconceptual-izationofemotionalandbehavioralwell-beingisconsideredwithrespecttofive core areas: interpersonal, intrapersonal, affective, connection to family,andschoolfunctioning.Theseemotionalandbehavioraldimensionsmarkkeyareasofchildhooddevelopment(CoppleandBredekamp2009).Theassocia-tion of language competences and emotional and behavioralwell-being areanalyzedwithinanecologicalmodelconsideringchild,home,andschoolfac-torswithanestablishedortheoreticallinkinthecurrentliteraturetobothchil-drenofimmigrants’languagecompetenceandtheirwell-being.

3.1. Research questions

a) HowareLatinochildrenofimmigrants’L1andL2competencesrelatedtotheiremotionalandbehavioralwell-beingconcurrentlyinkindergarten?

b) Whatarethecontributionsofchild,home,andschoolfactorstoemotionalandbehavioralwell-beingafterconsideringL1andL2competences?

4. Methods

4.1. Participants

Latino dual language children of immigrants ( N=228) with demographiccharacteristicssimilar tothoseofNortheasternurbanimmigrantpopulations(U.S.Census 2000)were recruited from public schools in theBoston,MAarea.AllchildreninthestudywerebornintheUnitedStatesorarrivedpriortoagethreeandwerefirstlanguagespeakersofSpanish.Mothers,families,and/or caregivers communicated solely ormainly in Spanish. Participantsweresequentialbilinguals,withlittleornoexposuretoEnglishpriortoagethree.AtleastoneparentwasbornoutsideofthecontinentalUnitedStates,withmany

Page 7: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Spanish and English competence of immigrant children 11

parentsfromtheDominicanRepublic(52percent)andPuertoRico(21per-cent).Most of the families were living below poverty levels (86 percent).Mothershadmoderatetolowlevelsofeducationwithslightlymorethanhalf(63percent)havinggraduatedfromhighschool.Approximatelyhalf(46per-cent)ofthechildrenwereinsingle-parenthouseholds.Childreninthesampleagedbetween5.2and7.0(meanage6.1)andconsistedofequalnumbersofboysandgirls.

4.2. Procedures

SchoolswithhighpopulationsofLatinostudentswererecruitedtoparticipateinthestudy.Afterdeterminingeligibilityandobtainingwrittenparentalcon-sent, children were assessed in three 45-minute sessions, conducted at theschoolonseparatedaysforeachlanguage,andinmostcaseswithinaweek.Athirdsessionwasdesigned tomeasure thechildren’scognitivecompetencesand includednon-verbal IQtasks.Parent interviewswereconductedmainlyin Spanish at the children’s homes by trained bilingual research assistants.Teachers reportedon theirbackground,experience,andclassroompracticesandchildren’swell-beingatschool.

4.3. Measures

4.3.1. Oral language competences. Specificlinguisticdomainsweremea-suredusing theWoodcock language proficiency battery — Revised:Englishand Spanish forms [WLPB-R] (Woodcock 1991). Four WLPB-R sub-testscores—MemoryforSentences,PictureVocabulary,ListeningComprehen-sion,andVerbalAnalogies—yieldanorallanguageclusterscore(computedfromtheaverageofWscoresfromeachsub-testandnormalizedaccordingtoage).MemoryforSentencesisamixedexpressive-receptivemeasureofsyn-tactic and semantic competence, where the child is asked to repeat words,phrases,andthenwholesentencesthatincreaseinlength.PictureVocabularyisameasureofexpressivevocabularyknowledge that requiresnamingpic-turesof items.ListeningComprehension isareceptivemeasureofsyntacticandsemanticcompetence,wherethechildlistenstoincreasinglycomplexsen-tencesorpassagesandisaskedtoprovidethewordthatismissingattheendofthesentenceorthepassage.VerbalAnalogiesisareceptivemeasureofse-mantics,wherethechildisaskedtoprovideabriefverbalanswertoamissingiteminasentencethatcarriesalogicalrelationship.TheWLPB-Risamea-sureoflanguagecompetencewidelyusedinempiricalstudiesandeducational

Page 8: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

12 B. A. Collins et al.

settings. Ithaspublishedvalidity, reliability,andnorms forages2.0 to90+in English and Spanish; the present study uses standard scores (M=100,SD=15).

4.3.2. Emotional and behavioral well-being. Teachersreportedonfivedi-mensionsofemotionalandbehavioralwell-beingandschoolfunctioningusingtheBehavior and emotional rating scale — 2 [BERS-2](Epstein2004).This52-item,strength-basedscaleassessesemotionalandbehavioralstrengthsonfivesubscales(interpersonal,intrapersonal,andaffectivestrengths,familyin-volvement,andschoolfunctioning).TheInterpersonalStrengthssubscaleas-sessesachild’sabilitytocontrolemotionsorbehaviorinasocialsituation.TheIntrapersonalStrengthssubscaleassessesachild’soutlookonhisorhercom-petenceandaccomplishments.TheAffectiveStrengthssubscaleaddressesachild’sabilitytoexpressfeelingstowardsothersandtoacceptaffectionfromothers.TheFamilyInvolvementsubscaleisameasureofthechild’sconnec-tiontofamilyandfocusesonthechild’ssenseofbelongingandrelationshipwithhisorherfamily.TheSchoolFunctioningsubscalefocusesonachild’scompetenceandengagementinschoolandclassroomtasks.Foreachof thesubscales,astandardizedscaledscoreisusedwithadistributionmeansetat10andthestandarddeviationat3.Studieshavedocumentedstrongpsychometricproperties,content,criteria,andconstructvalidityandlackofracialorethnicbias(Epstein1999).

4.3.3. Child characteristics. Children’s non-verbal cognitive ability and generalintelligencewasmeasuredwiththeUniversal Nonverbal Intelligence Test[UNIT](BrackenandMcCallum1998),whichisadministeredandcom-pletedwithout theuseof language.TheUNIThasbeenwidelyused in thecognitiveassessmentofchildrenwhosetestperformancemaybeaffectedbylanguageimpairmentorsecondlanguagebarriers.Extensivepublishedstudieshavedetermined theUNIT’s reliabilityandvalidity tobeconsistentlyhigh.Thefullscaleintelligencequotientstandardscoreisusedinthepresentanal-yses.Genderwasdummycoded(female=0,male=1).

4.3.4. Home factors. Primary caregivers, for the most part mothers, re-spondedtoseveralquestionnairesregardingthehomecontext.MaternalEdu-cationwasmeasuredbythehighestlevelofschoolingcompletedbymothers(elementaryonly,partialorcompletehighschool,collegedegree,andsoon).Ordinalscoresrangedfrom0(none)to12(MA/PhD).Thechild-per-adultratiowasdeterminedbydividingthenumberofchildrenbythenumberofadultsinthehousehold.All relatedandunrelated individuals living in thehouseholdunderage18werecountedaschildren;thoseolderthan18yearswerecounted

Page 9: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Spanish and English competence of immigrant children 13

asadults.Inthepresentstudy,achildisconsideredtobelivinginpoverty,andscoredoneonthisdichotomousvariable,whenthehouseholdisarecipientofat least one government programwhich ties eligibility to income thresholdlevels set by federal or state poverty guidelines (U.S.Census 2000).TheseprogramsincludetheSpecialSupplementalNutritionProgramforWomen,In-fants,andChildren(WIC),FoodStamps,HeadStart,Section8housingassis-tance,andWelfare.

4.3.5. School factors. Threerelevantcharacteristicsoftheschoolcontextwereevaluatedthroughteacherquestionnaires—teachingexperience,transi-tiontokindergartenpractices,andproportionofSpanishspeakers.Numberofyearsasclassroomteacherswasusedasanindicatorofteachingexperience.TheTransition practices survey( NationalCenterforEarlyDevelopmentandLearning1996),a21-itemcomprehensivesurveyofpracticesrelatedtofacili-tatingthetransitionofchildrenintokindergarten,wasadministeredtoteach-ers.Asummedscoreoftransitionpracticesusedbykindergartenteachersisused in the present analyses. Proportion of Spanish speaking students wascomputedbydividing thenumberofSpanishspeakingchildrenby the totalnumberofchildreninthekindergartenclassroom.

5. Results

Themeansandstandarddeviationsofthescoresforthestudy’svariablesarepresentedinTable1.Children’soral languagecompetenceforbothSpanish(M=67,SD=19)andEnglish(M=70,SD=18)arewellbelowthenormedaveragemeanstandardscoreof100.Belowaverageperformanceforduallan-guagechildrenonmeasuresoflanguagecompetenceistypicalwhencomparedtomonolingualnormsandhasbeenreportedinstudiesofsimilarpopulationsusingthesamemeasures(OllerandEilers2002;Páezetal.2007).Children’snon-verbalIQswereintheaveragerangeforthegeneralpopulation(M=96,SD=13).Theemotional andbehavioral strengthsof childrenwereaverageoraboveaverageinalldimensions(scaledscoremeans11–13SD=3).Con-nections to familywereparticularlyhigh (1SDabovenorm).Kindergartenteachershadawiderangeinyearsofteaching(M=16,SD=8)andvariedintransitionpractices(M=9.6,SD=4.1).TheproportionofSpanishspeakersinclassroomswashigh,withtheaverageclassroomcomprisedofapproximatelythree-quartersSpanishspeakers(M=.73,SD=.31).Therelationshipwithinandacrossthepredictorandoutcomevariableswere

analyzedusingbivariatePearson-correlations(Table1).Ofnote,themainpre-dictors, Spanish and English competence, were not significantly associated

Page 10: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Table1.

Biva

riat

e Pe

arso

n’s c

orre

latio

ns o

f stu

dy v

aria

bles

Mean

SD1

23

45

67

89

1011

1213

14

OralL

anguageCom

petence

1Spanish

67.4

21.1

12

English

69.6

18.7

.13

1

EmotionalandBehavioralW

ell-being

3Interpersonal

Strength

12.3

3.3

.19*

.21**

1

4Intrapersonal

Strength

12.2

3.5

.26**

.36**

.68**

1

5Affective

Strength

12.5

3.5

.15*

.23**

.71**

.82**

1

6Connectionto

Family

12.8

3.0

.11

.30**

.67**

.69**

.66**

1

7School

Functioning

11.3

2.9

.36**

.43**

.61**

.68**

.48**

.53**

1

ChildFactors

8Gender

.5

.5

−.17**

.01

−.05

−.02

.03

−.16

−.02

19

Non-verbalIQ

95.5

12.5

.18**

.48**

.23**

.25**

.14

.19*

.39**

−.01

1

Hom

eFactors

10Maternal

Education

5.1

2.7

.05

.31**

.11

.20**

.14

.17*

.24**

.04

.17*

1

11Poverty

.86

.35

−.06

−.19**

−.17*

−.10

−.06

−.08

−.20**

−.02

−.18**

−.21**

112

Children-Per-

Adults

1.7

1.1

−.12

−.03

.10

.02

.03

.07

.01

.07

−.008

−.10

.25**

1

SchoolFactors

13Teacher

Experience

16.0

8.4

−.22**

−.06

.06

−.04

.07

.13

−.12

−.05

−.09

−.15

.03

.09

1

14Kindergarten

Transition

9.6

4.1

.28**

−.01

−.05

.08

.07

.12

.14

.13

−.01

.09

.05

.01

−.06

1

15Proportion

Spanish

Speakers

.73

.31

.28**

−.20**

.07

.06

.13

−.05

.13

.08

−.09

−.08

.10

.14

.08

.38**

* p≤.05level.**

p≤.01level.

Page 11: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Spanish and English competence of immigrant children 15

withoneanother.Theemotionalandbehavioraloutcomesweresignificantlyassociated across dimensions (rs ranging from .48 to .82, p<.01). Spanishcompetencewassignificantlyassociatedwitheachofthedimensionsofwell-being,exceptconnectiontofamily(rsrangingfrom.15to.36,p<.05).Eng-lishcompetencewassignificantlyassociatedwitheachofthefivedimensionsofwell-being(rsrangingfrom.21to.43,p<.01).The relationship between child, home, and school factors and language

competence and well-being were also analyzed using bivariate Pearson-correlations. Gender was significantly associated with Spanish competence(r=−.17,p<.01),indicatingapositiverelationshipamonggirls.Non-verbalIQwasassociatedwithSpanishcompetence(r=.18,p<.01),Englishcompe-tence(r=.48,p<.01),andeachofthedimensionsofwell-being(rs rangingfrom.19to.39,p<.05)withtheexceptionofaffectivestrengths.Ofthecon-sideredhomefactors,maternaleducationwasassociatedEnglishcompetence(r=.31,p<.01),intrapersonalstrengths(r=.20,p<.01),connectiontofam-ily (r=.17,p<.05), and school functioning (r=.24,p<.01). Povertywasnegatively associatedwithEnglish competence (r=–.19,p<.01), interper-sonalstrengths(r=−.17,p<.05),andschoolfunctioning(r=−.20,p<.01).Of the considered school factors, Spanish competencewas associatedwithteaching experience (r=−.22,p<.01) and kindergarten transition practices(r=.28,p<.01).TheproportionofSpanish speakers in the classroomwasassociatedwithSpanishcompetence(r=.28,p<.01)andnegativelyassoci-atedwithEnglishcompetence(r=–.20,p<.01).SpanishandEnglishcompetenceaccountedforamoderatetolargepropor-

tion(Cohen1988)ofvariance(13–36percent)inallfivedimensionsofwell-being(Table2).Duallanguagecompetencesaccountedfor13percentofthetotalvarianceofinterpersonalstrengths(Spanishcompetence,enteredfirstintheblock,accountedfor6percentandEnglishcompetence,enteredsecond,accountedforanadditional7percent);25percentofthetotalvarianceofintra-personalstrengths(Spanish12percentandEnglish13percent);13percentofthetotalvarianceofaffectivestrengths(Spanish6percentandEnglish8per-cent); 13 percent of the total variance of connection to family (English 10percent);and36percentofthetotalvarianceofschoolfunctioning(Spanish21percentandEnglish15percent).Ineachofourmodels,languagecompetenceswerethestrongestpredictors

ofwell-being.Noneofthesubsequentblocksofchild,home,orschoolvari-ablesaccountedforasignificantchangeinvarianceinanyofthefivedimen-sionsofwell-beingaboveandbeyondthatoflanguagecompetence.Teachingexperiencewastheonlyotherindividualpredictorwithasignificantassocia-tion,andsolelywithconnectiontofamily(β =.19,p<.05).Thebivariatecor-relationwasnotsignificant,however,indicatinganassociationonlyafterhold-ingtheotherpredictorsinthemodelconstant.

Page 12: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

16 B. A. Collins et al.

6. Discussion

Themainpurposeofthisstudywastoinvestigatetheassociationofduallan-guagecompetenceswithemotionalandbehavioralwell-beinginLatinochil-drenofimmigrantsinkindergarten.Thepresentstudydocumentstheassocia-tionsofbothSpanishandEnglishwithchildren’sconcurrentwell-being.Thesecondpurposeofthestudywastoexaminethecontributionofchild,home,andschool factors tochildren’semotionalandbehavioralwell-beingand tothedevelopmentofduallanguagecompetences.Languagecompetencevari-ableswerethestrongestpredictorsinourmodels.Thecontributionsofchild,home,andschoolvariablestowell-beingwasminimaland,inmostcases,non-significantafterconsideringlanguagecompetence.Theeffectof languagecompetencewasmoderatetolargeforeachof the

fivedimensionsofwell-beingconsideredinthestudy.Thesefindingsarepar-ticularlyrelevantconsideringthatpredictingwell-beingduringtheearlyschoolyearsisdifficult.Ithasbeenrecognizedthatthemajorityofvariabilityinchil-dren’searlyschooladjustmentremainsunexplained(Piantaetal.1999;Pianta

Table2. Emotional and behavioral well-being regression models

InterpersonalStrengths IntrapersonalStrengths

Block Block

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

OralLanguageCompetenceSpanish .25** .19* .18* .20* .21* .35** .27** .27** .28** .26**English .26** .17 .15 .17 .37** .32** .29** .30**

ChildFactorsGender .00 −.02 −.01 .03 .03 .02Non-VerbalIQ .18 .16 .16 .09 .09 .09

HomeFactorsMaternalEducation .02 .03 .11 .11Poverty −.13 −.13 .00 .00Child-Per-Adults .15 .14 .08 .07

SchoolFactorsTeacherExperience .08 .03KindergartenTransitionPractices −.14 .00ProportionofSpanishSpeakers .09 .05

R2 .06** .13** .15** .18** .20** .12** .25** .26** .27** .28**ΔR2 .06** .07** .02 .03 .02 .12** .13** .01 .01 .00

Notes: StandardizedBetacoefficients(β)foreachpredictorandoutcome(andsubsequentblocks)arelisted.Totalvariance(R2)andchangeintotalvariance(ΔR2)foreachblockofpredictorsarelisted.* p≤.05level.** p≤.01level.

Page 13: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Spanish and English competence of immigrant children 17

andMcCoy1997).Furthermore,emotionalandbehavioralwell-beingduringtheearlyyearsofschooliscloselyrelatedtomanyshortandlong-termout-comessuchasacademicsuccess,employment,andpersonalresilience(Corn-wallandBawden1992;KameenuiandSimmons1998).OurfindingsstronglysuggestacriticalanduniquerelationshipbetweentheL1andL2competenceofLatinochildrenofimmigrantsandtheirwell-being.Theseextendpreviousfindings that themajorityofchildrenwith languagedeficitsalsoexperienceadjustmentproblems(Beitchmanetal.1996;Toppelbergetal.2002;Toppel-bergandShapiro2000).Inthepresentstudy,children’sduallanguagecompetences(inbothSpanish

andEnglish)werecloselylinkedtotheirinterpersonal,intrapersonal,andaf-fective strengths.This suggests thatchildrenwhoareable toexpress them-selves in both their first and second language and understand the languagebeingusedaroundthemmayfeelmoresecureandself-confident,andthusmaybebetterabletoformstrongerrelationships.BothSpanishandEnglishcompe-tencewereassociatedwithschoolfunctioning.Meaningfulcommunicationisessentialforoptimallearningandmeetingacademicexpectations.Competence

AffectiveStrengths ConnectiontoFamily SchoolFunctioning

Block Block Block

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

.23** .17* .18* .18* .15 .17 .11 .10 .11 .13 .46** .38** .37** .38** .31**.28** .27** .26** .28** .33** .30** .27** .26** .39** .31** .26** .29**

.09 .08 .07 −.07 −.07 −.08 .02 .00 −.03

.04 .03 .05 .06 .05 .07 .16 .14 .15

.04 .05 .05 .06 .09 .08−.03 −.03 −.07 −.07 −.13 −.14 .06 .03 .09 .07 .06 .04

.09 .19* −.01−.03 .16 .04 .15 −.09 .15

.06** .13** .14** .15** .17** .03 .13** .14** .15** .21** .21** .36** .38** .40** .43**

.06** .08** .01 .00 .02 .03 .10** .01 .01 .05 .21** .15** .02 .02 .02

Page 14: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

18 B. A. Collins et al.

inboththefirstandsecondlanguageisassociatedwithcognitive,social,andacademicbenefits(Cummins1977),whichisfurtherevidencedinourstudy.Furthermore,ouranalysesindicatetheuniqueroleofEnglishcompetence

andteachingexperienceinchildren’sconnectiontofamily.ThisissurprisingasallofthechildreninthestudycomefromSpanishspeakingfamilies.Thefindingcouldreflectthatexperiencedteachersmayknowmoreaboutthewell-beingofchildrenwhoareabletocommunicatemoreeffectivelyinEnglish,and that theconnection to familymaybedifficult for teachers to recognizewithouthavingastrongrelationshipwithchildren.ThelargecontributionofEnglish competence to school functioning is not surprising, as previous re-searchhasdemonstratedtheimportanceofdevelopedEnglishlanguageskillsforacademicachievementandschool success (Carhilletal.2008;Ruiz-de-Velascoetal.2000;Suárez-OrozcoandSuárez-Orozco2001).Acloselyrelatedaimofthisstudywastoexaminetheassociationofdual

languagecompetencesandchildren’swell-beingwithinanecologicalframe-work.Theadaptationanddevelopmentofchildrenatschoolisinfluencedbyindividual,family,andinstitutionalcharacteristics(Bronfenbrenner1979).Inourmodels,themostproximalfactorswereconsideredfirstandlessproximalfactorswereexaminedsubsequently.Languagecompetencevariableswerethestrongest and, inmost cases, the only significant concurrent predictors thatexplained the well-being outcomes. By including rival predictor variablesshown tobeassociatedwithchildren’s linguisticandpsychosocialdevelop-ment,wewereabletogetarelativelyunbiasedestimateoftheeffectoflan-guagecompetenceonwell-being.Factorsathome,suchasmaternaleducationlevels,poverty,andtheratioofchildrenperadults,didnotcontributetoourmodelsbeyondthelinguisticpredictors.Furthermore,factorsinschool,suchas teaching experience, kindergarten transition practices, and proportion ofSpanishspeakingchildrenintheclassroom,didnotcontributetoourmodelsbeyond the linguisticpredictors.As languagecompetencewas, for themostpart, theonlysignificantvariableinourinclusiveanalyticalmodels,wecansuggest that dual language competence is a strong contributor to children’swell-being.Itisimportanttonotethatthewell-beingofthechildreninthepresentstudy

isaveragetoaboveaverage.Eachofthemeansforthefivedimensionsofwell-beingareabovethenormativemean(scaledscoreof10).Childreninoursam-ple mean averaged a full standard deviation above the normative mean inconnectiontofamily(scaledscoreof13).Thisfindingishighlyencouragingconsideringthatthemajorityofthechildreninthesamplelivewithmultipleestablishedriskfactors(highlevelsofpoverty,limitedaccesstohealthcare,discrimination,andimmigrationstress).TheoverallemotionalandbehavioralstrengthsofthechildrenmayberelatedtodeeplyheldvaluesinLatinofami-lies,includingastrongsenseoffamily,respect,courtesy,andloyalty.These

Page 15: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Spanish and English competence of immigrant children 19

arecommonlyemphasizedinLatinocultures.Thestrongemotionalandbe-havioralwell-beingofthechildreninthesamplecouldbearesultofLatinoparents’emphasison theirchildrenbeingbien educado (responsibleandre-spectfulmembersofsociety).

7. Conclusions

Thisstudyhasseveralpotentialimplicationsforaquicklygrowingchildpopu-lationthatoftengrowsupinlinguisticisolation,entersschoolatadisadvan-tage,andexperiencesincreasingacademicachievementgapsandmentalhealthdisparitiesovertime.Thereisaneedforsystematicinvestigationofthedevel-opmentofL1andL2competenceandpsychosocialwell-being indual lan-guagechildren.Thisstudyaddsimportantfindingstotheearlyidentificationofwell-beingandlanguagedevelopmentandmayinformeducationalpolicyandteachingstrategiesforchildrenofimmigrantsinthefollowingways.Thisstudysuggeststheimportanceofdevelopingthehomeandschoollan-

guageswhenconsideringthewell-beingofyoungchildrenatschool.Increasedlanguagecompetencecomeswithincreasedabilitytoselfregulate,expressandunderstandfeelings,andcreateknowledgeandawarenessofemotions,form-ing the basis for positive emotional and behavioral adaptation (Raver andKnitzer2002).Thus, childrenwhohave strongL1competences are able todeveloppositiveemotionalandbehavioralskillsathome,whichinturncon-tributetotheclassroom.ChildrenwhohavenotyetfullydevelopednecessaryL1competencesmaycometoschoolunequippedtohandletheincreasedcog-nitive,social,andemotionaldemandandmayhaveahigherrateofemotionalandbehavioralproblemsthanthosewhoarefluentintheirL1.CompetenceintheL2,theschoollanguage,mayaidineffectivecommunication,cooperation,directionfollowing,problemsolving,andthedevelopmentofarangeofaca-demicskills.Ourfindingsindicatethattheaveragechildinoursampleentersschoolwith

averagetoaboveaveragelevelsofwell-being.However,wealsoknowfrompreviousliteraturethatLatinochildrenofimmigrantsareatahighriskforaca-demic struggles and emotional and behavioral problems.As dual languagecompetenceisassociatedwithwell-beinginLatinochildren,educationalandmentalhealthprogramsshouldconsider targeting theacquisitionofcompe-tencesinboththeL1andL2.InthisstudywefoundthatL1andL2competencesaccountforoverone

thirdofthevarianceofschoolfunctioning.Specialistsshouldconsiderbothoftheseareasofchilddevelopmentineducationalandclinicalsettings.Consid-erationofboth theL1andL2 iscritical in theassessmentofdual languagechildren.Fromaneducationalperspective,wecansuggestthatsupportingthe

Page 16: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

20 B. A. Collins et al.

developmentofboththeL1andL2,especiallyduringtheearlyschoolyears,islinguisticallyandpsychologicallyappropriate.Itisimportantthatschoolpro-gramsestablishaconnectionbetweenthehomeandschoolbyincorporatingaspectsofthehomeandcommunityintotheschoolcurriculum.TargetingthedevelopmentofL1andL2competencesatschoolcouldbebeneficialtochil-dren’semotionalandbehavioralwell-being.

Hunter College, Harvard Medical School, Judge Baker Children’s Center, New York University and Boston College

Correspondenceaddresses:[email protected];[email protected];[email protected];[email protected];[email protected]

References

Aragno,Anna&PeterJ.Schlachet.1996.Accessibilityofearlyexperiencethroughthelanguageoforigin:atheoreticalintegration.Psychoanalytic Psychology 13(1).23–34.

Arcia,Emily,LynetteKeyes&JamesJ.Gallagher.1994.Indicatorsofdevelopmentalandfunc-tional status ofMexican-American andPuertoRican children.Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics 15(1).27–33.

Baker, JeanA.,SycarahGrant&LarissaMorlock.2008.The teacher-student relationshipasadevelopmental context for children with internalizing or externalizing behavior problems.School Psychology Quarterly 23(1).3–15.

Beitchman, JosephH.,Nancy J.Cohen,M.MaryKonstantareas&RosemaryTannock (eds.).1996.Language, learning, and behavior disorders: developmental, biological, and clinical per-spectives.NewYork:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Bianchi,SuzanneM.1984.Children’sprogressthroughschool:aresearchnote.Sociology of Edu-cation 57(3).184 –192.

Bracken,BruceA.&SteveR.McCallum.1998.Universal nonverbal intelligence test.Itasca,IL:Riverside.

Bronfenbrenner,Urie.1979.The ecology of human development: experiments by nature and de-sign.Cambridge,MA:HarvardUniversityPress.

Cantwell,DennisP.&LorianBaker.1991.Psychiatric and developmental disorders in children with communication disorders.Washington,DC:AmericanPsychiatricPress.

Carhill,Avary,Carola Suárez-Orozco&MarielaM. Páez. 2008. ExplainingEnglish languageproficiency among adolescent immigrant students.American Educational Research Journal 45(4).1155–1179.

Chapman,Mimi&KristaM.Perreira.2005.Thewell-beingofimmigrantLatinoyouth:aframe-work to informpractice.Families in Society: The Journal of Contemporary Social Services 86(1).104 –110.

Cohen,Jacob.1988.Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.Hillsdale,NJ:Law-renceErlbaum.

Copple,Carol&SueBredekamp.2009.Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs serving children from birth through age 8.Washington,DC:NationalAssociationfortheEducationofYoungChildren( NAEYC).

Page 17: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Spanish and English competence of immigrant children 21

Cornwall,Anne&HarryN.Bawden.1992.Readingdisabilitiesandaggression:acriticalreview.Journal of Learning Disabilities 25(5).281–288.

Crittenden,PatriciaM.1996.Languageandpsychopathology:anattachmentperspective.InJo-sephH.Beitchman,NancyJ.Cohen,MaryM.Konstanareas&RosemaryTannock(eds.),Lan-guage, learning, and behavior disorders: developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives,59–77.NewYork:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Cummins,James.1977.Cognitivefactorsassociatedwiththeattainmentofintermediatelevelsofbilingualskills.The Modern Language Journal 61(1/2).3–12.

Cummins,James.1979.Cognitive/academiclanguageproficiency,linguisticinterdependence,theoptimumagequestionandsomeothermatters.Working Papers on Bilingualism Toronto 19.197–202.

Darling-Hammond,Linda.1994.Professional development schools: schools for developing a pro-fession.NewYork:TeachersCollegePress.

Epstein,MichaelH.1999.Thedevelopmentandvalidationofascaletoassesstheemotionalandbehavioralstrengthsofchildrenandadolescents.Remedial and Special Education 20(5).258–262.

Epstein,MichaelH.2004.Behavioral and emotional rating scale (2ndedn.) (BERS-2).Austin,TX:Pro-Ed.

Farmer,Marion.1997.Exploringthelinksbetweencommunicationskillsandsocialcompetence.Educational & Child Psychology 14(3).38– 44.

Fishman,JoshuaA.1991.Reversing language shift: theoretical and empirical foundations of as-sistance to threatened languages.Clevedon&Bristol,PA:MultilingualMatters.

Flanagan,Owen.1996.Ethicsnaturalized:ethicsashumanecology.InOwenFlanagan(ed.),Self expressions: mind, morals, and the meaning of life, 117–141.NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.

Foster,RoseMariePerez.1996.Thebilingualself:duetintwovoices.Psychoanalytic Dialogues 6(1).99–121.

Gándara,Patricia,KatherineLarson,HughMehan&RussellRumberger.1998.Capturing Latino students in the academic pipeline.Berkeley,CA:Chicano/LatinoPolicyProjectReport.

GarciaColl, Cynthia,Gontran Lamberty, Renee Jenkins,Harriet PipesMcAdoo,KeithCrnic,BarbaraHannaWasik&HeidieVazquezGarcia.1996.Anintegrativemodelforthestudyofdevelopmentalcompetenciesinminoritychildren.Child Development 67(5).1891–1914.

Hernandez,DonaldJ.,NancyA.Denton&SuzanneE.Macartney.2007.Childreninimmigrantfamilies—TheU.S.and50states:nationalorigins,language,andearlyeducation.Child Trends and Center for Social and Demographic Analysis, University at Albany, SUNY, Research Brief Series 2007–11 (1–9).

Johnson,KirkpatrickMonica,RobertCrosnoe&GlenH.Elder,Jr.2001.Students’attachmentandacademicengagement:theroleofraceandethnicity.Sociology of Education 74(4).318–340.

Kameenui,EdwardJ.&DeborahC.Simmons.1998.What reading research tells us about chil-dren with diverse learning needs: bases and basics.Mahwah,NJ:LawrenceErlbaum.

Kao,Grace&MartaTienda.1998.Educationalaspirationsofminorityyouth.American Journal of Education 106(3).349–384.

Masten,AnnS.1994.Resilienceinindividualdevelopment:successfuladaptationdespiteriskandadversity.InMargaretC.Wang&EdmundW.Gordon(eds.),Educational resilience in inner-city America: challenges and prospects,3–25.Hillsdale,NJ:LawrenceErlbaum.

Mather,Mark.2009.Childreninimmigrantfamilieschartnewpath.Population Reference Bu-reau, Reports on America.

McLoyd,Vonnie.1998.Socioeconomicdisadvantageandchilddevelopment.American Psycholo-gist 53(2).185–204.

Page 18: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

22 B. A. Collins et al.

National Center for Early Development and Learning. 1996. The transition practices survey.ChapelHill,NC:UniversityofNorthCarolina.

O’Connor,Erin,EricDearing&BrianA.Collins.(2011).Teacher-childrelationshiptrajectories:predictorsofbehaviorproblemtrajectoriesandmediatorsofchildandfamilyfactors.American Educational Research Journal48(1).120–162.

Ogbu,JohnU.1987.Variabilityinminorityschoolperformance:aprobleminsearchofanexpla-nation.Anthropology & Education Quarterly 18(3).312–334.

Oller,D.Kimbrough&RebeccaE.Eilers(eds.)2002.Language and literacy in bilingual chil-dren: child language and child development.NewYork:MultilingualMatters.

Orfield,Gary,DanielLosen,JohannaWald&ChristopherSwanson.2004.Losing our future: how minority youth are being left behind by the graduation rate crisis.Cambridge,MA:TheCivilRightsProjectatHarvardUniversity;TheUrbanInstitute;AdvocatesforChildrenofNewYork;TheCivilSocietyInstitute.

Páez,MarielaM.,PattonO.Tabors&LisaM.López.2007.Duallanguageandliteracydevelop-mentofSpanish-speakingpreschoolchildren.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 28(2).85–102.

Pianta,RobertC.&SandraJ.McCoy.1997.Thefirstdayofschool:thepredictivevalidityofearlyschoolscreening.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 18(1).1–22.

Pianta,RobertC.,MarthaJ.Cox,LorraineTaylor&DianeEarly.1999.Kindergartenteachers’practicesrelatedtothetransitiontoschool:resultsofanationalsurvey.The Elementary School Journal 100(1).71–86.

Portes,Alejandro&RubenG.Rumbaut.2001.Legacies: the story of the immigrant second gen-eration.Berkeley,CA:UniversityofCaliforniaPress.

Portes,Alejandro&RichardSchauffler.1994.Languageandthesecondgeneration:Bilingualismyesterdayandtoday.International Migration Review 28(4).640 – 661.

Pumariega,Andres.2009.Acculturationandmentalhealthinadolescents.PaperpresentedattheAmaricanAcademyofChildandAdolescentPsychiatry56thAnnualMeeting,Honolulu,HI,27October–1November.

Raver,CybeleC.&JaneKnitzer.2002.Readytoenter:whatresearchtellspolicymakersaboutstrategies topromotesocialandemotional school readinessamong three-and four-year-olds.ColumbiaUniversity,NationalCenterforChildreninPoverty,PolicyPaper3.

Rivera, Fernando I., Peter J. Guarnaccia,NorahMulvaney-Day, JuliaY. Lin,MariaTorres&MargaritaAlegria.2008.FamilycohesionanditsrelationshiptopsychologicaldistressamongLatinogroups.Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 30(3).357–378.

Ruehlman,LindaS.,RichardI.Lanyon&PaulKaroly.1999.DevelopmentandvalidationoftheMultidimensionalHealthProfile.Part I:Psychosocial functioning.Psychological Assessment 11(2).166 –176.

Ruiz-de-Velasco,J.,MichaelE.Fix&BeatrizChuClewell.2000.Overlooked and underserved: immigrant students in U.S. secondary schools.Washington,DC:UrbanInstitutePress.

Silver,RebeccaB.,JeffreyR.Measelle,JeffreyM.Armstrong&MarilynJ.Essex.2005.Trajec-toriesofclassroomexternalizingbehavior:contributionsofchildcharacteristics,familycharac-teristics,andtheteacher-childrelationshipduringtheschooltransition.Journal of School Psy-chology 43(1).39– 60.

Snow,CatherineE.1991.Thetheoreticalbasisforrelationshipsbetweenlanguageandliteracydevelopment.Journal of Research in Childhood Education 6(1).5–10.

Steelman,LalaCarr&JamesA.Mercy.1980.Unconfounding theconfluencemodel:a testofsibshipsizeandbirth-ordereffectsonintelligence.American Sociological Review 45(4).571–582.

Steinberg,Laurence,SanfordM.Dornbusch&B.BradfordBrown.1992.Ethnicdifferencesinadolescentachievement:anecologicalperspective.American Psychologist 47(6).723–729.

Page 19: Cross-sectional associations of Spanish and English ...

Spanish and English competence of immigrant children 23

Steinberg,Laurence,SanfordM.Dornbusch&B.BradfordBrown.1993.Ethnicdifferencesinadolescentachievement:anecologicalperspective.InMargaretE.HertzigandEllenA.Farber(eds.), Annual progress in child psychiatry and child development, 528–543. New York:Routledge.

Suárez-Orozco,Carola.2002.Commentary. InMarceloM.Suárez-Orozco&MarielaM.Páez(eds.),Latinos: remaking America,302–305.London:UniversityofCaliforniaPress.

Suárez-Orozco,Carola&MarceloM.Suárez-Orozco.2001.Children of immigration.Cambridge,MA:HarvardUniversityPress.

Toppelberg,ClaudioO.&TheodoreShapiro.2000.Languagedisorders:a10-yearresearchupdatereview.Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 39(2).143–152.

Toppelberg,ClaudioO.,LauraMedrano,LisaPeñaMorgens&AlfonsoNieto-Castañon.2002.Bilingual children referred for psychiatric services: associations of language disorders, lan-guageskills,andpsychopathology.Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 41(6).712–722.

Tseng,Vivian&AndrewJ.Fuligni.2000.Parent-adolescentlanguageuseandrelationshipsamongimmigrantfamilieswithEastAsian,Filipino,andLatinAmericanbackgrounds.Journal of Mar-riage and the Family 62(2).465– 476.

U.S.Census.2000.Census of population and housing.U.S.SurgeonGeneral.1999.Mental health: a report of the Surgeon General.White,JenniferL.,TerrieE.Moffitt&PhilA.Silva.1989.Aprospectivereplicationoftheprotec-tiveeffectsofIQinsubjectsathighriskforjuveniledelinquency.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 57(6).719–724.

WongFillmore,Lily.1991.Whenlearningasecondlanguagemeanslosingthefirst.Early Child-hood Research Quarterly 6(3).323–346.

Woodcock,RichardW. 1991.Woodcock language proficiency battery — Revised. English and Spanish forms. Examiner’s manual.Chicago:Riverside.