Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

256

Transcript of Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 1/255

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 2/255

Classroom   Discourse

and the  Space  of  Learning

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 3/255

  his page intentionally left blank

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 4/255

Classroom   Discourse

and  the  Space  of L earning

Ference

 Marton,  Goteborg Universi ty

Am y B. M.  Tsui,

  The

 Un iversity

  of

  Ho n g K o n g

With

Pakey  P. M .

 Chik

PoYukKo

M un  Ling 33Lo

Ida

  A . C. Mok

Dorothy  F . P. Ng

M i n g   F ai Pang

Wing Yan  Pong

Ulla Runesson

IEA

1

  •

  LAWRENCE ERLBAUM ASSOCIATES, PUBLISHERS

2004  Mahwah, New Jersey London

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 5/255

C o p y r ig h t  ©

 2004

 b y L awren ce E r lbau m A s s oc ia t es , I n c .

A ll

  r i g h t s

 reserved.

  No part o f th i s book m ay be rep roduced in

any form, by pho tostat , micro form , ret r ieval sys tem, o r any othe r

m eans ,

 wi th ou t p r io r wr i t t en permiss ion o f the

  publ i sher .

Lawren ce E r lbau m As s o c i a te s . I n c . , P u b l i s h e r s

10

 In dus t r i a l Avenue

M a h w a h ,

  N ew

  Jersey

  07430

Cover des ign by John Leung

Library

  of Congress

  C a t a l o g i n g - i n - P u b l i c a t io n D a t a

Marlon , Ference .

Class room d iscourse and the

  space

  o f l ea rn ing / Ference Mar ton ,

A m y B . M .

  Ts u i  w i t h  Pakey

  P . M.

  C h i k

  . . . [et

 al . ] .

p. cm.

I nc l ude s

  b ib l i o g rap h i ca l r e f e ren ces

  a nd

 i n d ex .

I S B N

  0-8058-4008-7  (c lo th  :

 alk . paper)

I S B N

  0-8058-4009-5

  ( pbk . : a lk . paper )

1 .

  C o m m u n i c a t i o n

  in

  ed u ca t i o n .

  2.

  L e a r n i n g .

  3.

  Class room

envi ronment .

  I . Ts u i , Am y .  II . Ti t l e .

L B 1 0 3 3 . 5 . M 2 6 5

  2003

371.102'2^c21

  2003047019

C I P

Books publ ished

 by

 L awrence Erlb aum Associates

  are

 printed

 on

 acid-

free paper, and their bindings are chosen for strength and   durabili ty.

Pr in ted in the Un i ted S ta tes o f A m er ica

1 0  9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 6/255

C o n t e n t s

Contributors  v ii

P re f a c e  ix

Part

  I O n

  Learning

 and

  Language

1 The

 Space

 of

 Learning

  3

Ference M arlon ,  Ulla  Runesson, and Amy B. M.  Tsui

Part  II O n  Learning

2

  Vanation

 and the Secret of the

 Virtuoso

  43

Po Yuk Ko and  Ference Marlon

3

  Discernment

 and the

 Question, "What

 Can Be

 Learned?"

  63

Ulla  Runesson and Ida A. C. Mok

4  Simultaneity and the Enacted Object of Learning  89

Pakey  P. M.  Chik  and Mun  Ling  Lo

Part

  III O n  Language

5

  Questions

 and the

 Space

 of

 Learning

  113

Amy B . M.

  Tsui,  Ference Marlon ,

  Ida A. C. Mok,

and  Dorothy  F. P. Ng

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 7/255

V I

  CONTENTS

6 The Semant ic E nr ichm ent of the Space  of Learning  1 39

A m y B . M .

  Tsui

7  The

 Shared Space

 of

 L earning

  1 65

Amy B. M.

  Tsui

Part  IV O n  Improving  Learning

8  Toward a Pedagogy  of Learning  1 89

M un   Ling

 Lo ,  Ference Marlon , Ming  F ai Pang ,

and

  Wing

  Y an

 Pong

Epilogue

  227

References  233

Au thor Index  239

Subject  Index  241

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 8/255

Contr ibutors

Ference

 M arton,  Professor of Educat ion, Facul ty of Educat ion, Goteborg

Universi ty,

  Sweden ,

  and

  Honorary Professor , Facul ty

  o f

  Educa t i on ,

 T he

U nivers ity of Hon g K ong.

Am y B . M . Tsui , Chair Professor , Facul ty of Ed ucat ion, The Un ivers i ty of

Hong Kong .

Pakey P . M . C hik , S tuden t, Degree

 of

 M as ter

 o f

 Phi losophy, Facu l ty

 of Ed-

ucat ion ,

 The U nivers ity of Hong Kong.

Po Yuk Ko, Lec turer , Department

  of

 C urr icu lum

 and

 Ins t ruct ion,

 The

 H o n g

Kong Ins t i tu te o f Educat ion .

M u n L i n g L o ,  Head, Centre

  for

 Development

  of

  School Partnership

  and

Field

 Ex perience, The Hon g Kong Ins t itu te of Ed uca t ion.

Ida A. C.

 Mok, A ssis tant Professor , Fac ul ty

 o f

 E duca t ion ,

 Th e

 Un ivers i ty

of Hong Kong .

D or ot h y F. P. N g, Teaching Fellow, Fa culty of

 Educat ion ,

 The U nivers ity of

Hong Kong .

M ing Fa i Pang, A ssistant Professor , Fa cul ty of Educ at ion, The Un ivers ity

of Hong Kong.

W in g Y a n P on g ,

  Principal , Hong Kong Management Associat ion, David

Li

 K w o k

 Po

  Col lege.

Ulla Runesson,  Postdoctoral Fellow, Senior Lecturer, Faculty

  of

 E du c a -

t ion,

  Goteborg Universi ty, Sweden.

V I I

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 9/255

  his page intentionally left blank

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 10/255

Preface

This book

 is

 abou t learning

  in

 schools ,

  and the

 role

 of

 language

 in

 learning.

W e have tr ied to capture i ts tw o m ain ideas in the t i t le . Co ntained w ithin the

first  idea is the

 p rem ise

 that wh atever you are trying to learn, there are cer-

tain

 necessary condi t ions for succeeding. Al thou gh you cannot be sure tha t

learning w il l take place when those co ndit ions are m et , you can be sure that

no

 learning w il l take place

 if

 they

 are

 not .

 T he

 l imits

 o f

 w h a t

  is

 possible

  to

learn, w e call " the space o f learning." T he second premise  is tha t langu age

p l a y s a cen tra l role in learning : that i t does not m erely  convey m eaning, it

also creates

 m e a n in g .

 A n

 understanding

 of how the

 space

 of

 learning

 i s

 l in-

gui s t i ca l ly co nst i tute d in the c lassroom is best achieved through invest igat-

ing "classroom   discourse ," wh ich

  is

 w h a t

 w e a im to do

 here .

A teach er can never ensure that the intended learning  wi l l ac tual ly take

p lace , but a teacher should try to ensure that i t

 i s

 possible for the studen ts to

learn

  w ha t is inte nd ed. That is, the teacher sho uld ensure that the space of

learning

  a l lows

  for the

  intended learning

  to

  take place .

  F or

  every educa-

t ional

  aim, for every single thing that students are expected to learn, there

are

  specif ic

  c ondit ions necessary  fo r that learning. In our view,  f inding  ou t

w h a t

 these condition s are, and br inging them about , should be the  teacher 's

pr imary p rofess iona l task .

 A

 p rerequisi te

  fo r

 f inding

 ou t

 these condit ions

 is

the

 rea l iza tion tha t

 one's

 ow n w ay o f teaching is not the only w ay. Such a re-

al izat ion can only be achieved by co nfron ting different  way s of teaching the

same th ing , by exam ining how the different  w ays are actual ly played out in

the c lassroom and by com paring w hat is intended with wh at is enac ted. A nd

invest igat ing

  the discourse in which the teacher and the s tudents are en-

gaged

 in the

 classroom

  is an

 essential par t

 of

 this endeavor.

  It is,

 there fore ,

fundamenta l ly  important that teachers—an d student teachers , for that mat-

ter— are given opportuni t ies

 to

 observe different  teachers teaching

 the

 same

ix

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 11/255

X

  P R E F A C E

th ing ,

 and to analyze and ref lec t on how the c lass room d iscourse in w hic h

they

  a re

  engaged w i th

  th e

  s tuden t s m ax im izes

 o r

 m in im izes oppor tun it ie s

for

  learning.

This book

  is organized in

  four

  par ts . In Par t I , "On Learning and Lan-

guage,"  w e

  p resen t

  the

  theo r e t i ca l backg r ound , namely

  th e

  theor ies tha t

wh atever you are t rying to learn, there are cer tain necessary co ndi t io ns for

succ eeding , and that langu ag e play s a central role in lea rnin g. Par t II , "On

Learning,"

 com prises

 three chap ters , each

 o f

 w hich e labora tes

 on one of the

three  core

 e l emen t s

 of our

 view

 o f

 learn ing : va r ia t ion , d i scernm ent ,

 and si-

m ultanei ty . Part I I I , "O n  Language," comprises three chapters a s w e l l , each

of

 which deals wi th

 one of the

 three aspects

 of the

 role

 o f

 la n g u a g e

 in

 class-

room

 learn ing :

 th e

 role

 of

 quest ions

 in

 c ons t i t u t ing

 the

 space

 o f

 l ea r n ing ,

 the

semant ic n a ture o f the space of learn ing , and the jo i n t con s t i tu t io n of  tha t

space. In Par t IV, "On Improving Learning," we give three examples of

teachers

 w orking

  together a nd

 using

 the

 very the oretical tools presented

  in

the  previous chapters .

Three

 fea tures o f th i s book d is t inguish it f rom s im i lar books about learn-

ing in

 schools. First ,

 it is a

 book about both theory

  an d

 p rac t ice .

 I t

 con ta in s

 a

detai led ex pl icat ion of the theory of learnin g that m otivated the ana lyse s of

classroom teach ing in the res t of the book. I t presents deta i led an aly ses of

classroom teach ing that were dr iven by classroom discourse data in a nu m -

ber of authe nt ic cases  of learn ing  in school,  and w h i c h w i l l be of p r ac t i ca l

relevance  to  teachers .

Second, th is book is m ore cul tura l ly s i tuated than most othe r books about

learning

 in

 schools . M ost

 o f the

 studie s reported

  in

 th is book have been ca r-

ried out in

 Hong Kong .

 In

 every exa m ple,

 it is

 c lear ly demons t ra ted

 how the

specif ic  langu age , cu l tu re , and pedagogy m olds wha t i s happe ning in the

classroom . At the same t im e, however, w e w ou ld l ike to cla im that it i s a lso

possible

  to

 gen eral ize

 f rom the

 c u l tu r a l ly

 specif ic

  examples

 and

 a r gumen t s

presented

  in

  this

 book.  W e  argue

  that whatever skil ls , whatever ways

  of

th ink ing  th e  s tudents  a re  expected  to  develop, there  a re  necessary condi-

t ions for the developm ent of these sk i l ls . These con d i t ions are

 specif ic

  to ev-

ery s pe cific sk il l , to every

  specif ic

  way of th ink ing , and they m us t be met

regardless  o f where the learn ing is tak ing p lace , a nd rega rdless o f wha t o ther

condi tions there m ight be .

Third , th is boo k— just l ike other books— is good fo r cer tain th in gs and

not so

 good

  for

 other th ing s .

 A s

  imp lied earlier ,

  if you

 w a n t

 to f ind ou t how

cer tain specif ic capa bi l it ies (such as using elem entary ar i thme tic in

  flexible

way s , d i s t ingu ish ing be tween

  different

  tones in Can tonese, seeing bodies in

mot ion

  in

  accordance wi th

  a

  Newtonian f ramework , r ea l iz ing

 w hy

  green

plants

 are

 essent ial

  fo r l i fe on

 Ear th)

 can be

 best developed,

  you

 w i l l p roba-

bly f ind

 th is

 book

 use fu l .  However,

  i f you

 w a n t

 to f ind out

 about gen eral iza-

t ions that

  are

  universal ,

  if

  they exist

  a t

  al l , such

  as how

  people become

creat ive, wh at is the best ar range m ent for learning in gen eral , the exact nu m -

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 12/255

P R E F A C E  X I

ber of

  intel l igences

  or

  learning styles

  in

  humans ,

  to

  what extent those

intel l igences

 or

 learning styles

 are

 biologically, eco nom ically, cultural ly,

 or

l inguist ical ly

 determined,  and so on, this

 book

 is

 probably

 not a very good

source

 o f

 inspirat ion.

 This

 book,

 w e

 w ish

 to

 emp hasize,

  is not

 abou t univer-

sal general izat ions about learning,

  nor is it

 about

  the

  development

  of

  spe-

cific

 c apa bil it ies per se.

 This

 book is abou t the

 necessary condi t ions

  for the

development of any   specific  capability.

W e

 w ou ld l ike

 to

 sugg est that this book

 is

 best used

  in field

 practice

  for

teacher educat ion  and in-service training for teachers . W e feel  that it w i ll be

of m ost pract ical use when read in conjunct ion wi th arrangem ents w hereby

teachers and

 studen t teachers have o ppo rtunities

  to

 observe  different  teach-

ers teaching

  th e

 same topic,

 an d to

 investigate

 teacher-student

 discourse

 in

l ight

 o f the  opportunit ies  fo r  learning that  are  afforded.

Th is book can, however, also

 be

 juxtapo sed w ith other theoretical accou nts

of classroom learning. Dealt w ith

 in

 this way,

 it can be

 used,

 w e

 believe,

 in ad-

vanced

 sem inars in teacher edu cation, and for courses at Master's level in ed-

ucat ional

  studies.

A C K N O W L E D G M E N T S

T he

 research carr ied

 o ut in

 Hong Kong

 w as

 m ade poss ib le throu gh

 a

 grant

awarded to Professor P aul M orris ( then Chair Professor of Edu cat ion, The

Universi ty o f

 Hong Kong ,

 now

 President

  of the

 Hong Kong In s t i tu te

 of Ed-

uca t ion)  f rom the Standing Commit tee on Language Educat ion and Re-

search  in Hong Kong (SC OL A R). W e w ould l ike to express our s incere

grat i tude  to

 Professor M orris ,

  a

 bri l l iant scho lar

 and a

 m ost generous col-

l eague .

1

Ference M arton and Ul la Run esson w ould like to acknow ledge the sup-

port

  of The

  Tercentennary Foundat ion

  of the

  Bank

  of

  Sweden ,

  and the

Swedish  Re search C ounc i l, w i thout w hich thei r work w ould not have been

poss ib le .

The authors of th is volum e wo uld also l ike to thank the form er D epart-

m e n t

 of C urr icu lum S tudies (now part of the Facu l ty of E ducat ion) at The

U niversity of Hong Kong, fo r providing  the  Departmental Research Fund,

wh ich

  a l lowed

  the

  authors

  to pay for

  expenses incurred

  in

  collaborating

with

  Goteborg Universi ty

  and in

  securing l inguist ic edit ing assistance.

The research team included Dr. Tam m y Kw an, whose contr ibut ion to th is

book is indicated by expl ici t references to her work. V ikki W eston has car-

ried

  out a w ond e rf u l  job of

  editing

  th e

 l anguage

 o f the

 m anuscr ip t. An nie

Chow, M iranda Cheu ng, and W inky M ok at The U nivers ity of Hong K ong,

as wel l as Lisbetth Soderberg and Barbro Stromberg at Goteborg  Univer-

sity,

 have,

 in different  but

 essential w ays, helped

 us to put the

 m anuscr ip t

 in

shape .

 W e thank them al l .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 13/255

X ll

  P R E F A C E

Tw o reviewers , C our tney C azden  of  Harvard Graduate Schools  and

Gaalen Er ickson of the U nivers ity of  Br i t ish Columbia, have provided ex-

t r emely usefu l

 and

 critical

 comments

 that have sharpened

  the

  f o c u s

 of the

book. W e are very

 gra teful

  to them . Fina l ly , th i s book w ould never have ma-

ter ial ized without

 the

  support

  o f

 N aom i S i lverman, Sen ior Ed i to r ,

 and

 Lori

Haw ver, A ssistant E di tor ,

 o f

 Law rence E r lbaum A ssocia tes , bo th

 o f

 w h o m

have been  most encouraging, accommodat ing,  and  efficient . To them we

owe our  deepest g rat i tude.

-—

Ference

  M a r l o n

— A m y

  B . M .

  Tsui

E N D N O T E

'Part of the

  findings  from  this

 project have

 been repo rted

 previously in F.

 M arton

& P.

  Morris. (Eds.). (2002).

  What

  Mat ters:  Discover ing

  Crit ical

  C o n d i t i o n s

 o f

Classroom

  Learn ing . Goteborg , Sweden :

 Acta

  Universitat is G othoburgensis .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 14/255

O n   Learn ing  and  Language

 

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 15/255

  his page intentionally left blank

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 16/255

1

The

  Space

  of  Learning

Ference Marton

U l l a

  Runesson

A m y B . M .   T s u i

Schoo l is an inst i tut ion with w hich a l l c i t izens in the industr ia l w orld have

extensive fam il iar i ty, and one that

  f requent ly

  a t t racts considerable public

and po li t ica l a t tent ion. T he discussions abo ut school can be heated an d the

opinions polemic :  "W e should have less who le-class teaching," " W e should

have more pro jec t  work,"  "W e  should have more  peer  learning,"  "W e

shou ld have more problem -based

  learning,"

 "By the yea r 2006, a t least 20%

of a l l learning in our school should be information technology ( IT) sup-

ported,"  "Students should have more  homework ,"  "Students should have

less homew ork,"  "W e should do away w i th age group ing,"  "W e shou ld re in-

troduce  age  grouping,"  "W e  should have streaming,"  and so on.

A ll

 these opinions about w hat should

 be

 done assume,

  o f

 course, th at

 do-

ing this or doing that is bet ter than doing som ething e lse . B ut i f w e ask the

ques t ion , "Better for w hat?" the an sw er is l ikely to be, "Better for learning,

of course." "But

  for the

 learning

 o f

 w h a t?" "For

  th e

 learning

 of

  everything?"

These  are the ques t ions tha t must be  addressed.

Th e point is that i t is highly u nl ikely that there is any one pa r t icular way

of arrang ing for learning that is condu cive to all kinds of learning. In order

to find

 effective  w a y s

 o f

 arranging

  fo r

 learning, research ers need

  to

 first

  ad-

dress  what  it is that should be learned in each case, and

  find

  the

  different

con dit ions that are conducive to different  kinds of learning.

It  is only when we have a

  fair

  understanding of what learners are ex-

pe cted to learn in particu lar situations, w hat they ac tually learn in those si tu-

at ions, and w hy they learn som ething in one si tuat ion but not in another, that

pedagogy becom es a reasonably ra t ional se t of hum an a ct ivi ties . I t is the

aim

  of

 this book

  to

  provide such

 an

 u nders tanding.

3

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 17/255

M A R I O N ,

  R L ' N E S S O N , T S U I

W h e n

 people argue

 fo r a

 p ar t icu lar

 way o f

 ar ranging

 for

 l ea r n ing ,

 or for a

par t icular teaching method, such   as wor k ing  in groups  or the use of p e d a -

gogica l d rama, they should make i t c lear wh at the par t icu lar a r rang em ent , o r

the par t icular m ethod,  is good  for and why . Ped ag og ica l ac ts shou ld take as

their point

  o f

 departure

  the

  cap ab i l i t i es they

  are

  supposed

  to

 co n t r ibu te

 to

develop ing . The po in t o f sch ool ing i s no t tha t s tude nts shou ld or sho uld no t

be

  grouped together

  in

 ce r ta in ways un d er ce r ta in con d i t ions— such

 as b e-

ing

 divided

  up

 according

  to

 age, abil i ty level,

 or

 gender . N ei ther

 is the

 p o i n t

that teachers should do cer tain th in gs in cer ta in w ays, or tha t ce r t a in con ten t

should

 be

 covered.

  T he

 po in t

  is

 tha t

 the

  s tudents should develop cer ta in

 ca-

pabi l i t ies . '

T H E

  O B J E C T

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

Learn ing

  is

 a lways

  th e

 acq u i r ed knowled ge

  o f

  s o m e t h in g .

 A n d w e

 shou ld

a lways

 keep  in

 mind w ha t t ha t " someth ing"

  is ,  tha t  is , w e

  s h o u l d

 b e

 c l ea r

about

  the

 object

  o f

  learn ing .

In th is

 book ,

 the object of learning is a

 capab il ity ,

  and any capa b i l i ty has a

general  and a sp ecif ic aspect. T he genera l aspect has to do w i th th e na tu r e of

the  capabili ty, such  as  rem em ber ing , d i scern ing , in terpre t ing , g rasp ing , or

viewing, that

 is, the

 acts

 o f

 learn ing

 ea rned

 o u t .

 T he specif ic

 aspect

 h as to do

with  the th ing o r sub jec t o n w hich these ac t s are car r ied o ut , such a s fo rm u-

las , engineer ing problems, s im ultan eo us equ at io ns . W orld W ar I I . or Fran z

Kafk a ' s

  l i terary heritage.

  In

  other words,

  the

 ge ne r a l

  aspect

  r efer s

  to

 ac t s

(the indirect  object of learn ing) , w hereas the specific  aspect  r efer s to w ha t i s

acted upon ( the

 direct

  object

 o f

 learn ing) .

 T he

 learner s '  focus

  is

 no r ma l ly

 on

what they

  a re

  trying

  to

  learn ( the d irect object

  o f

  l ea r n ing ) , w her eas

  the

teacher 's focus should

  be on

 both;

  no t

  on ly

 on

  tha t w h i ch

  the

  learners

  are

trying

  to

 learn,

 bu t

 also

 on the way in

 w h i c h

 the

 learners

 are

 t ry ing

 to

 m a s te r

what they

  a re

 trying

  to

  l eam .

  W e

 m ight assum e therefore , tha t teachers

 are

trying   to work toward  an object

  of  l e ar n i ng .

 This o b jec t may be m ore or less

conscious for the teacher and i t m ay be mo re or less elaborated. Bu t , w ha t-

ever

  the

 c i r cumstances , w hat teachers

  are

 striving

  for is the in tended

  ob jec t

of learning,  an object o f the teacher 's awareness , tha t m igh t chang e dynam i-

cally during   the  course  o f  learn ing .

 Th i s

  is the  ob jec t  of  learn ing  as  seen

from   the teacher's

 perspec tive,

  and as

 such

 is

 dep ic ted

 i n

 th is book

  as

 be ing

evidenced

  by

 wha t

  the

  teacher does

  and

  says .

W h a t  is of imp or tance  for the  s tudents , how ever , is not so m u c h how the

t eache j  intends

  the

  object

  o f

  learning

  to

  come

  to the  fore,  but how the

teacher structures the conditions of learn ing so tha t i t is po ssible for the ob-

j e c t o f

 learning

  to

 come

  to the

 fore

 of the

  learners ' awareness . What

  the

 s tu-

dents

 encounter  is the  enacted  object  o f  learn ing ,  and it d e f ines wh a t  it is

possible  to leam in the

  actual sett ing,  from

  the

 po in t

  of

 view

 of the  specif ic

object  o f  learning.

 There

  are  obv ious ly cer ta in necessary cond i t ions fo r

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 18/255

1 .  T H E

  S P A C E

  O F L E A R N I N G

learning  one

  thing

  or

  another.

  T he

  enacted object

  of

  learning

  is the re-

searc her 's descript ion of w hether, to w hat ex tent , and in w hat form s the nec -

essary condit ions of a part icular object of learning appear in a certain

set t ing.

 The enacted objec t of learning is described

  from

 the point of view of

a  certain

 research

  interest and a part icular theoretical perspective.

Wh a t is of decisive  importance for the students,  is what actually

 conies

 to

the  fore  of their at tention, that is , w hat asp ects of the si tuation they discern

and  focus o n. In the best case, they focus o n the cntical aspects of the object

of learn ing, and by doing so they learn w ha t the teacher in tended . B ut they

m ay  also

  fail  to

  discern

  and

  focus

 on

  some

  of the

  cri t ical aspects,

  or

 they

m ay discern

 and

 focus

 on

 other aspects . Wha t they a ctua l ly learn

 is the

 l ived

object

  of learning, the o bject of learning as seen

  from

  the learner 's p oint of

view,

  that

 is, the outcome or

 result

  of

  learning.

T h e O r i g i n

  of

  Powerful Ways

  of  A c t i n g

Learn ing is the process  of  becoming capable  of doing som ething

  ("doing"

in  the  wide sense)  as a  resul t  of having had  cer tain experiences  (of do ing

some th ing

  or of som ething happen ing) . D eveloping a learner ' s cap abi l i ty

of hand ling novel si tuations  in pow erful way s is

 considered

 to be on e of the

m o s t  imp ortant educa t ional a ims. In order to address how this can be done,

w e

 have

 to

 ref lect

 on the

 na ture

 of powerful

  w a y s

 o f

 act ing, that

 is,

 w a y s

 o f

engag ing in acts ins t rum ental to ach ieving one 's goals eff iciently.  Ac t ing in

powe r f u l

  w ays mea ns, therefore, doing

 different

  th ings

 to

 achieve  different

a i m s ,

 and

 doing

 different

  th ings

 in different

  s i tua t ions .

 T he

 power fu lnes s

 o f

on e's acts is relat ive to

 one 's

 a ims and the s i tua t ions .

L et u s

 f i rst  cons ide r

 the

  s i tua t ions .

 A s

 r a t i on a l be ings ,

  w e

 a lways

 try to

act

  in

 a cco rdance wi th

 an y

 given s i tua t ion, that

 is, the

  s i tuat ion

 as we

 per-

ce ive

 i t.

 W h a t k n o w l e dg e

 w e

 m i g h t

 try to

 exp lo i t depends

 o n h o w w e

 m a k e

sense of the s i tua t ion . Our previous exper iences

  affec t

  t he way i n w h ich

w e perceive

 the s i tuat ion, but the way in wh ich w e perceive the  situation

also

  affec ts

  wha t exper i ences  we see as  relevant  in  tha t par t i cu lar  s i tua -

t ion . W e a re

 trying

 to ac t in

 pow er fu l w ays , tha t

 is, w e a re

 t ry ing

 to

 ach i eve

our a i m s ,

 not in

 relat ion

  to the

  s i tuat ion

 in an

 obje ct ive sense,

  but in

 r e l a -

t ion to the s i tua t ion as we see i t . P o w e r f u l w a y s o f ac t ing spring  from  p o w -

erfu l  w a y s

 o f

  seeing.

Let

 u s

 take

 an

 exam ple . Someone

  is

 standing

 in a

 lake wi th

 th e

 water

 up to

his

 knees

  and

 aiming

  at a

 fish

  in the

 w ater with

  a

 harpoon.

 He

 m ight

 a im at

the

  fish

  where  it  appears  to be,  that  is,  where  he  actually sees  it, or at a

sl ightly  adjusted  angle, that

  is,

 w here

  he

  th inks

 it

  should actual ly

 be if he

takes

 the

 refraction

  of the

 l ight into considera t ion.

 These tw o

 different w a y s

of act ing are based on two different w ays of understanding the si tuation and

the

 latter is

 m ore

 pow erful than the former. Let us take ano ther exam ple. Let

us  imagine that a sales tax of 10% is introduced in Hong Kong. One car

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 19/255

M A R I O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

dealer selling expen sive cars sim ply increases  the prices by  10%, while an-

other adds only  5% on to the  previous prices.  T he  first  car  dealer assumes

(wrongly) that sa les tax mu st be added to the previous pr ice an d the buy er

m ust a lw ays pay for i t . However, the second one p redicts ( r igh t ly) tha t de-

m and w il l be adversely  affected  by the pr ice increase and real izes that even

if

  10% of the net price has to be given to the tax auth or i t ies , a par t of this sum

has to be absorbed by the se ller. (A sim ilar exam ple is e laborated in chap. 8).

A gain, there are tw o different  w ays of unders tanding the same s i tua t ion and

hence tw o w ays of ac t ing , one of which i s mo re po w erful than the o ther .

Let us look a t some other exam ples th at i llustra te the thesis that po w erful

w a y s

 o f

 actin g derive  from p o w e r fu l w a y s

 o f

 seeing.

 L et us

 take

 a

 very s im -

ple one to

 begin w ith.

 A

 wo rd problem

  w as

 given

 to

 some 7-year-old ch i l-

dren.  T he problem  is as  fo l low s: "I d idn ' t have m uch m oney th i s m orn ing

w hen I w ent to schoo l . B ob gave back 4 kronor th at he had borrow ed f rom

m e last w eek, and w ith that I cou ld buy a  green  chocolate bar for 7 kronor.

How  m u c h m o n ey  did I have th i s morn ing w hen  I c a m e  to school?

1 1

Some of the chi ldren knew the answer a lm ost ins tant ly , w hereas o thers

struggled  in vain. W as there anyth ing tha t th e form er could do tha t the latter

could not? A ctual ly none of the chi ldren had ever seen a problem l ike th is ,

nor did they remem ber any addit ion tables . Those w ho d id not do too w el l

saw the problem  as one of addi t ion ; the chi ld had some kronor to begin w i th

an d

  then

  he got 4

  more , which made

  7

  al together .

  B u t

  w ha t then caused

these children  diff iculty  was the ques tion: How can you add w hen you do n ' t

k n ow w h a t

 to add to? Th e

 chi ldren

 w ho did not f ind the

 problem

 di ff icul t  a t

all said som ething like this

 to

 them se lves :

 "I can sa y

 tha t

 he had 7

 kronor

 a l-

together and I know that he got 4 kronor  from  Bob. S o I have to take aw ay 4

kronor f rom 7 kronor."

 They

 cont inued , "One goes aw ay: 6 . Tw o goes away :

5. Three

 goes

 away : 4. A nd four goes a w a y : 3. So he had 3 kronor th i s mom -

ing." O thers m igh t have said, "I have to look for the o ther part . I hav e to f ind

out how

 many kronor

  I

 have

 to add to the 4

 kronor

  I got  from

  Bob ,

 to get 7

kronor a l together . So the answer is 3 ." These chi ldren s ta rted w i th wh at they

had got , w hich w as 4, then they coun ted three  un i ts , 5 , 6 , 7 , and visu al ize d

the "threeness" of those three u ni ts . Or others perhaps s im ply knew tha t 7

can, among other things,  be  broken down into 4 and 3. Th e  chi ldren w ho

cou ld come up w ith the answer easi ly did not see the problem as an addit ion /

subtraction problem but as a

 part-whole

 problem: the who le and one of the

parts  are given, the w h o l e  is 7 and the given part  is 4; the m iss ing par t m us t

then be 3. So the dif ferenc e between the chi ldren who han dled the prob lem

easi ly an d the others w ho did not was not so m uc h w ha t they  did, bu t ra ther

w hat they saw, that is , how they understood the prob lem . The ch i ldren w ho

could solve the problem saw i t in term s o f par ts and w ho le , and therefore

could

 solve

 i t easily,

 whereas

 the children  w ho could not solve the

 problem

saw it in

  term s

  of the

  ar i thmetic operat ion, that

  is,

  addi t ion ,

 and

  therefore

had diff icult ies

  solving

 i t . Th e

 po int here

  is

 tha t ,

 in

 ma ny cases , see ing s im-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 20/255

T H E

  S P A C E

  O F  L E A R N I N G

pie ar i thmet ic problem s

  in

 term s

 of part-whole

 relat ions

 is a more

 powerfu l

w ay

  of seeing them than seeing them in terms of ari thmetic operat ions

(Neuman, 1987) as al ready shown. The

 part-whole

  way of seeing works

very wel l

 for

 an y

 of the

 p rob lem s

 in

 which

 tw o

 par ts

 are

 given

 and you

 have

to   find  the whole, or when the who le and one of the par ts are given and you

have

  to

  look

  for the

 other part .

  For

  example ,

a + b = _

a-b =

= c

a-_ = c

Seeing the prob lem as a

 part-whole

 relat ion en ables the ch ild to act in a

p o wer fu l  w a y ,

  in the

  sense

  o f

  hav ing

  a

  capab i l i ty

  to

  deal wi th

  dif ferent

prob l ems .

An other w el l-known

 a nd

 more complex exam ple

 of how

 "the ca pa bil i ty

of seeing" is of decisive im portance,  is de Groot's (1965) wo rk on expertise

in

 chess p lay ing .

 "What

 is it that chess m asters are esp ecial ly good at?" de

Groot asked, eag er

 to

 find

 out

 w hether

 it is

 true,

 as

 ma ny people bel ieve, that

w h a t

 chess masters

 a re

 good

 a t is

 being able

 to

 m ental ly visual ize

 and try out

a n u mb e r  of  al ternatives act ions (and their consequences)  in great depth.

However ,

 this

 did no t in fact

 turn

 o ut to be the

 case.

 The

 chess m asters

 d id

not try

 m ore al ternat ive courses

 o f

 a ct ion than other players ,

 o r

 follow  them

up   fo r longer. B ut the courses  o f act ion they considered were m ost ly more

powerfu l  w a y s

 of

 han dl ing

 th e

 si tuations than other cou rses

 o f

 ac t ion would

have

 been .

 S o ,

 w h a t

 was i t

 that enabled

 the

 chess masters

 to

 find

 m ore pow-

erful

  w a y s

 o f

 handl ing

 th e

 si tuations?

 T he

 m ost striking

  fact

 w as

 that chess

m as ters seemed

  to see the

 chessboard  differently

  to

  other people:

We know

  that

  inc reas ing

 ex p e r i en ce

 and

  k n o w l e d g e

 in a spe cific

  f ield  ( che ss ,

 for in-

s tance) has the

 effect  tha t

 th ings (prope r t ies , e tc . ) which , a t

 ear l ier

 s tages , had to be ab-

stracted, or even inferred are apt to be immediately perceived at later stages. To a

ra the r

  large

  ex ten t ,

 abs t rac t ion is  replaced by  percep t ion ,  but we do not k n o w m u c h

abou t how

 th i s

 w o r k s , n or w h e re the bord e r l ine l ies. As an effect  of th is

 r e p l a c e me n t ,

 a

so-cal led

 "given" problem s i tua t ion

 is not

 rea l ly g iven s ince

 it is

 seen

 different ly

  by an

expert than  it is perce ived by an  inexpe r i enced pe r son ,  (de  Groot, 1965, p p . 33-34)

Al though  de

 Groot also found  that chess masters were m uch bet ter

 at re-

m em bering posi tions o n the

 board

 than n ovices, this w as only true w hen the

arrangements represented m eaning ful pat terns , and was not the case w hen

the

  arrangements were random configurat ions .

 In the

 latter case,

  the

 chess

masters ' memories were

 no t

  significan tly better than that

 o f

  other people.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 21/255

8  M A R I O N ,  R U N E S S O N ,  T S U I

These  findings were replicated  by Chase  and  S imon (1 973) w ho by ex-

amining   the w a y s  in w hich chess m asters reconstructed c on f igu rat ion s that

they

 had

 br ief ly seen,

 and the

 errors that they m ade

 in

 doing

 so ,

 arrived

 at the

interpretation that chess

  masters

 can

 remember

  a

  great number

  o f

 pat terns

of about e ight pieces, and that they interpret every co nf ig ura t ion on the

board in terms of a t m ost seven or e ight such pat terns.

 These

 pat terns form a

kind of giga ntic a lphabet com prising up to 10,000 le t ters , each one corre-

sponding to a cer ta in pat tern (c i ted in Berei ter & S carda m elia , 1993).

T h e

 m a in d i f f e r ence be tween chess mas te r s

 an d

 less exper ienced p la y-

ers, acc ord ing to th is line of reason ing, h as to do w i th the d i f fe ren ces in

w ays of see ing the chessbo ard , and d i f fe re nc es in w ay s of see ing var iou s

co n f ig u ra t i o n s  a s

 m e a n in g f u l

  pa t te rns .

  I t is the

 chess m as te r 's

 w a y o f

  see-

ing that enables

  the

 p l a y e r

 to

 engage

  in

 p o w e r fu l  w a y s

 o f

 ac t i n g .

 A n d

 there

are o ther s im i la r find ings on the na ture of exper t i se . Glaser and Chi (1988)

showed that exper ts

 an d

 nov ice s

 di f fe r  as to the

 p r o b l e m s t h e y

 see as

 s i m i -

lar and

  those problems they

  see as

  dif ferent .  P h y s i c i s t s

 a re

  ab le ,

  for in-

s tance , to see tha t the problems of r iver cur rents , and the problems of

h e a d w i n d s   an d  t a i l w i n d s  in  a i rp l a n e s i n v o l v e s i m i l a r m a t h e m a t i c a l an d

p h y s i c a l a s p e c ts , s u c h a s r e l a t i v e v e l o c i t i e s ( B r a n s f o r d , B r o w n . &

Cocking, 2000) . S imi la r  f ind ings  or ig ina te  from  such d ive r se  f ields as

e lec t ronic c i rcui t ry (Eg an & Sch w ar tz , 1 979) , rad io lo gy (L esg old , 1988) ,

com pute r p rogram m ing (E hr l i ch & So loway , 1 984) and t each ing (Sabe r s .

C u s h m g ,

  &

  Ber l iner , 1991) .

Rela t ing

 these cases, Bra nsfo rd

 et al.

 (2000) stated tha t exp ertise

 in a do-

m a i n  is  characterized  by  sensi t ivi ty to pat terns  o f  mean in g f u l i n f o r m a t i o n

that m i g h t no t be  avai lable to o thers dea l ing w i th th e same p rob lem s w i t h in

the same dom ains. In this book, w e w ou ld l ike to assert that var ious degrees

of  expertise, that is, the  capabi l i ty of ac t ing in p o w e r fu l w a y s

 w i th i n

  a cer-

tain

 d o m a i n ,

 is

 re f lec ted

 in the

 var ious way s

 o f

 see ing , tha t

 is, in the

 var ious

meanings seen in a par t icular scenar io or problem.

T h u s  i t can be  seen th a t people  act not in r e l a t i o n to  s i t u a t i o n s a s s u c h ,

but in  re la t ion  to  s i t u a t i o n s as  they pe rce ive , expe r ience ,  an d u n d e r s t a n d

them. O ne o f the m os t f r e que n t ly r ecur r ing f ind ings

  f rom

  our own r e -

search , as w el l as f rom oth ers ' r esearch , i s tha t w ha teve r s i tu a t io n peop le

enco unte r , they  see i t , exp e r ience  i t , and  u n d e r s t a n d it in a l im i te d n u m b e r

of

 qua l i t a t ive ly dif ferent

  w ays (see M ar ton  &  Booth , 1997) . In r e l a t ion to

par t icu la r a im s,

 some

 w ays o f see ing a r e more po w er fu l than o the r s . Pow-

e'rful  w a y s o f a c t ing der ive from  p o w e r f u l w a y s o f see ing , and the way t h a t

s o m e t h i n g  is  seen  o r  exper ienced  is a  f u n d a m e n t a l

 f ea ture

  o f  l e a r n i n g . If

w e w ant lea rners to deve lop cer ta in capa bi l i t ie s , w e m us t m ake i t po ss ib le

for  them to deve lop a ce r ta in way of see ing or exper ienc ing . Conse-

quent ly , a r ranging for lea rn ing imp l ies a r ranging for deve lopin g

 learners'

w a y s

 o f

 seeing

 o r

 exper ienc ing , tha t

 i s ,

 deve lop ing

 th e

 e y e s t h r o u g h w h i c h

the world is perceived.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 22/255

T H E   S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G

WAYS OF  S E E I N G

Wh a t

 does it

 take

 to

 develop

 the learner's

 eyes? W hat

 is

 meant

 by "a way of

seeing something?" Whatever we at tend to is inexhaustible, in the sense

tha t

 the

 inform at ion that

 can in

 principle

  be

 gained

  from  it is

 un l im i ted.

 A t

the sam e t ime , as George M iller so aptly declared, a lm ost 50 years a go, our

capac i ty for process ing inform at ion i s seriously l imited (M iller, 1 956). T he

inevitable mechanism

  o f

 selectivity

 originates

  from

  the

 co ntradiction

  be-

tween un l im i ted in form at ion

 and the

 highly l imi ted capaci ty

 fo r

 process ing

this

  in format ion wi th w hich hum ans are equipped.

W henever people at tend

 to

  something, they discern certain aspe cts

 of it,

and by doing so pay more at tention to some things, and less at tention or

none

  at all to

 other  things.

If one

  person discerns certain aspects

  of

  something

  and

  another person

discerns par t ly o r wh ol ly  different  aspects, we say that the two people see

the  sam e thing in different  w a y s . So, a way of  seeing something  can be de-

f ined  in

 t e rm s

 of the

 aspects that

 are

 discerned

  at a

 certain point

 in

 t im e.

 T he

aspects

  are

 thus discerned (and

 attended to) at the same

 t im e rather than

 o ne

at  a  t im e . A par t icular  way o f  seeing something  can be

  defined

  by the as-

pects discerned, that

 is, the

 cri t ical features

 of

 w h a t

  is

 seen.

A n aspect of a thing co rresponds  to the way in w hich that th ing m ight di f-

fer  from, or be

  s imi lar

 to, an y

 other thing, that

 is, the w ay it is

 perceived

 to

be, or the w ay  that it is experienced by someone as different from, or similar

to

 som ething else. Th e problem cited previously (a bou t the child with 7 kro-

nor)

 w as

 unders tood,

 o r

 exp erienced,

 by

 som e chi ldren

 in

 te rm s

 o f its part-

w h o l e

 s t ructure. Some chi ldren not iced that it had the s ame who le in it

 ( i .e. ,

7)

  as

  some other problem they

 had

  experienced,

  and in

 that respect

  it

 d i f -

fered  from

 problems

  in

  which

  th e

  whole

  w as

  other than

  7.

  Furthermore,

am ong those problems

  in

 wh ich

 the

 w ho le

 w as 7, i t w as

 s im i lar

 to

 those

 in

w h i c h  one of the parts was 4 and  different from  those problems in w hich

no ne o f the parts w as 4. Seeing the problem in terms o f its

 part-whole

 struc-

ture mea ns, we bel ieve, seeing w hat the par ts a nd the who le

 a re

 and wha t

they

  are

  not .

However, other children understood

  the

  same problem

  in

  terms

  of the

ari thm etic operation involved. They tho ugh t

 i t w as

 similar

 to

 other addit ion

prob lems ,  and that i t differed  from problems about subtract ion. A m ong ad-

dition

 prob lems ,

  i t w as

  similar

 to

 those

  in

 wh ich

 the

 second addend

 w as 4,

and different

  from

 those

 in

 w hich this

 w as not the

 case. They probably also

t hough t

 tha t i t was  similar to those addit ions in w h i c h the s um is 7, and di f-

ferent f rom those in w hich this wa s not the case. Seeing the problem in terms

of the

 ari thm etic operat ion involved required

 a

 thou gh t process l ike this ,

 w e

bel ieve.

 The reason that som e children c ould solve the problem and others

could not was because they  saw the same problem in different ways, that is,

because they at tended

  to

  different  aspects . At tending

  to a

  certain aspect

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 23/255

J _0 M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

means compar ing something

  w e

 exper ience w i th o ther th ing s tha t

 w e

 have

experienced earl ier . Th e kin ds of thin gs broug ht into the comparison

  def ine

the   aspect that  is attended  to .

In

 Lo and Ko

  (2002),

  the

  Engl ish

  lessons

 of two primary

 Grade

 4

c lasses a re d iscussed . Eviden ce f rom the lea rn ing outcom es sug ges ts tha t

m ost chi ldren  in one o f the classes un de r s tood  the "s" at the end of verbs a s

s igna l ing th i rd person s ingu la r , tha t

 is ,

 that they "saw"

  the "s" in

 th is par -

t icu la r way. Th i s imp l ies ,

  w e

 be l ieve , tha t they m ade

 an

 im pl ic i t compar i-

son

  o f  verbs where  "s" was  present wi th  th e  same verb where  " s" was

absent . In the o ther c lass , most c h i ldren d id not seem to pay a t te n t io n to the

"s." They compared,  w e th ink , im pl i c i t ly , the verbs w i th o ther verbs .  T he

"s" ha s to do

 w i th

 the

 third

 person

  s ingu la r , t ha t

 is , i t ha s to do

 w i t h g r am -

m ar ; wh e reas the com par i son w i th o the r ve rbs—or o the r w ords— has to

do wi th the me aning . The m ajo r i ty of the ch i ldren in the fi r s t c lass seemed

to

  attend

  to

 both

  th e

  g r a m m a t i c a l

  and the

  mean ing a spec t s

  of the

  verbs,

w hereas m ost ch i ldren in the second c lass d id not seem to a t tend to the

grammat ica l aspec t  o f  these verbs.

A ccording to this l ine of reason ing, a way o f seeing can be ch aracter ized

in terms of the aspects

 discerned

  that  are  attended  to  s imultaneously. This

defini t ion   is very w ide and very narrow at the same t im e . It is very w ide be-

cause  it refers  to the  m e a n i n g o r  appearance  o f a l m o s t a n y t h i n g . A nd it is

very narrow because

  it

 s imply a im s

 a t

 te l l in g apar t

 d i f ferent

  w a y s

 o f

 seeing

in respects th at a re cr i t ica l in re la t ion to the eff ic iency  of the ac ts tha t  spnng

from

  those ways of seeing, those meanings, and those appearances.

D I S C E R N M E N T

In order

 to see som ething in a cer ta in w ay, a person m us t discern ce r ta in fea-

tures of that thing. We should a lso be c lear about the

  dif ference

  between

discerning   and  be ing to ld . M edica l s tudents , for ins tance , m ig ht be advised

by

 the ir professors

  to try to

 not ice dif ferent  fea tures

 o f

 the i r pa t ien ts , such

 as

the color of the l ips , the m oisture of the skin, the ease of breath ing , and so

on ;  th is  is be ing to ld . But in order  to  fol low  th is advice , the  s tuden t s m us t

experience those features,  and the only w ay to

 exper ience

 them is to

 expe ri-

ence ho w they can vary. N otic in g the color of a pa t ie nt 's l ips , for exa m ple ,

w o u l d

 no t

  mean ve ry m uch

  if lip

 color

  wa s the

  same

  fo r

  everyone.

Simi lar ly , / rawe

 o f

  reference

  in

 phys ics only mak es sense

 i f we can

 t h i n k

of more than on e f ram e of r eference .  Even very abstract not io ns der ive the ir

m eanin g ( in the sense of the exper ience of und ersta nd ing ) throu gh var ia-

t ion. H istor ical ly, no one was aware o f na tu ra l num ber s ,  fo r ins t ance ,

 unt i l

the re w ere o ther num bers such as nega t ive num bers . At tha t poin t , the na tu -

ra l num bers cou ld be  ident i f ied  as those tha t were not nega t ive num bers .

This

 is wh y in a sense we of ten  know very l i t t le abo ut our ow n coun try

 un t i l

w e

  leam   about other countr ies .

  A nd

  th i s

 i s why

  teachers gene ra l ly g ive

 a

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 24/255

1 .  THE  SPACE  OF  L E A R N I N G  1J_

number

 of different

  exam ples

 in

 order

 to

 explain

  a new

 concept

 o r

 principle.

If

 teach ers give one exam ple only, they do so because they them selves con -

sider the other alternatives  to be  obvious (which, however, they m ay not be

fo r

  the students). In order to explain w hat a frame o f reference is , i t is not

suff icient

  to

 point

 out

 that when

 w e

 consider

  a

 moving body

 in the sk y from

the point of view of the ground, w e con sider i t from a part icular fram e of  ref-

erence . W e also have to give other exam ples, su ch as considering i t from the

poin t of view of another body m oving in the sky in paral lel with the

 first

 one

and  at the same velocity.

B y  experiencing variat ion, people discern certain aspects

  of

  their envi-

ronmen t ;

 w e

  could perhaps

  say

  that they become  "sensitized"

 to

 those

 as-

pects . This m eans that they

 are

 likely

 to see

  future

  events

  in

 term s

  of

 those

aspects; the phy sician w il l pay at tention to the c olor of the pa tient 's l ips, the

phys ic i s t w il l pay at tention to the fram e of reference

  from

  w hich a body in

mo ve me n t

  is

 co nsidered.  This

  is

 w h a t

 w e

 me a n

  by

  learning

  to see

 certain

th ings in

 cer tain w ays.

 O ne

 m ain

 way of

 dealing w ith novel si tuations

 is to

m ake u se of previou s expe rience. It is im portant to develop the ca pa bil i ty

for profess ional  se e ing , that

 is,

 seeing si tuations

 in

 term s

 of

 features that

 are

general ly cri t ical w ithin

 one 's

 professiona l f ield. But i t is equa lly imp ortant

to be able to discern o ther featu res that are not cri t ical in a gen eral sense, but

that

 m ay be

  cri t ical

 in a

 specific  case.

 N ot

  only

 do you

 need

  to

 discern fea -

tures that have proved to be e ssential in the past , but you m ust be a ble to dis-

cern   new  fea tures when they  are  cri t ical . This  is in  fact  very central  in

research

 an d it is

 very m uch

 how new

 discoveries

 a re

 m a de.

 Bu t we

 should

rem em ber tha t even the discernm ent of enti rely new features depends on the

varia tion yo u h ave enc oun tered earl ier .

The w ord "fea ture" has been used to stand for at tribute, or aspect , such as

color of l ips, f rame  of

 reference,

  tal lness, and so on, and we ha ve pointed to

the

  fact

  that human beings cannot discern

  a

  feature wi tho ut experiencing

variation

 in a

 corresponding dim ension. How ever,

 we no t

 only discern fea -

tures,

 bu t a lso discern different  qu ali t ies ( i .e. , values ) in the relevant dim en-

sions such  a s  "blue," "ray  of  light,"  "very short," and so on.

D i s c e r n m e n t  a n d Context:  Parts  an d  W h o l e s

So far we ha ve talked about the  di scernment o f  features and values

 wi th in

features.

 Bu t i t i s a lso poss ib le to th ink abo ut discernm ent as a del im i tat ion

of wholes

  from

  thei r context and as a del imi tat ion of par ts w i thin wholes

(cf .  Svensson, 1976).

Mar ton

 and B ooth (1 997) gave an example:

W h a t

 does i t take to see a mo t ion l e s s de e r a mong the dark t rees a n d b ushe s of the

 n i gh t

w oods?

 To see i t at all we

  have

 to

 d isce rn

 it f rom  the

  su r r oun d ing t re e s

 a nd

 b ushe s ,

 w e

have  to see i ts

 c o n t o u r s ,

 it s ou t l i ne , the  l imi t s tha t d i s t i ngu i sh  it from

  w h a t s u r r o u n d s

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 25/255

\2  M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

it.

 W e have t o see,  a t  least par t ia l ly , w h e r e i t s tar ts and  w h e r e i t e nds  . ..  [ b u t ] no t o n l y

do we have to d isce rn i t from  i ts

 c on t e x t ,

 a s a de e r in the woods , bu t we a l so have to d i s -

cern   i ts par ts ,  the way  t h ey

  re la te

  to  each  o ther , and the way  t hey r e l a t e  to the

  w h o l e .

There fore , on s e e ing the de e r in the w oods ,  in s e e i n g its co n t o u r s , w e a l so s ee pa r t s o f

i ts body, i ts he a d , i ts a n t l e r s , i t s forequ ar ters , an d so on, and  t h e i r r e l a t i o n s h i p i n t e r ms

of  s tance , (pp. 86-87)

Discern ing the re la t ion of par ts w i th in wh oles and d iscern ing the wh ole

from   the con tex t is an im por tan t aspect o f d i sce r nm en t . Eq ua l ly impo r tan t is

discern ing the w ay w holes r e la te to the con tex t . Th is i s because the way the

w hole relates to the con text shapes the d iscern m ent of the par ts

  w i t h in

  the

wh ole . W hat does th i s m ean? Le t us take some classroom data as exam ples.

Let us  star t with th e  role that context p lays  in d e t e r min ing t he m e a n i n g

assigned

  to a

  p h e n o m e n o n .

  In a

  pr imary Chinese lesson

  on

  s e m a n t ic s ,

wh ich   is d iscussed  in chap te r 2, we see how one w ord took on a very

  differ-

en t

 mean ing when

  i t w as put in di f fe rent

  con tex t s .

 In

 th is lesson,

  the

 teacher

told a story about how one w ord in Chinese  y ao  (P u to ng hu a) [ -f|- ] (w hich

can be

  translated l i terally

 a s

 "wan t")

 w as

  explo i ted

 by a

 barber,  Afa n t i ,

  to

carry the d iametr ical ly

  opposed

  m e a n i n g s ,  "give"

  (gei)

  ( P u t o n g h u a )

  [$&]

and

 "keep"

 (liu) ( P u t o n g h u a ) [^] in order  to take his r ev enge on a cus tomer .

A h u n g ,  w ho  kept

  r e fus ing

  to pay for the  service.  In the  story,  the  barber

asked Ahu ng wh ether he

 "w an ted" (yao buyao

  (P u to ng hu a) [ - f c^ -l c - ]) h i s

eyebrows , w hereupon A hu ng rep l ied tha t

 h e

 "w an ted"

 h is

 eyebrows  (yao),

m eaning he wanted to keep h is eyebrows .

  A f a n t i

  shaved of f A hu ng ' s eye-

b ro ws

 and said, "You 'w an t '

  (yao)

  your eyebrows , so I ' l l g ive them to yo u "

Ahung was speech less because he had indeed sa id yao .  Af te r  th i s ,  A f a n t i

asked

  Ahung i f he

  "wan ted"

  h i s beard , and Ahung, who had a  b e a u t i f u l

beard , immediately said ,

 buyao

  ( P u t o n g h u a ) [^ - fc ] ,  tha t

 is ,

  "don ' t want ,"

mean ing  he did not  w a n t

  Afa n t i

  to  shave  off his  beard . However ,

  Afan t i

shaved off his beard all the s ame . Aga in , A hu ng was speech less because he

h ad  indeed said buyao

 ("don't

  want") .

In

 th is story,

 we see

 tha t

 the

 wordjao

 w as

 ass igned di f fe rent  m e a n i n g s

 in

relation   to  d i f ferent  contexts .  In the  first  in s t ance , A hun g r e l a ted  it to the

context  o f a

 barbershop

  where people  had  the ir hair  and  beard tr im m ed  o r

cut , but wou ld  n o t  typica l ly have their eyebrows shaved off . Let us r efer to

this context  as Contex t  A .  Therefore, when  he  said yao,  he  m ean t tha t he

wanted to

  keep

  his eyebrows. However,

  Afa n t i

  del ibera tely relate d i t to a

different

  context where  "wan t"

  (yao)

  can  mean "give  " Let us r efer  to th i s

contex t as Contex t B . For exam ple , in the con tex t o f m ak ing an  offer,  such as

offer ing   a dr ink, "Do you wa nt a

 dr ink?"

 a pos i t ive rep ly from  the addressee,

yao,

  wo uld entai l the person m ak ing the

 offer

  ac tua l ly

 g iv ing

 a drink to the

addressee . Then, in the second  ins tance , when Ah ung used buyao  to m e a n

that h e "did  no t w a n t" h is beard, h e w a s relat ing y ao  to the con tex t in w h i c h

Afan t i

 w as operating  in the previous exchange w here ya o  m e a n t

 "give,"

 tha t

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 26/255

1.  T H E S P A C E O F

  L E A R N I N G

  1 J3

is C ontext B. However, A fanti

 deliberately

 related buyao ("don't

 want")

 to

the  context in which A hung w as op era ting previous ly , w here  y a o  m e a n t

"want" or "keep,"

 that

 is

 C ontext

  A .

T he

  story

  in

 this lesson sugg ests that

 the

  context

 to

 which som ething

 is

related

 cannot necessari ly

 be

 taken

 for

 granted.

 In

 c lassroom learning s i tua-

tions,

  it is

  very important that there

  is a

  m utua l unders tanding (be tween

teacher and students) of the context to w h i ch the teacher re la tes the

 object

 o f

learning in

 order

  to

 help learners discern

  its

 cri t ical

  features .

Let

 u s

 look

 at tw o

 lessons

  on

 w riting

  a book

 report,

  for

 example, also

 re-

ported by  Ch ik (2002). In one lesson, the teacher (T eacher A ) used genres o f

w ri ting as the

  context

  for

 discussing

  the

  com ponen ts that s tudents should

include in a

 book report.

 She

 sp ecifically used na rrative

 as a

 genre

 of w ri ting

and

 asked the s tudents to th ink abo ut th e features that dis t ing uish a book r e-

port

 from  a

 narrative.

 In the

 other lesson ,

 the

 teacher (Teacher

 B )

 used

 a

 dif-

ferent

  context

  for

  discussing

  a

  book report , that

  is,

  different  w a y s

  o f

present ing  a book report .

  Specifically,

  she cited the use of pic tures to pres-

ent a

 book report,

  a

  format with which students were also familiar.

Wh i c h

 is a more

 power fu l

  context , in the sense of be ing more

 effective

  in

bringing about learning? W hich w il l help learners discern

 the

  cr i t ical f ea -

tures of a book

 report?

T he

 answer

 to

 this question depends

 o n the

 aspects

 of the

 book report

 o n

w hich the teacher wants to focus. In these tw o lessons, the ob jec t of learning

is the

 essentia l comp onents

  of a

 book report.

  O ne

 could argue that pic toria l

representat ion

 is one kind of

 form at

 and that i t can be con trasted w ith a w rit -

ten   format .

 B u t

 there

 i s

 noth ing m uch tha t

 can be

 said apart

 from

 this ,

 and it

does

  no t

  help

  the

  s tudents

 to

  discern

  the

  com ponen t s t ha t

  are

 cri t ica l

 to a

book report .

 O n the

 o ther hand, other genres

 o f

 w ri ting , such

 a s

 narra t ives,

contain various components such  as  time, p lace, people, and  sequence of

events

 tha t

 are not

  found

  in

 book reports . What dis t inguishes

 one

 genre

 o f

wri t ing

  from

  another

  is the

  com ponents tha t they conta in .

 In

 o ther w ords ,

the g e n re s

 o f

  wri t ing, o f wh ich book reports  are one instantiation and narra-

tives

 another, provides

 the

 context

 to

 w h i ch

 the

 cri t ica l

 features  o f

 book

 re-

ports re la te .

Let us look a t how  Teacher A  dealt with th e com ponents cr i t ica l to a book

report . Sh e put e ight com ponents that w ere re la ted to books on the board.

They were: price

 of the book,

 date

 of

  reading, genre, call number, author,

n a m e

 of the

 book, summ ary,

  and

  com m entary ( impression

  af ter

  reading) .

Am o n g th e m ,

 the first

 four, a l though having

 to do

 w i th books , were

 no t

 rele-

vant

 to book reports. The

 teacher asked

 the

 students

 to

 take away those com -

pon ents that they  felt  should not be included in a book report. B y asking th e

students to do that , the teacher w as doing two things. Firs t , she was a t tend-

ing  to the

 internal relationsh ip,

  a part-whole

 relationship, betw een

 the

 c o m -

pon ents and the book report . T he com ponen ts that rem ained on the board

were author , name

  of the

  book, commentary,

  and

  summary. These

  four

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 27/255

J _4 M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N ,   T S U I

com ponents w ere related

  to

 each other

  in the

 sense th at they were co nsti tu-

tive

 of a book report in such a way that if any part w as m iss ing, i t w ould no

longer  be a book report .  F or exam ple, if the comm entary  w as m issing , the

book

 report

 w ould becom e a piece of text in an annotated bibl iography . Sec-

ond, she was at tending to the wa y in w hich the inclus ion of par ts that w ere

not

 critical

 to a

 book report wou ld affect

  the

 who le .

 F or

 example ,

 the

 inc lu-

s ion of the pr ice of the book and the pub l isher wou ld const itu te a wh ole that

is

 no

 longer

 a

 book report,

 bu t

 another genre,

 for

 example ,

 an

 advert isem ent .

T he

 discernmen t

  of the

 cr i tical com pone nts

 o f a

 book report

 w as

 b rough t

about

 b y

 discerning

 the

 book report

  as an

 ins tant iat ion

 o f

 genres

 of

 w ri ting.

In other words,

 ge nr e s  o f  wri t ing

  w as the con text to wh ich the book report

w as related. It is in relat io n to the book report a s a genre, as opposed to other

genres such

 as the

 n arra t ive ,  tha t discernmen t

 of the

 parts

 w as

 made poss i-

ble . In other w ords, i t i s the discernm ent of how the wh ole relates to the con-

text that enabled the discernm ent of the par ts (of the w ho le) .

VARIATION

W e i l lus t ra te the  s i gn i f i cance of var ia t ion for the  poss ib i l i t ies  to learn by

referr ing  to some s i tua t ions tha t w i l l

 hope f u l l y

  be  eas i ly recognizab le  to

the   reader .

Con sider h ow w e leam w hat coldness, ta l lness , or heaviness i s . Sa ying

that

 som ething is heavy does not mean an ything to us unless we experience

this agains t the background o f a difference  in w e igh t , t ha t is, w e igh t t ha t can

vary.

 In the

 same w ay,

 for

 i n st ance , know ing wha t

 red is

 presupposes

  the ex-

perience of other colors , that is , a var iat ion in colors . Even k now ing w hat

color i s presupposes an experienced variation of colors . Im ag ine for a mo-

m ent that there w as no variation of colors , tha t everything a round u s had the

same color. I t w ou ld be impo ssible for us to know w ha t red, green, or yel low

w ere, ju s t as i t wo uld be imp ossible for us to discern co lor as a feature. I f ev-

ery

  object

  w e

  encountered

  had the

  same color, this

  feature  of the

  object

w o u l d

 not be discerned.

T he

 s ign i f i cance

 of

 variat ion

 for

 seeing so m ething

 in a new way

 app l ies

to abstract objects as w el l . Research on the w ay that young chi ldren solve

simple ar i thmet ic problems has reported that chi ldren who always solve

simple addit ions ( l ike

 2 + 3 =, 5 + 1 =, 1 + 4 =,

 etc.)

 by

 start ing w ith

 the first

addend, can suddenly chang e s trategy and s tar t w i th the b iggest num ber. In-

stead of 2 + 3, 1 +4 and so on, the order of the addends is chan ged , and the

child adds 3 + 2 =, 4 + 1 = (cf. Carpe nter & M oser, 1984). In this si tuation, a

variat ion of order of the addends is open ed. Y ou co uld say that an aspect that

was taken for granted or was undiscerned became a discerned aspect . By

this opening

 of variat ion, a fea ture of addit ion (that is , that the sum is inde-

pendent  of order  of the addends)  is discerned.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 28/255

1 .

  THE

  S P A C E

  OF  L E A R N I N G  1 _5

O ne o f the m ain these s in th is book is that the pa t tern of var iat ion inher-

ent

 in the

 learning situation

 is

 fundamenta l

 to the

 developm ent

  of

  certain

capab i l i ti es ( see fo l low ing tex t ) . A n exper im enta l s tudy on mo tor l earn ing

(Moxley, 1979) demonstrated

  the

  importance

 of

  variation

  on

  l earn ing .

The exper im ent inc luded ch i ldren prac t i c ing to h i t a t a rge t w i th a ba l l .

C hi ldren in the control group alw ays threw the bal l a t the target  from  the

same di rec t ion , whereas ch i ldren in the exper imenta l g roup prac t i ced

throwing

  the

  bal l

  a t the

  t a rge t  f rom di f ferent  d i r ec t i ons . When

  the two

groups were compared t rying to hi t the target

  from

  a di rect ion that was

new to

 both groups ,

 the

 group tha t

 had

 prac t iced h i t ting

 th e

 target from d i f -

ferent di rect ions  w as then

  found

 to be  superior  to the group tha t h ad p rac -

t iced hi t t ing the target

  from

  the same direct ion al l the t ime. I t can be

con cluded that var iation

  in

 direction seemed

 to be a

 cri t ical  feature

  of the

pract ice and thus also cr i t ical for learning. However , we are not arguing

for variation in general , and we are not saying that the more variation there

is , the bet ter the poss ib i l i t ies to learn. W hat w e bel ieve is tha t var iat ion en-

ables learners

  to

 experience

  the

  features that

  a re

 critical

  for a

  part icular

learning as wel l as for the development of cer tain capabi l i t ies . In other

w ords, these fea tures m us t   be  experienced  as  dimensions  o f  variat ion.

Lea rn ing ,

 fo r

 i n s t ance ,

 to

 solve

 a

 prob lem

  in

 d i f ferent  w ays requi res expe-

r ience

 o f

 variat ion

 in

 solving s t rategies . U nde rs tanding

 the

 "manyness"

 of

a

 num ber requi res the exper ience of

 d i f ferent

  num bers , ju s t as the ab i li ty

to

  throw

  a

 bal l  from

  dif ferent

  di rec t ions

  and

  still

 hit a

 target requires

  the

exper ience  o f th row ing an ob jec t from

  different

  ang l es , and rea l iz ing h ow

a

 genera l p r inc ip le can encom pass

 d i f ferent

  exam ples requ i res the exper i-

ence of at leas t tw o

 d i f ferent

  examples , and so on .

In m a them at ic s , different  strategies for solving problem s,  different n u m -

bers,

  and so on all

 m a ke

 up

 dim ensions

  of

 variation.

  O ne

  particular solving

strategy is one

 va lue

 in a

 dimension

 of

 variat ion, w hereas

 a

 different

  strat-

egy is another value in this dim ension. And thus the strategy as used in a spe-

cif ic  example i s an ins tance of tha t s t ra tegy . When we exper ience

something, we discern aspects, or features, of the object and we experience

values in the corresponding dimensions of var iat ion. The experienced as-

pects

 are

 discerned

 as

 values

 in dimensions of

 variation.

 I

 experience

 the ob-

ject on my table , for example, as a b lue, cyl indrical , ceramic mug with a

handle (an  instance).

 "Blue"

 a nd "cylindrical"  are values in dimens ions of

variat ion (e.g. , these are perceived in relat ion to the experience that they can

vary).

 Features of the m ug , such as the color, shap e, ma terial , and so on, are

sim ultaneo usly discern ed as a pattern of dim ensions of variat ion (or to put i t

s imply, as a pattern of variat ion), and these featu res consti tute the pa rt icular

object .

 In

 order

 to

 experience

  the

 object

 as a

 blue, cylindrical , ceramic m ug ,

all of these aspects m us t be discerned and related to potential dim ensions of

variation.

 A nd because these aspects are necessary for defining th e object in

quest ion, they

  are

 also cal led

 its

  cr it ica l fea tures .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 29/255

M A R T O N ,  R U N E S S O N , T S U I

Patterns  of  Variation

A ccording to the preceding l ine of reasoning, i t i s necessary to pay close at -

tent ion

  to

 w h a t

 varies

 a nd

 w h a t

 is

 invariant

  in a

 learning s i tuat ion,

 in

 order

to

  understand wh at

  it is

 possible

  to

  leam

  in

  that si tuation

 and

 w ha t no t .

 In

the  different  studies reported  in th is book, w e are able to

  ident i fy

  certain

patterns

  of

  vanat ion :

1

 . Con tras t.  A s al ready m ent ioned, in order to experience som ething, a

person must experience something else

  to

 com pare

  it

 w i t h .

 In

 order

  to un-

derstand wha t "three" is , for ins tance, a person m us t experience som ething

t ha t

 is not

  three: "two"

  or

 "four,"

  for

 exam ple . This i l lus t ra tes

 h o w a

 value

(three,

  fo r

 ins tance )

 is

  experienced wi thin

 a

 cer ta in dim ens ion

 of

 vanat ion ,

w h i c h

  corresponds

  to an

  aspect (nu m erios i ty

  or

  "manyness") .

2.

  General iza t ion.  In order  to fu l ly  under s tand wh a t

 "three"

 is, w e m u s t

also experience varying appearances

  of "three,"  for

 exam ple th ree apples ,

three monkeys, three

 toy

 cars, three books,

  and so on.

 This vanat ion

  is

 nec-

essary in order for us to be able to grasp the ide a of "threeness" and separate

it

  from  i r relevant features (suc h

 as the

 color

  o f

  apples

 or the

  very

  fact  tha t

they

 are apples) .

3.

 Separat ion .

  In

 order

  to

 expenence

  a

 certain aspect

 of

 s o m e t h in g ,

 and

in

 order

  to

 separate th is aspect

  from

 other aspects ,

  it

 m us t vary w hi le o ther

aspects rem ain invariant . This

  i s how the "angle"

 aspect

 o f

 h i t t ing

 a

 target

wi th a bal l w as developed  in one  group o f ch i ldren in M ox ley ' s (1979) ex-

periment , m ent ioned previously .

 The

 expenm ent co uld a l so

 be

 expanded

 by

sys temat ica l ly

  varying

  the

  di s tance

 to the

  t a rge t ,

 fo r

  ins tance , w hi le o ther

aspects were kept invariant; then sy stem at ica l ly vary ing  the w e i g h t of the

bal l ,

 w hi le keeping other aspects invariant ,

 and so on . In

 th is

 way the

 ch i l -

dren could

 be

  prepared

  fo r

 various other s i tuat ions , such

 as

 hi t t ing

  targets

from   dis tances they have never thrown  f rom,  w i t h  ba l l s

  o f

  vary ing (and

novel ) weigh ts ,

 and so on.

4.

 Fusion.

  If

  there

  a re

 severa l c r i t i ca l a spec t s t ha t

  th e

  learner

  has to

take in to con s idera t ion

  a t the

 s a me t i me , t h e y m u s t

 a l l be

 exper i enced

  s i-

m u l t a n e o u s l y . I n

 eve ryday

  l i f e , i t is

 s e ldom tha t on ly

 o n e

 a spec t

 o f

  some-

th ing va r ie s a t a t ime , and so t he way i n w h ic h we re spond to a s i t ua t i o n ,

such

 a s

 h i t t i n g

 a

 t a rge t w i th

 a

 ba l l

 o r a

 p rob l em

 o f

 h u m a n re l a t i o n s , sp r in g

f rom

  a

 more

 genera l ho l i s t i c perce p t ion

  of the

 s i t u a t i o n .

 W e c a n

 c o m p a re

t h i s

 w i th a m ar r i age cou nse lo r ' s p ro fes s iona l w ay o f s ee ing hu m an re l a -

t i onsh ip p rob l ems . The counse lo r i s p robab ly s ee ing   d i f f e r en t  cases in

terms

  o f a  l i m i t e d n u m b e r  o f  a n a l y t i c a l l y s e p a ra t ed ,  b u t  s t i l l  s i m u l t a -

neous ly exper ienced, aspec t s .

  O u r

  c o n j e c t u r e

  is

  tha t see ing

  a

  cer ta in

c l a ss o f phen om ena i n t e rm s o f a s e t o f a spec t s tha t a re an a ly t i ca l l y s epa-

ra ted b u t s i m u l t a n e o u s l y e x p e r ie n c e d p r o v i de s a mo r e  ef fec t ive bas i s fo r

p o w e r f u l

  ac t i on t han

 a

  g l o b a l ,  und i f f e r e n t i a t e d

  w a y o f

  s ee ing

  th e

  s a m e

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 30/255

1 .

  THE SPAC E O F  L E A R N I N G  T 7

c lass

 o f

 p h e n o m e n a .

  W e

 bel ieve th at sep arat ing

  th e

 aspec t s

 f i rs t and

 then

fus ing  them

  together  is

  more ef f i c ien t ( f rom

  th e

  view

  o f

  being able

  to

adap t to chan ging condi t ions ) than never t ak ing th e cr i t ica l aspec t s apa r t .

W e also bel ieve that th is

  fus ion

  wi l l unavoidab ly t ake p lace th rough  th e

s i m u l t a n e o u s

 var ia t ion  in the  d imens ions  o f var ia tion cor responding  to

th e

  cr i ti ca l aspec t s .

A nother exa m ple of th is type of

 fusion

  is presen ted in the com parison of

tw o

 primary classes

 in

 Chinese languag e

 in

 chapter

 4,

 whereby different

  as-

pects of the  same word ( form, meaning , p ronuncia t ion)  are  successful ly

fused  in one of the

 classes,

 but not in the

 other.

 In

 chapter

 8,

 there

 is a  further

example of this type, in the comparison of two groups of secondary eco-

nomics classes .

  In

  this exam ple,

  the

 elasticity

  o f

 demand

  and the

 elasticity

of

 sup ply are f irst separated and then

  fused

  in the classes in one group ("the

learning   study" group),

  but not in the

  other group ("the lesson study"

group), in w hich they are only deal t w i th one at a t ime .

S I M U L T A N E I T Y  AN D  AWARENESS

W e

 have thus m ade

 the

 point that

 in

 order

 to

 discern

 a

 feature,

 a

 person m us t

experience variation

  in

 tha t

  feature .

  For

  example ,

  in

 order

  to

  experience

 a

teenage girl  as strikingly  tall ,  w e  must have encountered teenage  girls  as

typ ica l ly

 being sho rter

  or

 considerably shorter than this pa rt icula r girl .

 The

experience of tal lness derives

  from

  jux tapos ing w hat we see and w hat we

remem ber ; wha t we exper ience now and w hat we have experienced  befo re .

W e

 have

 to be

  aware

  of

 both

 at the

  same t ime.

In

 general ,

 to

 experience variation am oun ts

 to

 experiencing  different

  in -

stances at the sam e t im e. A part icular co lor is experienced aga inst the bac k-

groun d of other colors tha t w ere experienced in the past; an act of kindn ess

is

  experienced against

  the

  background

  o f

  acts

  o f

  kindness

  o r

  cruelty  that

w ere experienced in the past . V ariat ion is experienced very m uc h as we ex-

perience

  a

 m elody. Each tune

 is

 experienced

 in the

 context

 of

 other tun es.

 O f

course ,

 we w ou ld never experience a m elody i f w e experienced each tone

separately, one at a t im e. In the sam e w ay, w e can never exp erience variat ion

in

 any respect i f w e expe rience every instance one at a t im e. In order to exp e-

rience variat ion

 in a

 certain respe ct ,

 w e

 have

 to

 experience

  the

 different

  in-

stances that vary

  in

  that respect s imul taneously , that

  is , we

  have

  to

experience instances that

 w e

 have encountered

 a t different

  points

 in

 t im e,

 at

the s a m e

 time. W e call th is

 diachronic

 s imul tanei ty . This i s the s im ul taneou s

experience  of  different  ins tances  at the  same t ime, which  is necessary  for

experiencing variat ion   in a certain dim ension and for discerning  the aspect

of an

 instance

 corresponding to the

 dimension. How ever,

  you

 wi l l remem -

ber

  that

 w e

 have previously  defined

  a way o f

  seeing som ething

  as the

 dis-

cernment of var ious cr i t ical features of an ins tance s imul taneously . This

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 31/255

j_8

  M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

type of s im ul tanei ty , is cal led

 synchronic s imul tane i ty ,

 and is the  experience

of different  co-exist ing aspects of the same thing at the same t ime.

Let us br ief ly return to M oxley 's s tudy of chi ldren p ract icing to hit a tar-

get with a ball . In the ci ted exam ple, variat ion in the direction or an gle of the

throwing w as com pared to the absence of var iation, in relat ion to a group 's

abili ty

 to

 per form

 the

 task

 o f

 hi t t ing

 the

 target

 f rom a

 direct ion that

 was new

to both groups . A s we m ent ioned, i f the chi ldren were to leam to hit a target

wi th  a ny bal l , f rom any di rect ion, unde r any c ondi t ions , they w ould need to

pract ice throw ing the bal l f rom different  di rect ions , w i th

 different

 ba l ls (e .g .

each  one a different  w eigh t ) , under different  con di t ions (e .g ., different w i n d

force) .  M as ter ing th is capab i li ty w ould am ount to be ing ab le to discern a ll

o f

  those features

  and

  being able

  to

  take them

  a ll

  in to considerat ion

 at the

same t im e ( i .e . , seeing bal l -throw ing s i tua t ions

 in

 t e rm s

 o f

 features cr i t ical

for hi t t ing a target ) . Doing so amoun t s to exper iencing th e different  aspects

present  at the same t ime s im ul taneous ly . In a m etaphor ica l sense , w e could

ta lk abo ut spat ial in tegrat ion  o r  synchronic  s imul tane i ty  a t one par t i cu lar

point in t ime.

W hat does

  it

 take

 to

  experience

  different

  aspects

 o f the

  s ame p h e n o me -

non? C lear ly , the aspects m ust be discerned and the person m ust be   focal ly

aware o f t h em . So s imu l tanei ty in the synch ronic sense is obvious ly a  func-

t ion

  of

 di scernm ent .

Ex per i enc ing va r i a t i on i s , however , a s w e have show n , con t i ng en t on

th e

 s i mu l ta n e o u s a w a r e n e s s

 o f

 i n s t ances t ha t appea r

 a t d i f fe ren t

  p o i n t s

 in

t im e . W e w ou ld no t be ab l e to s ee a s t ory a s a s to ry w i tho u t t he s im u l t a -

neous awareness o f o ther s to r ies tha t we have come across in the pas t .

T h i s is a k i n d o f t e mp o r a l i n t e g r a t i o n , a s i m u l t a n e o u s a w a r e n e s s o f w h a t

w e a re exper i enc ing and wh a t w e have exper i enced be fo re . C l ea r ly , a s

w e l l ,

  i n w h a t w e a r e e x p e r i e n c i n g , w h a t w e h a ve e x p e r i e n c e d b e f o r e

m u s t  be or m u s t h a ve

 been

 d i s ce rned  in order  for us to e x p e r i e n c e it . In

th i s r e spec t , no t on ly synch ron i c bu t a l so d i ach ron i c  s i m u l t a n e i t y  is a

fu n c t i o n

  o f d i s ce rnmen t . Fu r the rmore , t he re can be no exper i ence o f

s y n c h ro n i c s i m u l t a n e i t y w i t h o u t

  th e

  e x p e r ie n c e

  o f

  d i a c h r o n i c s i m u l t a -

ne i ty ,

  b e c a u s e

  in order  to

  exper i ence

  tw o

  aspec t s

  of the

  s a me t h i n g

 to -

ge the r we mus t d i s ce rn bo th s epa ra t e ly , and  t h a t  c a n o n l y h a p p e n b y

h a v i n g exper ienced va r i a t i on i n t he d im ens ions o f va r i a t i o n  co r respond-

in g

  to

 e a c h

  one of the

  a spec t s .

There

  is another

  form

  o f the s im ul taneo us expenenc e in the synchronic

sense, tha t is, one t ha t is different from exper iencing different  aspects o f the

same th ing  at the s ame t im e . W ha t w e have in m in d is the s im u l taneous ex-

perience of the w hole and i ts par ts ; the wh ole be ing, for exa m ple, a deer in

the

 fores t (m ent ioned previously) ,

 a

 text

 w e

 read

 in

 school,

 o r

 bas ica l ly any-

th ing  that can be m ea ning ful ly divided into com ponen t par ts .

In an example tha t appears in chapter 4, tw o teac hers ' w ays of teach ing

Chinese a re com pared. B oth teach ers w ere try ing to develop t h e s t uden t s '

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 32/255

1.

  THE

  S P A C E

  OF

  L E A R N I N G

  1 _9

vocabula ry in the context o f the same story. O ne of the teachers deal t w i th

the story as a whole first, then wi th

 different

  paragraphs, sentences,  and fi-

nal ly with individual words, taking each feature of the words (such as

m eaning , sp el l ing, i .e . ,

 stroke

 pat tern,  and pronun c ia tion) o ne at a t im e . In

other words,

  the

  teacher dealt with

 the

  vocabulary i tems (words) sequen-

t ial ly and in  isolation, instead  of in context . T he  other teacher seemed  to

act in

 accordance w ith

 the

 view tha t

 the

 m ean ing

 of a

 character

 is mo di f ied

by

 the

 word

  or phrase in

 w hich

  it

 occurs; that

 the

 m ean ing

  of the

 wo rd

 or

phrase is m odif ied by the sentence in w hich i t appears; and that the m ean -

ing  of the

  sentence

  is

 m odi f ied

  by the

  text

  in

  wh ich

  it

  forms

  a par t .  S he

tried to make sure that al l of the   different  levels of the text (story, para-

graph, sentence, w ord, character)

  were

 present  in the  s tudents ' awareness

at  the

  same t ime ,

  by

  deal ing with each level

  in the

  context

  of the

  nex t

superordina te

  level. Furtherm ore , she deal t w i th di f ferent  features  of the

cha racters in the con text of each w ord, instead of deal ing w ith each char-

ac ter

 as an ind ividua l un i t as the other teacher had done.

Awareness

B u t ,

 where

 are instances, aspec ts, parts, and wh oles w hen they are exp eri-

enced sim ultan eo usly ? In a vivid sense they are present to us. They are not

s imply s tored away somewhere deep down

  in our

  m e m o ry;

  we can

  hear

them or see them ,  feel  them, sense them or imag ine them . They are in our

awareness .

Awareness  (w e use the

 word

  as a

 synonym

  to

 consc iousness)

 is the to-

tality  of a pe rson 's experiences of the wo rld, a t each p oint in t im e. I t i s a l l

that

 is

 p re sen t

 on

 every occas ion . A wareness changes dyna m ica l ly

 all the

t ime and every s i tuat ion is experienced against the bac kg rou nd o f previ-

ous experiences. T his occurs to varying degrees of course , bu t poten t ia lly ,

aw areness is present aga inst the backg round of a very, very g reat nu m ber

of previous experiences. And to

  affect

  our experiences a t this very mo-

m ent , here and now, these previous experiences a l l have to be present a t

any one

  t im e .

 In a

 wa y ,

 i f we

 exaggera te th ings

 a

  l i t t le ,

 we

 m i gh t

 say

 tha t

we are aware of everything a l l the t ime, s imply not in the same way.

Actua l ly there

 are

 very

  few

 th ings tha t

 we can be focal ly ,

  tha t

 i s ,

 sharply,

aware  of at the  same t ime ,  but a lot can  affect  the way in  w h i ch  we are

aware  o f these th ings .  In  accordance w i th Gu rw ich ' s (1964) account , w e

mi g h t say tha t the charac te r is t ic of hum an aw areness i s tha t a l im i ted num -

ber of ob jects , aspects of objects , or s i tuat ions com e to a t t rac t our a t tent ion

( i .e. ,  become focused), whereas a very great number of other things are

there as bac kg rou nd. I t i s against this back grou nd that w e experience the

things that w e are foca l ly

 aw are

 o f, that is, the things that are the focus o f

our a t tent ion .

 A

 gen eral ized

  and

 ever chang ing f igure-grou nd st ructure

 is

t hus  characteris t ic

 for our

  awareness .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 33/255

20

  M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

Discernment,

 Awareness and  Si mul ta ne i ty

Al though  learning can be viewed as the developm ent of both cap abi l i t ies

and

 values,

  the

 focus

 o f

 this

 book,

 as we

 stated earlier,

  is on

 learning

 as the

developm ent o f capab i l i ties . The kinds of

 capabil i t ies

 we

 focus

 on are those

that empow er learners to deal w ith s ituat ions in

 power fu l

  w ays , t ha t is, to si-

m ultaneously ( in the diachronic sense) focu s on features cri t ical for achiev-

ing a

  certa in a im. However,

  we can

  o nly exper ience s im ul tane ou s ly tha t

w h i c h w e can discern; we can only discern wh at we experience to vary; and

we can  only experience variation  i f w e have experienced  different  instances

previously

  and are

  ho ld ing them

  in our

  awareness s imul taneous ly

 ( in the

diachronic sense). So the three (or rather

  four)

  key concep ts of the theory

are  intimately l inked,  each of  them being  a func t ion o f  another.

T H E   SPACE  O F  L E A R N I N G

A s w e have a lready pointed ou t , nobo dy can discern a certa in feature w i t h -

out exp eriencing varia t ion in a d im ens ion corresponding to that fea ture . L et

us assume that tw o

 persons,

 A and B, are engaged  in a conversat ion w ith a

stranger, C , in a sm al l cot tage in a N epa le se m oun ta in v i ll age . A f t e rward , it

turns o u t tha t A had not iced that C spoke w i th a typica l southweste rn N epa-

lese dialect (a ctually all

 three

 o f them did so), w hile B did not notice this at

all.  W hy was it

 tha t

 B did not

 discern

 C 's

  d ia lec t w hereas

 A

 d id? W hat

 w as

varying, as far as dia lects are concerned? A s it was , no th ing var ied in the ac-

tual

  situation,

  but A had

  heard

  a lot of  different

  Nepalese dialects previ-

ously

  and he re la ted C's w ay of speaking to w ha t he had experienced and

not iced the s im ilarity w ith a pa r t icula r N epa lese d ia lec t , and the  difference

w hen compared

  to

 other

 Nepalese

 dialects.

 B , on the

 other han d,

 had

 never

been ou ts ide

 h is

 v i l lage ,

 had

 never w atched

 T V o r

 l i s tened

 to the

 radio,

 and

had actua l ly never heard any other dialect . He cou ld no t discern the d ia lec t

because he did not

 know

 of any

 other.

 T he

 variation exp erienced

  by A was a

resul t of the m em ories of past events that A brou gh t into the s i tuat io n, and

t hus a lso into his encoun ter w ith C .

Now let us

  assume that

  C

 speaks

 a

 different  dialect.

If this were

  the

 case ,

 A

 w ould probably not ice

 th e  fact, bu t

 also

 B

 wo u l d

not ice that

 C

 speaks

 in a w ay

 that

 is

 different

 from all the

 other people

 he has

ever

 heard  (he may even find it very

 difficult

  to understand  C). A t the  same

time,

 h e

 w ould probably becom e co nscious

 of the fact

  tha t

 he

 h imse l f

 has a

certain

 way of

  speaking.

In  the first

 example,

 the only variation w ith

 respect

 to dialects w as the

fac t

 th at A had had previous exp erience of hearin g other dia lects , and that

C's way of

  speaking

  w as

 juxtapo sed w i th th i s exper ience .

  In the

  second

example , there

 is

 variation

  to be experienced  in the

  actual s i tuat ion.

 T he

ins igh t th at there are

 di f ferent

  w a y s o f speaking can be derived  from  s i m -

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 34/255

1 .  T H E S P A C E O F  L E A R N I N G  21

p ly

  being exposed to them in this s i tuat ion.

 There

  is a space created for

gaining

 this

 insight in the second case.

 This

 space is created by  chal leng-

ing

  th e  taken-for-granted nature  of the

 experience

  o f people's  w a y s  o f

speaking

  in the

 person

  wh o h as

 previously only been exposed

  to one way

of speak ing .  Creat ing  a space  m eans opening u p a dime nsion of variat ion

(as

 compared

  to the

 taken-for-granted nature

  of the

 absence

  o f

 var ia t ion) .

As o u r exam ple i llus t rates , however , som eone w hose past experiences  a re

suf f ic ien t

  fo r

 pe rce iving

  th e

 necessary pat tern

  o f

 variat ion

  can

 exper ience

variat ion w i thou t that space ( i .e .,

 t h e

 necessary pat tern

 o f

 variat ion ) being

cons t i t u t ed in the

  imm edia te s i tua t ion .

 T he

 space  o f  learning  re fers

 to the

pat tern o f var iat ion inherent in a s i tuat ion as observed by the researche r .

This

 space

 is a

 necessary condition

 for the

 learner's experience

 of

 that pat-

tern of var iat ion unless the learner can experience that pat tern du e to w ha t

she has encou ntered in the pas t .

N ow let us consider another type of var iat ion. W hen w e con sider a mov-

ing

  body  for  example ,  w e  usu al ly look  at i t f rom the po in t  of view  of the

ground.

 W e can say

 that

 the

  ground

  is

 taken

  as

 a f rame

  of

  reference.

  But we

could

 also look

 a t the

 m oving body

  from  the

 po in t

 o f

 view

 of

 another

  f r ame

of

 r eference;

 from our own mo ving body ins tead o f the ground, for ins tance .

A nd if we do that , w e introd uce variat ion  in

  f rames

  of re ference . T he very

idea

  o f  f rames  of refere nce presuppo ses variat ion, as does the insight tha t

looking   at a body in movement f rom th e point of view  of  rest (the ground)

amo unts to

  adopt ing

 a

 par t icular  f ram e

  o f

 re ference .

In

 addit ion

 to

 looking

  at

 m oving bodies

  from the

 point

 of

 view

 o f

 rest ,

 w e

also  ha bitually look

 at

 them when they

 a re

 under

 the

 influence

 of the

 gravita-

t ional

  force  of the

 earth.

  So

 wi thout being aware

 of

 this ,

 we

 take

 the

 gravita-

tional force  for granted. In order to break this taken -for-granted natu re of our

awareness  of the  world, variation  in the  gravitational  force  mus t  be  intro-

duced.

  This can be done by traveling in a space shuttle, or more

  easily,

 by

looking

 a t

 pictures

 f rom a

 space shuttle,

 by

 engaging

 in a

 simu lation

 of the ef-

fects  of varying gravitat ional force  or sim ply carrying out a thoug ht experi-

ment .

 These tw o

 dimensions—gravity

  and frame of

 reference—form

  a space

of learn ing . A space of  learning

 com prises

 any

 number

 of

 dim ensions

 o f vari-

ation  and denotes the aspects of a situation, or the phenomena embedded in

that

 situa tion, that

 can be

  discerned

 due to the

 variation presen t

  in the

 situa-

t ion.

 V ariation that

 is not

 present

 in the

 situation

 can

 still

 be

 discerned, how -

ever,

 if variat ion is brought in by m eans o f the learner 's m em ory of previous

experience.

 W e

 should notice, here, that

 a

 space does

 n ot refer to the

 absence

of

 constraints,

 but to

  something actively constituted.

 It

 del im i ts w hat

 can be

possibly learned (in sense of discerning) in that pa rticular situation.

B ut w e are not interested in al l types o f variation. W e m ust look at the sit-

uation

 from the

 point

 o f

 view

 of a

 particular object

  of

 learning.

  And by do-

ing so we can   find  out

  whether

  or not it is

  possible

  for the

  learner

  to

appropriate that part icular object o f  learning  in that par t icular s i tuat ion.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 35/255

22

  M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

Th e  Object  of  L e a r n i n g

The object

  of

  learning  can be d efin ed by i ts cri tical fea tures, that is, the fea-

tures that m ust

 be

  discerned

  in

 order

  to

 const i tu te

 the

 m eaning a imed for .

The

 quest ion

  w e

 mu s t

  ask

 therefore,

  is to

 wh at ex ten t

 we f ind

 variation

  in

the relevant respects.  A s learners  can  only  discern  that which vanes,  w e

m ust look for the pattern of variat ion necessary for deve loping the required

capa bil i ty. U nless the re is variat ion in al l the respects corresp ondin g to the

cri t ical fea tures, that

 is, the

 necessary pattern

 of

 variat ion

 is

 presen t , this

 ca-

pabil i ty cannot

 be

 developed.

  W e a re

 thus ta lking about

 the

 necessary p at-

tern

  of

  variat ion.

  In

 order

  to

 develop

  a

  cer tain capabi l i ty ,

 the

  l earner mu s t

enco unter a cer tain p at tern of var iat ion, regardless of the arrange m ents for

teach ing

 tha t

 are

 m a de,

 a nd

 regardless

  of the w ay in

 wh ich learn ing

 is

 orga-

nized (e .g . , wh ether the learner par t ic ipates in an au the nt ic pract ice, or s it s

in a lecture h all with 300 students). The space o f learn ing tel ls us wh at i t is

possible

  to

  learn

  in a

 cer tain s i tuat ion.

In

 th e

 light

 of a

 specific object

 of

 learning ,

 the

 space

 of

 learning

 is a

 rather

specific

  character izat ion of the in teract ion in the classroom. As al ready

pointed out ,

 the

 object

 of

 learning

 is

 there,

 to

 beg in w i th ,

 as an  in tended o b-

ject

  o f  learning  as seen

  from

  the teacher 's perspective, then i t is somehow

realized  in the

 classroom

 in the form of a pa rt icular space of learning. This is

the  enacted object  o f  learning  as

  seen

  from  th e

 researcher ' s po in t

 of

 view,

const raining w ha t i s poss ib le to leam .

W e

 can see

 tha t

 the

  space

  of

 learning con st i tu ted

 in the

 classroom

  is the

enacted object

 of

 learning.

 This is how the

 object

 of

 l earn ing

 is

 con st i tu ted

in  the m ost con crete sense. A nd i t is this that m atters rega rding op portuni-

t ies to learn in school . F actors such as c urr icu lum , teach er ' s in te nt ion , and

so on, are

 m edia ted th rough

  the

 enacted object

 of

 l earn ing .

The way

  that students see, understand,

  and

 make sense

  of the

 object

 of

learning when

  th e

  lesson ends

  and

 beyond,

  is the  l ived object

  o f

  learning.

T he

  pedagogical s i tuat ion

 and

 w h a t

 the

 students

  actual ly

  learn

 can

  thus

 be

described

 in the

 same terms.

 So now we can describe

 learning

 and

  teaching

in

 relat ion to one another,  from  the point of view of learn ing, not as a rela-

t ionship

  between cause

  and

  effect ,

  but as a

  re la t ionsh ip be tween w hat

  is

m ade poss ib le and w hat poss ib i l i t ies are actu al ly m ade use of .

The focu s should thus be on learning  in the first instanc e and on  teaching

in the second; the

 focus

  should be on wh at shou ld be learned and w hat i s ac-

tual ly  learned.

 A nd

 w h a t

 is

 actua l ly learned should

 be

  understood

  in

 term s

of the con dit ions of learning . If learning is ac tua lly tak ing place in the class-

room,  w e should try to unders tand w hat the  students leam  in term s of wha t

is

  taking place

  in the

  classroom. Whatever takes place

  in the

  classroom

makes

  differing

  sense  to  different  students. This  is one of the  most solid

conclusions that  can be  drawn  f rom our own  research (Marton  &  Booth ,

1997)

 o r

 tha t

 of

 others.

 N o

 co ndi t ions

 of

 learn ing ever

 cause

 lea rning. They

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 36/255

1 .  T H E  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  23

only m ake i t possible for learners to learn c ertain things. A nd this , in our

view,

 is exact ly wha t pedagogy is about , especial ly the type of pedago gy of

learning that we are putt ing forward in this book,  that is,

 making

  learning

poss ible. Le t us consider tw o si tuations ( tw o classroom scenarios)

  from

  the

point of view of learning or developing a certain c apabil ity. W e m ight de-

scribe what i t is possible to learn with view of that capabil i ty in the two

classrooms; we might descr ibe what i s taking place in each classroom in

terms o f the possibilities for learning that are brought about, an d in terms of

w h a t kind o f space of learning i s con st i tu ted. The space

  of

  learn ing thus de -

pic ts  the possibil it ies of learning in relat ion to the ca pab il ity in qu estio n.

Th rough out th is book, w hen w e talk abo ut wha t i t i s poss ib le to learn, w e

restrict ourselves to the kind o f learn ing we h ave discussed in this chap ter,

namely learning to see certain things in certain w ays. But even bearing this

res t ric tion in m ind, there w i l l und oub tedly be a great num ber of th ings that

vary, and hence that w i ll or could be discerned. The space of learning does

not d enote al l that is possible to leam , even in the restricted sense of learn ing

(i.e., learning to see). The space of learning ca ptures o nly what i t is pos sible

to learn  in a situation

  from

 the point of view of wha t is m eant to be learned.

W hat i s m eant to be learned is the o bject of learning, wh ich in our case is

a c apab i l ity of seeing som ething in a cer tain w ay. A s we p ointed out ear lier,

a

 cer tain way o f seeing som ething can be character ized in term s of the as-

pec ts of a s i tuation or phenom enon that are discerned and focused on s imul -

taneously,  or more  precisely,  the  crit ical  aspects of the  situation  or the

phenomenon that are discerned and  focused  on s imul taneously . Now the

quest ion  is : How do we know w hat dimensions o f var iat ion w e should look

for?

 How do we know w hat cr i t ical features there  are for a certain class of

s i tuat ions

 to be seen in a cer tain w ay? How can w e character ize the nature of

a  certain capability?

C arlsson (1 999) investigated "the a natom y

 o f

 ecological u nders tanding,"

com pris ing, am ong other th ings ,

 th e

 unders tanding

 o f

 pho tosynthesis , recy -

cl ing,

 an d the con servation of energy. One of the cr it ical feature s— actua l ly

the  most cr i t ical  feature  of al l of these component capabil i t ies—was the

idea

 of

 transformation, that

  is ,

 that

 one

 thing turns into something  qualita-

tively different:  Sunl igh t and water are t ransform ed into carbohydrates a nd

oxygen in photosynthesis; material microscopic part icles   from  hum an be-

ings

 and

 an im als,

 and so on, are for

 instanc e rearrang ed into soil,

 and

 soil

 re -

arranged into plants; and on e

 form

 of energy, such a s hea t , can be turned into

and  stored  in solid or l iquid form through  th e conservation of  energy.

In  what sense

  is

  t ransformat ion

  a

  critical feature

  of

  ecological under-

standing

 and how can i t be  found  or discovered? Is it by m eans of co ntem-

plat ing  the  concept  o f  ecologica l unders tanding W ell, this w as  definitely

not

 the way in

 wh ich C arlsson  found

 it. In

 C arlsso n's study, this critical fea -

ture (transformation) appeared empirically, as a contrast to another

  frequent

w ay

 o f thinking that im plied that certain things were perceived a s consum ed

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 37/255

24  M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

(e.g., sunlight, water, dead  bodies, energy)

 and

  that other things were  per-

ceived

  as

 independen tly  produ ced (e.g., coal, oil ,

 or the

 chlorophyl l

 in f low-

ers and

 trees).

The law

 o f

 variation  applies thus

 to the

 research er 's

 work

 as

 w el l.

 It is not

possible to discern  a certain  way o f  th inking about something wi thout the

contrast

 o f other

 ways

 of

 th inking about

 the

 same thing.

 T he crit ical feature

is

 critical

 in

 dis t inguishing

 one way of

 th inking

 from

 another,

 an d is

 relative

to the  group participating  in the  study, o r to the p opu lat ion represented  by

the

 sample.

  F or

 instance ,

  if all the

 p art ic ipants

  in

  Carlsson's study

 had em-

braced the idea of transform ation, t ransform ation m ay never have shown i t-

self as a cri t ical feature. The cntical features have, at least in part , to be

found

  empir ical ly—for ins tance, through interviews wi th learners and

through the analysis of w hat i s happening in the classroom — and they also

have to be fou nd for every object o f learning

  specifical ly,

 becau se the cnt i -

cal features are cri t ical features of spe cific obje cts of learn ing.

Th e  Space  of  Le a r n in g  Is the  E n a c t e d  Object  of  L e a r n i n g

The enacted object  of  learn ing is the research er 's descr ipt ion of w hethe r , to

w hat extent

 an d in

 what fo rms ,

 the

  necessary condi t ions

 of a

 par t i cu lar

 ob-

jec t of learning appear in a certain set t ing. Th e ena cted obje ct of lear nin g is

t hus what w e have also called the space of learning, thereby de pict in g w ha t

is

 po ss ib le

  to

  l earn . W hat fo l low s from  th is l ine

 o f

 reasoning

  is  tha t

 w h e n

w e talk about learning, teaching , and other related mat ters, w e shou ld try to

be exp l ici t about w hat we h ave in our m inds as far as the quest ion of "W hat

is

  learned?"

  ( i .e. , "What should  be

  learned?"

  "What  can be  poss ib ly

learned?" "What

  is

 actu al ly learned?")

  is

  concerned.

In th i s way , the space o f l earn ing , w hich com pr ises different d imens ions

of variation, is con sti tuted by l ing uist ic m ean s in the inte rac tion between

teacher  and  students.

T H E

  L I NGUI S T I C

  CONSTITUTION

OF THE

 SPACE

  O F

  L EARNI NG

In

 the

 previous discuss ion,

 w e

 descnbed

 learn ing as the

 process

  o f

 coming

to experience the world in a certain way. W e put forw ard the thes is tha t  the

space o f learn ing is the space of variat ion, and that the dim ens ions of varia-

t ion that can

 be

 opened

 up and

 those tha t

 are

 actually  opened

  up

 contribute

to qua l i ta t ive di f ferenc es between the wa y in w hich so m ething

 c a n be

 expe-

r ienced an d the

 ac tua l

 way in

 w h i c h

 it

 is experienced.

 T he

 space

 o f

 l earn ing ,

therefore,

  is

 also

  an

  experiential space.

In talking abo ut  the space of learning as an experiential space, w e are re-

ferring

  to experience not as an in stan tiat ion , but rather as a po tentia l for ex-

periencing, seeing, and un der stan din g (see also   Hal l iday  & Mat th iessen ,

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 38/255

1 .

  T H E

  S P A C E

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  25

1999). It is in relation to this potential that learners can m ake sense of a par-

t icular object of learning. A s such , this space is elastic; i t can be w idened if

the teacher affords  learners opp ortunit ies to explore the ob ject of learnin g in

a variety of ways.

A nother thesis

 we put

 forward

 in

 this volum e

 is

 that language plays

 a

 cen-

tral

 role in the construal of experience,

 that

 it does not

 simply

 represent

 expe-

rience, as is widely perceived, but mo re imp ortantly, it constitutes experience.

Seen in

 this l ight, languag e plays

 a

 central role

 in

 learning,

 and

 understanding

how the learning experience is being con stituted by langu age is crucial to un-

derstanding

 how

 different  ways

 o f

 experiencing

 the

 object

 of

 learning

 are be-

in g

 brought about

 in the

  classroom.

L a n g u a g e   a n d t h e C o n s t r u a l o f

  E x p e r i e n c e

L et

 us clarify  w hat w e mean by langu age and the construal of experience.

The p osi t ion that w e are adopt ing is that the same phenom enon can be expe-

rienced in   qualitatively different ways,  and  that  the  different construal  of

the experience w il l be ref lec ted in the lang uage used. For exam ple , le t us

imagine that the jan i tor of a school locked the door o f a com puter laboratory

and

 tha t tw o teachers were t rying to open the door bu t co uld no t . Teacher A

said, "The

  jani tor

  locked

  the

 door,"  whereas Teacher

  B

  said, "The door

  is

locked." The same phenomenon was construed in  different  w ays by these

tw o

 teach ers . For Teacher A , the jani to r w as the point of departure o f the

message, whereas for Teacher B, the door was the point of departure .

Hence ,  for Teacher A, the m essage w as  about w ha t the jan i tor did: that is ,

that  he had

  locked

 the

  door.

 B y

  contrast ,

  for

 Teacher

 B, the

 m essage

  w as

about the state of the door, that is , that i t w as locked and therefore could no t

be opened.

The re la t ionsh ip between langu age and experience can be best seen by

examining   data  on  chi ld language development . Hal l iday's (1973, 1975)

seminal  work  on  Nigel ' s language development shows that  for ch i ldren ,

learning language

  is also learning

  about

  the

  world through language.

Hall iday  ( 1 993) observed that

Whe n c h i l d r e n lea rn langu age , they

 a re no t

  s imp ly engag ing

  in one

  type

  o f

  l ea rn ing

am ong m any; ra ther , they  a re learning the foun dat ions o f  learning itself . T he dist inc-

t ive

  cha rac te r i s t i c  o f

  human l e a rn ing

  is

  tha t

  it is a

  process

  o f

  m a k i n g m e a n i n g — a

semio t i c   process ;

 and the

  p ro to typ ica l f o rm

 o f

 h u m a n semio t ic

  is

 l a n g u a g e ,

 (p. 93)

A s

  the  child

  experiences

  th e

  world

  and as he  learns  how to  mean

(Hall iday,  1973), his m eaning p otent ia l is being recon st i tuted. The reconst i-

tut ion of his m eaning potent ia l  finds  its rea lization  in the w ay the chi ld re-

constructs his

  "grammar"

  so that i t eventually shares the conventions of

adult  gramm ar.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 39/255

26   M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N ,

 T S U I

Let us take for exam ple chi ld lang uag e developm ent . A t an early s tage

of language deve lopment , the ch i ld ut ters words as "annotat ions  of experi-

ence"

  (H alliday 1993,

  p .

 99).

 For

  example ,

  on

  seeing

  a

  round objec t ,

 a

Cantonese speaking chi ld w il l say "bo bo"

  [ > .

 $.] (b a l l ), no m a t te r whe the r

it   is a round doorknob, a balloon, or a basketball .  A s adul t s comm ent  on

the

 chi ld 's ann ota t ions

 b y

 say ing , "Yes, it 's

 a

 bal l"

 o r

 "No, th a t ' s

 not a

 ba l l ;

it 's a

 doorknob,"

 they a re c lass i fy in g th ings for the ch i ld . ( In Can tonese ,

"bo" [ j& ] (bal l) is a generic term for a ll kinds of ba l ls . ) W hen the ch i ld ap-

propriates the

  adul t c lass i f ica t ion

 of a

 round o bjec t

 as "a

  ball ,"

 he is

 also

implic i t ly c lassi fying what  is not a  bal l .  In  Hal l iday's

  terms,

  he is

"outclassing" (1 993,

 p.

 99).

2

In other words, the chi ld experiences varia tion am ong the round objects

that he sees: Objec ts tha t a re round and he can p lay w i th h i s m om m y are

called

 "balls,"

  and

  objects that

  are

  round

  and are

  stuck

  on

  doors

  are

  "not

balls." This

 experienced

  variation

  is

 realized

  in the

  language that

 th e

 child

uses

  to

 m a k e

 a

 dis t inct ion between  different  k inds

 o f

 rou nd ob jects . Later ,

the child experiences variation within the class of "balls."  He experiences

balls that can be blown up into big on es, are

  l ight,

 and can nse into the air,

and those that cannot. This kin d of "ball" is called a "balloon." A t this stage,

the

 child

  is

  experiencing variation

  in the

 dimension

  of

 "ball,"

 and

 there

 are

three values in this dim ension : "ball," "balloon," and "not ball." W ith this

further

  dist inction, the  ch i ld 's seman t ic sy stem  of "ball" changes .  It is no

longer a system w ith two term s, but w ith three term s. The m eaning of "ball"

changes

 as

 we l l.

 "Ball" no

 long er ju st means "not doorknob";

  it

 m eans "not

balloon"

 a s w ell.

The re la t ionship between langu age and experience is dia lect ic . The ex-

perienced variation enables

 the

 chi ld

 to

 discern

 th e

 d is t inc t ion s tha t

 are

 real-

ized  in  languag e , and the  l inguis t ic d is t inc t ion enables the ch i ld to discern

the variation. A s Ha llida y (1 978) observed, in this process:

th e

  construal  of  reali ty  is inseparable

  from

  th e  construal  of the  semantic

  sys tem

 in

wh ich the reali ty is

 encoded.

 In

 this sense, l anguage

 is a shared

 m ean ing

 poten tial , at

once both a part  of exper ience and an  inter-subjective

  interpretation

  of

 experience,

(pp. 1-2)

This m eaning potent ia l

 is

 being reco nsti tuted every t im e

 the

 ch ild experi-

ences language

 in

 use,

 and

  experiences what

 he can do

 w i th l anguage .

L a n g u a g e   an d  Di s t i nc t i o ns

In order to m ake sense of w hat w e have experienced, w e need to be able to

reduce the in defin i te ly varied phenom ena of the world into a mana geable

number o f phenomena  of sim ilar types.  A s  Britton (1970) pointed out, o b-

jec ts in the wo rld do not present them selves as readi ly

 classified.

 T he c lassi-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 40/255

1 .

  T HE  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  27

f icat ions are given by hum ans in order to handle the world, and language is

a

 principal

  m e a n s

 o f doing this . W e say "a principal means" because there

are certain class ificat ion s that

 can be

 done w i thout languag e .

 F o r

 example ,

if

 someone

 is asked to class i fy all the clothes in the w ardrobe,  he could pu t

all

 the w inter clothes in one pile, all the

 w ork

 clothes in ano ther pi le, and all

the

 form al evening clothes in a third pile. However, in the m ajori ty o f cases,

it  is not  possible  to  m a k e

  class i f icat ions wi thout language.

  F or

  example ,

how

 does  one

 dis t inguish betw een

  a

 m ale hum an being

  who i s

 related

 by

blood

  to

 one's  father

 and one who i s

 not ,

 by

 nonl inguis ti c m eans?

A s

  Sapir (1961) pointed out ,

.. .  language

 is

 pr im ari ly

 a

 vocal actualization

 of the tendency to see

 realities symbo l i-

cal ly  ... an

 actualization

 i n

 terms

 of vocal express ion of the tende ncy to

 master

 reality

not b y direct and ad hoc hand l ing of

 this

 elem ent but by the reduc tion of experien ce to

famil iar

  forms,  (pp. 14-15)

Adopting a similar stance to Sapir, Hall iday (1978) proposed that

Language

 has to in te rpre t t he

 w h o l e

 of our

 exper ience ,

 r ed uc ing the indef in i t e ly var-

ied p henom ena o f the wor ld

 a round

 us , and

 also

 of the

 wor ld

  inside us, the processes

of

 our own co nsc iousness , to a

 manageab le

 num ber o f

 c lasses

 o f

 phenom ena : types

of

 proces ses, events

 and  ac t ions , classes o f

 objects ,

 peop le and  ins t i tu t ions , and the

l ike.  (p . 21)

W e

 po in ted o ut earl ier  in this chapter tha t the contradiction between  the

unl imi ted  and

  inexhaus tib le am ount

 of

 inform at ion avai lable

 to us an d the

l imi ted capaci ty

 of the

 hum an mind requi res tha t

 we be

 select ive

 in

 wha t

 w e

attend to. Th e th ings that w e at tend to are th ings tha t w e discern as cr i t i ca l ,

not in a

  general sense,

  but

  cr i t ical

 in

 relat ion

  to a

 cer tain co ntext .

 F or

 th is

reason ,

 the

  dist inctions

 or

 c lassi f ica t ions

  that

 w e

 ma k e

 in the

 process

 o f re-

ducing the  indefini tely  varied phenom ena  of the  world into  a m a n a g e a b le

n u m b e r

 o f

 classes

 o f

 phenomena

  a re

 n ecessar i ly select ive.

Let us

 take

 fo r

 example

 the way

 th ings

 are

 class i f ied

 in

 different cu l tu res .

The fam ou s B ri ti sh anthropologis t Mal inow ski (1946) observed that societ-

ies

  classify  thei r surroundings according

 to

  thei r needs

  and

  interests ,

 and

that

  th is is  done through language. F or  example ,  in M al inowski ' s s tudy of

pr imit ive

 l anguages ,

 i t was

  found  that

 the

 indigen ous people

  o f a

 p r imi t ive

communi ty t ended

  to

  identify

  and

  different iate

  the few

  objects that were

use fu l

  to them an d the rest were treated as an undifferen tiated  heap . A p l an t

or a tree in a fo res t that was not connected to them t radi t ional ly or ri tually, o r

that

 w as not  usefu l  to

 t hem w ou ld

 be

  s im ply dismissed

 as "a

 bush."

  A

 bird

tha t p l ayed

 no

 part

 in

 the ir tradition

 o r was not

 part

 o f

 their diet w ou ld

 be re-

ferred

 to as "just a flying animal" (1946, p . 331). How ever,  if an object w as

useful ,

  it

 w o u ld

 be

 n a me d

 an d its

 uses

 and

 propert ies described

 in

 detai l , that

is, it

 w o u l d

 be

  dis t inct ly individua l ized.

 T he  fact

  that trees

  and

 b i rds were

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 41/255

28

  M A R T O N ,

  R U N E S S O N ,  T S U I

not nam ed separately for their individual appearance s does not m ean that

the people o f th is com m uni ty could not see that th e trees o r b i rds were phys-

ical ly different .  I t s im ply m eans that to them the di fferenc es were not so cr i t-

ical in

 relat ion

  to the

  specif ic

  contex t

 o f

  their everyday l ives

 as to

 warran t

m aking di s t inc t ions

 b y

  l inguis t i c m eans .

On e of the

 m ost widely quoted examples

 is

 that Esk im os have seven wo rds

fo r

 "snow,"

 each of which m akes very fine distinctions between  th e size of the

flakes of snow  to indicate ho w heavy the snow is. Th e Sam i l anguage has al-

mos t  200 w ords  for "snow," a nd each word indicates the con di tion of snow,

that is,

 whether

 one can

 w a l k

 on it, or ski on it,

 w hat tem perature

 it is,

 whether

it

 w i l l change quic kly

 and how

 (see V uolab, 2000). L angu age, therefore,

 en-

codes

  the

 dist inctions m ade

  and the

 c ri tical features

 of the

  dist inctions;

 th e

distinctions

 are

 app ropriated

 and

 m ain ta ined

 so as to

 ma k e

 th e

 di scernmen t

 of

the cri t ical features possible.

Another example

  is the

  central i ty

  of the  fami ly  and the

  impor tance

  o f

rank according   to gen erat ion in the Chinese cu l tu re . This  is ref lec ted  in the

complex kinship terms that make very   fine  d i s t i nc t i ons be tween w he the r

one is

 related

 to the

  fa the r ' s s ide

 or the

 m othe r ' s s ide,

 and w ho is

 related

 to

w h o m

  in the  f ami ly

  tree.

 These

  dis t inct ions

 a re

  cr i t ical

 in

 Ch inese cu l t u re

because  they indicate the place that one occupies in the   fami ly tree,  and

hence

 the

 status

 in the f ami ly ( in the

 sense

 of the

 ex tended

 fami ly) .

 Because

of  these dis t inct ions , it is possible  to  have a  s i tua t ion  w h e re a ve ry youn g

person   has a  h igher rank than somebody  who i s  con s iderab ly o lder,  and

therefore  the younger

 person

 i s addressed w i th a kinsh ip term that indicates

he or she is

 zhang  b e i ( P u t o n g h u a )

 [ -^ j£ ],

 t ha t

 is, of a

 m ore senior genera-

t ion

  in the

  extended  family .

Dist inct ions that have been m ade

  to

 su it socia l purposes

  a re not

 only

 e n-

coded

  in the

  l anguage

 bu t

 a lso ma in ta ined

 b y

 m e a ns

 o f

  l anguage .

 I n

 other

words,

  th e

 categories

  se t up, and

 hence

  th e

 di s t inc t ions m ade

 by

 language ,

no t

  only express

  the

  social s tructure

 but

  also create

  the

 need

  fo r

 people

  to

conform   to the behavior a ssociated w i th these categories . The com plex kin-

ship term s

 in

 Chinese

 a re a

 real izat ion

 a s

 w e l l

 as a

 me a n s

 o f

 m a in t a in in g

 the

hierarchical human relat ions

  in

  Chinese societ ies.

  A s

  Hal l iday  (1978)

pointed out , "By thei r everyday a cts of m ean ing, people act out the social

structure,  aff i rming  thei r ow n statuses and roles, and es tabl ishing and trans-

mit t ing  th e

  shared system

 of

 value

 a nd

 know ledge"

  (p. 8).

N ot

 only

 are

 distinctions m ade

 to

 suit social

 purposes,

 they

 a re

 also m ade

to serve spe cific nee ds at par t icu lar m om ents in t im e . Let us use, for e x a m-

ple, color dist inc tions . Th e dist inction betwe en brigh t red and dark red is

commonp lace

  and

 mos t people w ould

 be

 able

 to

 di s t ingu ish

 a

 bright

 red car

from a dark red car. Suppose you w i tnessed an acciden t between a dark blue

car and a br ight red car and you were asked by the pol ice to provide an eye-

witness account

 of

 w hat happened.

 Y ou

 probably w ould

 say

 som ething l ike

this:

 "A red car was

 going

 a t full

  speed when

 a

 b lue

 car

 c o mi n g

 from  the op-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 42/255

1 .  T H E

  S P A C E

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  29

posite direct ion suddenly swerved into the opposite lane and coll ided

head-on wi th

 the red

 car."

 Y ou

 w o u ld

 not

 take

 the

 trouble

 to say

 "the bright

red car" and the "dark blue car" because the distinction between and bright

and

 dark colors  is not c ri t ical . How ever, if it w as a bright  red car co l liding

wi th  a dark red car, then the dist inction between dark red and bright red

w o u l d be cr i tical . Of cou rse, if there w as o nly one car , there w ou ld be no

nee d to

 describe

 the

 color

 at

 al l .

 For

 exam ple,

 i f you saw a car

 crashing into

 a

supermarket , your eyewitness account to the police  would be  something

l ike

 th is : "I saw a car crashing into th e supermarket at  10:00 a.m . this m orn-

ing,"

 but not "I saw a

 bright

  red car

 crashing into

 the

 supermarket

  at

 10:00

a.m." be ca us e the co lor of the car is not c ri tical in relat ion to the e vent or the

contex t .

 C ri t ical to the event or the con text is w ha t happe ned.

In

  the  contex t  of a  learning si tuation, such  as the

 classroom,

  the  lan-

g u a g e

 th a t is use d by the t eache r and the s tude nts to m ake di s t inc t ions in

re la t ion

 to the

 ob j ec t

 o f

 learn ing

 is

 often

  o f

 cr i t ica l imp or tance .

 L et us use

an e x a mp l e

 f rom

  a lesson tha t w i l l be repor ted  in greater detai l in chap t e r

5.

 A sc ienc e t eacher , in the contex t o f teach ing how a reed re lay

3

 ope ra te s ,

asked the  s tudents what happened to the electrical resistance va lue w hen

l i g h t  w as  shone  o n a  l ight-diode res is tor

4

  ( L D R ) .  O n e  s tudent rep l ied ,

"Sm al l. " W hen the s tudent used the adjec t ive

 "small,"

 she was m ak ing a

dis t inc t ion b e t w e e n  th e  size of the res i s tance in the LD R be fo re and  af ter

the

  l igh t

 w as

  shone

  on the

 L D R .

 In

 o ther w ords ,

 she was

 descr ib ing res is -

tance as a state. T he  teacher, however,  w as no t happy w i th  the  descript ion

b e c a u s e   he r ques t i on was "What  happened to the res i s tance?" In o ther

w o r d s ,  she w anted the s tuden ts to descr ibe the chang e of s t a te cause d by

l i gh t sh in ing on the LDR , and "small" was therefore no t an ap propr ia te an-

swer .

  S he

 ins i s ted tha t

 the

  s tudent

 use the

 verb

 "decreases"

 i n s t ead

 of the

adjec t ive "small." This t eacher w as no t n i tp i ck ing , bu t was m a k in g an im-

por tan t

 di s t inc t ion be tween  a state

  and

  a c h a nge

  o f

 state.

 In

 o ther words ,

the two   s ta tes were

  c lass i f ied

  as two  dif ferent  p h e n o m e n a .  In  o ther con -

t ex t s , such

 a

 di s t inc t ion

 m a y n o t

 have been imp or tan t

 a t

 a l l ,

 but in the

 c o n -

text

 o f un der s tand ing a p h e n o me n o n  in phys ics , spe c i f i ca l ly the ope ra t ion

of a ree d relay, th is w as an im portant dis tinct ion (see chap. 5 for a m ore de-

ta i led

  exp l ica t ion of the reed re lay) .

Let  us  take another example  from  a  classroom learning s i tuat ion. T w o

H o n g  Kon g primary teachers were teaching lessons about  festivals.

 O ne o f

the

  objects

  o f

  l earn ing

 in

 both lessons

  w as to

 he lp

  the

  s tudents

 to

 indicate

their p references for the festivals, usin g th e phrase

 "like best."

 Both teachers

gave  thei r s tude nts the sam e l ist o f

 festivals.

 Teacher A asked the s tudents to

first  indicate

  the

  festivals

  that they l iked and  then indica te w hich one they

l iked bes t .

 B y

 doing this ,

 th e

 teacher opened

 up a

 dim ension

 o f

 variat ion

 in

indicat ing preferen ces that included "like"

 an d

 "like best,"

 an d

 p resupposed

th e

 choice of "do not like." In other w ords, in the sem antic system of

 "like,"

there we re three choices : "do not like,"

 "like,"

 and "like best." B y j ux t apos -

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 43/255

30  M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

ing

 "like" a nd

 "like

 best," the teacher  w as able to help  the students experi-

ence variation in their fee ling s towa rd these  festivals an d thereby discern the

meaning

  of

  "like

 best"  as the

  superlative form

  of

  "like."

  B y

  contrast ,

Teacher

  B

 made

  no

 such dist inction.

  S he

 sim ply asked

  her

 students

 to

 indi-

cate what they "liked best." Consequent ly , some  of the s tudents in this class

took  "like

 best" a s

 having

 a

 s imi lar m eaning

  to "like."

La n gu a ge

  an d

  Structure

  of

 Awareness

In our discuss ion of s im ul tanei ty and aw areness, w e pointed out that aware-

ness is the total i ty of our experience of the w orld an d that w e experien ce ev-

ery

  s i tuat ion against

  th e

  b a c k g r o u n d

  o f a

  vas t number

  o f

  p rev ious

experiences . W e fur ther pointed ou t that the c harac ter is t ic of hu m an aware-

ness is that only a limited num ber of thing s com e to the

 fore

 and becom e fo-

cused, whereas

  the

  rest recedes

  to the

  background.

  W e

  observed that

  the

f igure-ground s tructure of awareness i s dyn am ic and ever ch an gin g.

In the discuss ion o f language and the const rual of experience, we pointed

out that the enc oding o f previous ex perience by  l inguis t ic m eans enab les us

to m ake  sense of w hat we are experiencing. W e w ish to  further  argue tha t

language also plays

 an

 im portant role

 in not

 only represe nt ing

 the

 structure

of

 aw areness

  but

 also

  in

 changing

 it . Let us

 take

 the

 previou s ly c i t ed exam -

ple of the janitor locking the door of the computer laboratory. If we say.

"The janitor locked

  the

 door,"

 the

 jan i to r becom es

  the

  t hem e

 or

 sub jec t

 o f

the  clause  and is the grounding  for the rest of the clause . That  is.

  what

  the

janitor d id w as the figu re. How ever, if w e say, "The door w as locke d by the

janitor," the

 fact

  that the door w as locked becom es the ground, and the ja n i-

to r becomes  the f igure .

In  classroom situations,  it is very imp ortant

 that

 the teacher is able to bnng

critical fe ature s of the object of learning into s tuden ts'

  focal

 aw areness. That

is to

 say,

 it is

 crucial that

 th e

 teacher

  is

 able

 to

 bring

 out the figure and

 ground

relationsh ip. This  is often  achieved through  linguistic means, and m ost obvi-

ously through

  the use of

 questions because

  th e

 structure

 of all

 quest ions

 in-

volves

  a f igure-ground

  relationship.

  For

 exam ple,

  the

  often  ci ted question,

"Have  you stopped beating your w ife?" presupposes that yo u have been beat-

in g

 your wife

 and

 queries whe ther

 y ou

 have stopped

 th e

 action

 or

 no t .

 In

 other

words, "beating yo ur w ife"

 is

 ground,

 a nd

 "have

 you stopped" is figure.

 W h at

th e

  teacher presents

  as

 ground

  is

 wha t

 he

  assumes

  to be

  shared knowledge,

and wh at he presents as f igure is w hat he w ants to be the  focus o f the students '

attention.  For exam ple,  let us see how

 different

  questions asked  by  different

teachers can result in different  learn ing experiences  for students ( this exam ple

is presented in m ore detail in chap. 5). Tw o primary E ng lish teachers in Hong

Kong were teaching

 the

 determ iner "some"

 to

 indicate inexac t quanti ty. Both

teachers used  O ld

 M acDonald

  's

 Farm as the

  context

  for

 teaching,

 and

  both

showed  th e students a series of pictures with  different  kinds of animals and

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 44/255

T H E

  S P A C E

  O F  L E A R N I N G

different

  numbers of animals on the computer screen. Both teachers also

started by showing one anim al and asking the students w hat they c ould see so

as

 to

 elicit

 th e

 n a me

 of the

 anim al shown.  After  this,

 the first

 teacher repea t-

edly

  asked

  th e

  students, "How many (name

  of

  animal)

  can you

 see?"

 This

elicited exact num bers, such as

 "three,"

 "four," "five," and so on. The second

teacher, how ever, repeatedly asked the students, "What can you

 see?"

 and did

not ask

 "How many?"

 The

 question "How m any cats

 can you see?"

 presup-

poses that the an ima ls that the students can see are cats and queries the exact

num ber of cats . Hence, the cats are the ground and the num ber of cats the fig-

ure.

  This question  focuses  the students' attention on the exact number. The

question

 "What can you see?" presupposes that the students can see some-

th ing

 (on the

  computer screen)

  and

 queries

 "what"

 it is

 that they

 can

  see.

 In

other

 words,

 the

 exact number

 of

 animals they

 can see is not the f igure.

 Their

focus  of

 attention

  is on

 whether there

  is

 only

 one or

 more than

  one

 animal .

The students ' performance in a test administered at the end of the lesson

showed that

 the

 students taugh t

 by the first

 teache r

 did

 less we ll w hen u sing

the

 determiner

 "some"

 to indicate inexact qu an tity than the students taug ht by

the

  second teacher.

T H E

  SEMANTIC DIME N SIO N

OF THE  SPACE  O F  L EARNI NG

S o

 far ,

 w e

 have focuse d

 on the

 necessary co ndi t ions

 f o r

 learning,

  the

 nec-

essary   con di t ions being the discernm ent of cr it ical features of the objec t

o f

 l earn ing th roug h expe r iencing dimens ions of var ia t ion re la t ing to th i s

speci f ic  ob j ec t

  o f

  l earn ing bo th diachronica l ly

  an d

  synchronica l ly ,

  and

h o l d i n g

  these c r i t ica l fea tures

  in

  focal  awareness s imul taneous ly . How-

ever , these necessary con di t ions

 m ay no t be

  suf f ic ien t

  fo r

 pow erfu l l earn-

ing to

 t ake p l ace .

Let u s

 take

 fo r

 exam ple some science lessons that

 wi l l be

 explained

  in de-

tai l  in chapter 6. Two science teachers e xplained  the process  o f

  neutraliza-

tion   to their students. Both explained that  neutralization  is a reaction

between acid

 a nd

 alkali that results

 in a pH

 value

 of 7.

 Both

 o f the

  teachers

used

  laboratory demonstrat ions

  to

 show w hat

 happens

 when neutral izat ion

occurs , by us ing a chang e of the color of the solution as the m eans of m aking

the

  process visible. Both varied

  the

 prop ort ion

  of

 acid

  and

 alkali ,

  and the

chang e of pH

  value

 to

  indicate

  the

  levels

 of

 acidity

 and

  alkalinity,

 to

 help

students  understand the concept of neutral izat ion. However, one teacher

used many everyday examples that were   familiar  to the students to help

them make sense

  of

  neutral izat ion (e.g. ,

  the use of

  alkaline ointment

  to

sooth the i tchiness caused by a cids injected into the skin by m osquitoes, a nd

the use o f alkaline toothpaste to neu tral ize the acidic sal iva w h e n  cleaning

our

 teeth) .

 The

 other teacher, however, dealt w ith neu tral ization very m uch

as

 a proc ess of m ixing ac ids and alkalis in a laboratory experim ent. B y using

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 45/255

32  M A R I O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

examples that  the  students  were  able  to  relate  to  easily,  the  first  teacher

m ade available possibili ties  for students  to assign  a m u c h richer mean ing to

the  object o f learning. F o r these students, neu tralization w as no longer ju s t a

process

 that

 took place

 in the

 laboratory,

 b ut a

 also

 a

 process

 tha t

 had a

 p lace

in   their daily lives.

T he

 space

 o f

 learning

  is

 const i tuted therefore

 no t

 only

 by the

 possibi l i t ies

for  discernmen t of cr it ica l features of the object of learnin g, of ten  b rough t

about

  by

  l inguist ic means,

  bu t

 also

 by the

 examples

  and

 ana log ies tha t

 the

teacher uses,

  the

  stories tha t

  the

  teacher tells,

 the

  contexts that

 the

 teacher

brings

 in, and so o n. I t is

 constituted

 by the

 me anin gs th at learners assign

  to

these examples, analogies, stories,

 and

 contexts ,

 as

 w e ll

 as by the

 previous

experience that they bring

  in as

 they

 try to

 m ake sense

 of the

 object

 of

 learn-

ing. A ll of this const i tutes the sema ntic dimen sion o f the space of l ea rn ing in

w hich the cr i tica l features of the object of learning are interpreted and un-

derstood.

  A

  space

  o f

  learning that

  is

  semant ica l ly

  rich

  a l lows s tudents

 to

come

  to

 grips wi th

 th e

 crit ical features

 of the

 object

 o f

 l ea rn ing m uch m ore

effectively

  than one that is semantical ly impover ished.

T H E   SHARED SPACE

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

In order  for us to discern cr i t ica l features of the ob jec t in q u e s t i o n , w e need

to be able to make sense o f w h a t w e are exper ienc ing in re la t ion to w h a t we

have

 experienced

 before; this is the d iachronic d im ension o f our aw areness .

A s we poin ted out before , w e cannot ta lk about ta l lness un less w e have a lso

exper ienced shor tness; w e canno t say wh at br igh t red is un less w e have ex-

per ienced other shades of red. In other wo rds, w ha t learners have expe r i-

enced before

 is

 crucia l

 to how

 they m ake sense

  o f

 the i r cur rent expenen ce .

In

 order

 to

 m a k e

 i t

 possible

  fo r

 learners

 to

 discern

 cri t ical feature s

 of the ob-

j e c t o f learning, teachers mu st be aware o f w hethe r lea rners c an make sense

of

  these  cr i t ica l features through their previous exper ience.

  T he

  teacher

m ust a lso be aware of how m u c h is shared be tween h im se l f and the learners.

In other words, w hat the teacher presents a s ground should b e  shared com -

m on

  ground between h im se l f

 and the

 s tudents ,

  so

 tha t w ha t

 he

 presents

  as

f igure can be made sense of by the s tuden ts in re la t ion to the ground. In this

sense,  the  space  o f  learning  is a  shared space  o f l e a rn ing .

Lan guag e is the key m eans by w hich th is shared space is con st i tuted. W e

have

 i l lustra ted this poin t in the previous discussion on teach ers ' quest ion s.

The fo l lowing is a further  exam ple . In a s tudy of some m athem at ics lessons

of Brazi l ian kindergar ten chi ldren, in wh ich the teacher was tea ch ing addi-

tion and the concepts

  o f

  m o r e and

 less,

 Cestan  (2001)

  found

  that the use of

unclear deic tic reference and the lack of an explic i t indica t ion as to wh ich

rea lm the teacher was m oving in confused the s tudents and made them un-

able

  to

 answer

  her

  quest ions.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 46/255

1 .

  T HE  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  33

The lesson was a mathematics lesson at kindergarten level and the stu-

dents w ere around  5 or 6 years old. T he teacher w as  teaching addit ion and

v/as helping the s tudents to unders tand not only addi tion but also the m athe-

mat i ca l

  concepts

 o f

 more

  and

  less.

T he

  teacher

 put

  three boxes

  on one

  side

 and tw o

 boxes

  on

 another.

 S he

then asked the s tudents whether there were the same number of boxes on

both s ides , w hich s ide had m ore, and wh ich s ide had less . The teacher had

no prob lem s get t ing the s tudents to indicate that the s ide wi th three box es

had m ore. However , w hen she asked the s tudents about the s ide w i th two

boxes ,  confus ion

  set in. The  fol lowing  is an

 excerpt

  f rom the

  lesson:

1.1

 [Cestan,

  2001]

5

  T :

  W hich s ide

 has

  m ore?

6 S: 3

7

  T :

  (The side)

 of how

 m a n y?

8 Ss :

  O f 3

9

  T :

  Which

 has 3. A nd

  this side, w hich

 has 2?

10  S: 2.

11

  T:

  H m m ?

12  S: 2.

13  T:  This side has m ore or less? (po inting to the side with 2 boxes)

14  S:  M o r e

15  T:

  H m m ?

16  Ss:

  M o r e

Ss:

  Less

17

  T : W hy

  less?

 It is

 becau se there

  are

 only

  .. .

18  Ss:  M o r e

19

  T: 2, and here there are m ore beca use there are . ..

20   Ss:  Less

21   T: Here there are m ore becau se how m any are there?

22   So me

  Ss: 3

23

  T: 3, then 3 is bigge r than . . . ?

24 S: 2

In

 line

 9 ,

 w h e n

 the

 teache r asked

 the

 que stion, "And this side, w hich

 h as

2?" one s tudent s imply reported the num ber of boxes on the other s ide ( line

10), and the same answer w as given when the teacher asked for the answ er

again w ith an

 interrogative

 interjection

 (line

  11). The teacher then repeated

her

 quest ion

 in a

 sl ightly fuller  form that included

 the

 q uant i ta tive com para-

tive no t ion

 "more

 or

 less."

 She

 also g ave

 a

 more expl icit indicat ion

 of

 w h ic h

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 47/255

34  M A R IO N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

side  she was referring  to by  point ing  to the  s ide w ith tw o  boxes ( l ine 13).

This,

 how ever, did not meet with m uch success. T he c lass w as sp l i t between

"more" an d "less" (see lin e 16). In line  1 7, even thou gh the teacher provided

the l inguist ic sc affoldin g to help the s tudents to

 jus t i fy

  why one s ide was

less than the other side, and tr ied to get them to fill  in the b lank w i th

 "two."

she still faile d to elicit the correct answ er.  I t was not un t i l sh e made the j u s t i -

fication  very explic i t by s ta t ing the qu anti ta t ive com parat ive no t ion "more"

and the number "how many are there" ( line 19 and line 21) tha t she

  f inal ly

succeeded  in get t ing th e  correct answer.

Cestari (2001)  ident i f ied  the source of confus ion  as em ana t ing from the

absence   o f a

 defini te

  reference in the incom ple te in te rroga t ive in l ine 9, and

observed

 that even tho ug h the teacher t r ied to repair the com m un icat io n gap

by

 point ing

  to the

 s ide w ith

 tw o

 boxes

  and

 exp l i c i tly a sk ing wh e the r

 i t had

more   or less , the con fus ion rem ained. Th e  confus ion  has very m uc h to do

with the  fact  that the teacher was t rying to establ ish a correspondenc e be-

tween  the  n u m b e r  o f  boxes  and the  m a t h e m a t i c a l n o t io n  o f "more"  and

"less,"  and the  fact  there w as a lack of com m on ground, or m utu a l i ty , be-

tween her and the s tudents with regard to whether she was moving in the

realm of quant i ty of boxes, or in the rea lm of the m ath em at ica l no t ion of

"more"  and "less."  In line 9, w hen the teache r asked the cryp tic qu est io n

"And th is side , which h as 2?" she was m oving in the rea lm of the m athe m at-

ical notion

 o f

 "more"

 and

 "less,"

 and

 t rying

 to ge t the

 s tudents

 to

 ex t rapola te

f rom the ques tion w hat she wan ted them to te ll he r, w hich was w hether the

side with tw o boxes  had more  or  less boxes than the s ide w ith three boxes.

The students , how ever, were m oving in the realm of the qua nti t y of box es.

W hen the teacher then tr ied to get the studen ts to ju s t i fy wh y one s ide had

less , she was m oving in the realm of the qua nti ty of boxes and w as t rying to

get the  s tudents  to com ple te  the ju s t i f ica t ion  by  s u p p l y i n g the n u m b e r o f

boxes ( l ine

  17 and

 lin e 19). Yet,

 the

 s tudents were m oving

 in the

 r ea lm

 of the

m athemat ica l not ion  o f "more" or "less." I t was on ly when she m a d e the re-

la t ionship e xplic i t between  the m a thema t ica l no t ion o f  m o r e and the  quan-

tity

  of boxes ( l ine 21) and said,

 "Here

 there are m o r e because

 ho w

 m a n y are

there?" that

 the

 students w ere able

 to

 answer

  the

 ques t ion . This exam ple

 il -

lustra tes nicely that unless the teacher and the learners share a common

ground  in relation to the ob jec t o f lea rn ing , it is no t po ss ib le for the learners

to   make sense of the object  o f  learning.

There

 is a fur the r sense in wh ich the space of lea rnin g is a shared space.

W e have a lready pointed  ou t th at previous exper ience  is the  f r ame  o f refer-

ence against which current experience is interpreted or made sense of , but

that the current experience a lso mo dif ies the way in w hic h the previous ex-

perience

  is construed. This  is an ever-changing and interact ive process in

which

 the

 features that

 w e

 discern

  in a

 situation

 and

 w h a t

 w e

 discern

 as

 criti-

cal keep being revised.

  O ne

 crucia l mean s

 in

 wh ich th i s

 is

 done

 is by

 t a l k i n g

over events w ith people an d by ref lect ing on the events  after  they have oc-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 48/255

T H E

  S P A C E

  O F  L E A R N I N G

curred (B ritton,  1 970) . L angu age n o t only plays a crucial

 role

 in the m odif i-

cation

  o f

 each  other 's un derstanding

  and

 perception

  of the

 world

  bu t

  also

becom es part  of the exper ience itself. T he m odif ica t ion of the learners '  and

teacher 's un derstanding

  of the

 w orld

  is

 w hat c lassroom teaching

  and

  learn-

ing is all

 about . This

 process is

 brough t about joint ly

 by the

 teacher

  and the

learners through interaction   in  which meaning  is neg otia ted  and  co-con-

structed. In this sense, th e space of learning  is a shared sp ace of learning. In

chapter 6 , we see how the s tudents m odify each

 other's

 understanding  of the

wor ld by engaging in collaborative talk. In chapter 7, we see how the teacher

and  the students togeth er co-construct an understan ding of the funct ion  of a

"clan"

 (a

 comm uni ty

 in

 w h i ch

 a ll

 m embers have

 the

 same surname)

  in a

 his-

tory lesson .

TOWARD A

  P E D A G O G Y

  O F

 L E A RN I N G

The expression  pedagogy  o f  learning  may strike the reader as vaguely odd.

Pedagogy is about teaching and upbringing after all; and teaching and upbring-

ing is m uch about learning, so is the pedagogy of learning not a tautology?

Well ,

  it could have been, but it is not . W e set out at the beginning  of this

chapte r

 by

 poin t ing

 to the

 fact that discussions abo ut pedagogy are,

 a s a

 ru le ,

not

 phrased in terms of learning but in term s o f the co ndit ions of learning ,

and  that they  of ten  lack precise s ta tements about the ways in which those

condi t ions  facili tate  learning and what kind of learning they would

  facil i-

ta te . It is of ten  s imply assumed that t he  condit ions argued for w o u l d  facil i-

tate all kinds of learning.

W hen w e use the expression

 pedagogy

  of

  learning,

  we do not refer to the

process whereby   the condit ions o f learning  are taken as the poin t o f depar-

ture

 an d it is

 s imp ly assum ed that these condit ions embo dy " the

 a rt of

 teach-

ing

 a l l thin gs to a l l men." T he pedagogy of learning m eans ta kin g learning

as

 the po int of depar ture an d exploring the con dit ion s that m ight be condu -

cive to  br ingin g that learning ab out .

As lea rn ing

 is

 a lw ays

 th e

 learning

 of

 som ething, there

 are

 severe lim its

 to

discussing  learning in gen eral , w ithou t reference to w hat is learned. W hat is

learned is the ob ject of learning and we argue that a pedagog y of learning

m u s t  take as i ts poin t of departure the very o bject of learning .

Most

 of the stu dies reported in this book

  follow

 the simple design of com-

paring how the same o bject of learning was dealt w ith in two or more

  different

classrooms.

  W e

  have used this model because

  w e

  believe that

  the way in

which

  the obje ct of learning is dealt w ith in the classroom is of decisive im -

portance , and that w e can o nly f ind out how the object of learning is dealt w ith

in

 the

 classroom

 by

 com paring

  it

 with another

 way of

 deal ing w ith

 the

  same

object of learning (this follows from the theory of variation discuss ed in this

chapter) .

 In this book, w e describe m uch of w hat we found about  differences

in   the w a y s in w hich  the  same objects of learning were dealt w ith.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 49/255

36  M A R T O N , R U N E S S O N , T S U I

In

 all

  studies reported

  in

 this book,

  w e

  t ranscr ibed verbat im everything

that  w as said  in the

 classroom

  and  carried  out our  analyses  on the  t ran-

scripts.

 W e

 have also used som e

  of the

  transcripts

  or

 par ts the reof (w i tho ut

ident i fying

  the  school, class,  or the  teacher)  in  seminars  and  workshops .

Frequently  we let the

 workshop

  par t ic ipants—educational  researchers,

school  administra tors , teacher educators , and teachers—read through the

transcripts

  and

 then address

  our

 in i t ia l ques t ion:

 "W e

 have here

  tw o

 classes

deal ing w ith the same top ic (content or obje ct of l ea rn ing) . W h a t w o u l d y o u

say you f ind the most s t r iking dif feren ce between the two?"

Very rarely

 did we

 hear comm ents

 on the way in

 w h i c h

 th e

 object

 of

 learning

w as handled  in the two classes,  that  is, w h a t it was possible  for the  student to

leam

  in one

 case

  and

 wha t

 it was

 possible

  for the

 student

 to

 learn

 in the

 other.

M ost comm ents concerned w hether there was a case o f

 wh ole-class

 teaching o r

group work, w hether the teaching w as teacher-centered or student-centered,

whether

 or not

  audiovisual tools,

  IT ,

 m am pulatives were used,

 and so on.

W e do no t

 deny that

 the way in

 w h i ch

 the

 lea rn ing s i tua t io n

 w as

 a r ranged

in each c lass w as of impo rtance for the pos sibi l i ty to learn. For ins tan ce, w e

agree   wi th  the  a rgument tha t  the  opportuni ty  fo r  c o m m u n i c a t i o n in the

classroom

  is of

  impor tance

  fo r

  s tudents ' lea rn ing ,

 and  w i t h  the

 a rg u m e n t

that

 the wa y in

 w h i ch

 the

  learners

 are

 able

 to

 par t ic ipa te

 in the

 cons t i tu t ion

of the ob jec t o f learning i s a lso imp ortant . How ever , w e c l a im that w h a t s t u -

dents a re com m unica t ing and w hat they a re in te rac t ing abou t i s ju s t as s ig-

n i f ican t

  as how

  they in te rac t

 and

 c o m m u n i c at e .

Le t us use an example

  from

  a s tudy of a m ath em atics lesson repor ted by

Voigt (1 995,

 pp .

 173-174)

 to show that there can be features of the learning

si tuat ion other than interac t ion tha t are a lso impo r tant fo r s tuden t s ' l e a rn ing .

Voig t  described

  how two

  students , Jack

  and

  Jamie , went about so lv ing

 a

group   o f  ar i thmetic tasks:

1 .

  5 0 - 9  = 41

2 . 60-9  = 51

3.  6 0 - 1 9  = 41

4. 41 + 19 = 60

5.

  31+29

 = 6 0

6. 31 + 19 = 50

7. 32 + 18 = _

L et us  take  a  look  at how  problem  7 was  ta lked about by the  boys.

Jack: U h-huh, that 's

  18, not 1 9.

Jamie: Yeah , but th at 's 32 no t 31.

Jack:  Oh

 yeah

Jamie:

  They're  the

 same th ing .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 50/255

1 .

  T HE  S P A C E  O F

  L E A R N I N G

Researcher: What's the

 same  thing?

Jam ie : These two. [He points to the 3 1 + 1 9 = 50 and 32 + 1 8 = _ ]

Researcher: Hang

  on , I w as

  asking Jack. Which

 ones?

 W e've

  got 3 1

and 19.

Jack:  Makes 50.

Researcher : Yeah.

Jack:

  A nd

  look,

  32 and 18.

 See,

  its

 jus t

  one more

  than that

[points  to the  task],  and that 's  one higher than that .

It  can be

 seen

 f rom the

 excerpt that

 the

 last task

 w as

 solved

 by a

 compen-

sation

  s tra tegy, a s tra tegy that the boys actual ly had not u sed in the previou s

tasks.

  S o,

 this tas k

 w as

 handled differently from

 the

 previous ones.

 In

  other

w ords, the boys learned to handle the problem in a new w ay. V oigt (1 995)

used

  the  not ions "negotia t ion  o f  mathemat ica l meaning"  and  "meaning

taken   as shared"  (p. 174) for understanding and explaining students ' learn-

ing .

 H e a rgu ed tha t wi thout the interact ion between  the boys, this new

 strat-

egy   never would have emerged. Undoubtedly,  th e  interaction  was o f

im po rtance for the s tud en ts ' learning . in this case . How ever, can this new

w ay of han dling the task, and hence the s tudents ' learning, be ex plained by

the interact ion and the negotia t ion of meaning

  on ly?

  W e be lieve we m ust

also

  unde r s t and w ha t

  it is

 po ssible

  to

  learn

  in the

  s i tua t ion

 in

  order

  to ac-

count

  for

 w h a t

  the

 participan ts actua lly learn.

If

 w e

 take

 a

 closer look

 a t the

 problem s,

  it is

 possible

  to

 look

 at

 this other

side of learning ; that i s , what  i t w as possible  to  learn. To us as authors , it is

obv ious  tha t

  the way in

  which this group

  of

  problems

  w as

  composed

  af-

fected  w h a t

 i t was

 possible

  to

 learn.

  In

  this group

  of

 problem s, there

 was a

pa rticula r variation present. S om ething varied,

 whereas other

  things were

invar iant

  be tween  the problem s. Firs t  o f all , there  was a varia t ion between

addi t ion

  and

  subtract ion (problem

  1

 through

  3 and 4

  through

  7,

  respec-

t ively).

 Further, if w e f irst an alyz e problem s 4 and 5 in detail , w e w ill  find

another  var ia tion , nam ely tha t

  the

  tenths chang ed both

  in the f irst and the

second addend (from 41 to 3 1, and from 19 to 29, respectively). B oth these

addit ions have  the  same tota l sum ( i.e. , 60), bu t they have  a different  par t

par t  -whole re la t ion .

  Let us now

  look

  at

 problem s

  6 and 7.

  Problem

  6 in-

volves  and

  combines numbers

  from

  problems

  4 and 5. Th e f irst

  addend

  is

the

 sam e as the f irst adden d in prob lem 5 and the second a ddend is the sam e

as the  second addend  in problem 4. In problem  7,

 there

  is a change  of the

one s in both the adden ds ( i .e ., increas ing from 3 1 to 32 and dec reasin g from

1

 9 to 1 8, respect ively) . Obviously, there  was a very system atic var ia tion in

the com posi tion

 of the

 problems.

  W e

 would

  say

 that

 a

 particular p attern

  o f

var iat ion

 w as

  afforded

  to the s tudents to exper ience. An d

  from

  the excerpt

ju st cited w e interpret that the students  did discern  a pattern  of variation. For

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 51/255

38

  M A R T O N ,

  R U N E S S O N , T S U I

instance, one of the boys said: "See, it 's ju st o ne more than that [points to the

problem],  and that 's one high er than that." So , we w a n t to co nc lude tha t th e

afforded  varia tion w as a feature of the learning si tuat ion that w as a lso s ig-

n i f ican t

  fo r

  s tudents ' learning.

In

 these exam ples, w e have tr ied to poin t out a varia t ion tha t w as presen t

in the  learning environm ent ,  a var ia tion tha t i t was  possible  for the learners

to   exper ience. What  w e  a rgue  for in the

  fo l lowing

  chapters  is  that this a f-

forded

  variation  is a cr i t ica l feature  in re la t ion  to the way in w h i ch  the in-

tended learning   is  brought about ,  as  w e l l  as  be ing  a  cr i t ica l  feature  fo r

students ' learning. The  thesis o f th is book  is tha t the dif ferences  in w h a t the

students leam is to a large extent a funct ion  of w hat they can poss ib ly leam .

W h a t

 they

 can possibly

 leam

 is a space of

 learning co nstituted

 by

 tha t which

it

  is

 possible

  to

  discern.

In

  the aforementioned example , there is cer ta inly an interact ion taking

place tha t contr ibutes to const i tut in g the space of learn ing. But the ser ies of

problems a lso cont r ibutes to c on s t i tu t ing the space of lea rn ing . W itho ut the

pa rticula r pattern

  o f

 var ia t ion tha t

 is

 embo died

 in the

 p rob lem s ,

 the

 s tudents

w ould not have been ab le to com e u p w i th the idea of comp ensa t ion , not in

that par t icular s i tuat ion, at least.  In several places  in th is book,  w e give ex-

amples

  of how the

  space

  o f

  learning

  is

  cons t i tu ted

  in the

  in te r ac t ion .

 In

chapter 5, chapter 6, and ch apter 7, for instanc e, we see how the teacher and

the s tudents join t ly co nst i tute the space of learning as the teache r takes on

the s tudents ' responses and opens them up for inq uiry and  further  develop-

m ent . W e a lso see how s tudents in te rac t ing in groups , jo in t l y cons t i tu ted the

space of learning by b r inging in their own cu l tura l and

 da i ly

 exper iences to

m ake sense o f the object of learning . For exam ple , in chap ter 6, w he n stu-

dents discuss h ow the appearance of the s loth sh ou ld be descr ibed, several

of them th ink tha t a ll eyes are b lack and tha t the re fore there is no need to in-

clude the color of the s loth 's eyes. In other w ords, they are not able to dis-

cern the color o f i ts eyes as a crit ic al feature . H ow ever , one s tudent draw s the

group's

 attention  to the

 fact

 tha t a rabbit ' s eyes a re r ed, thu s show ing tha t no t

all eyes are black. A nother s tuden t agrees w ith his observat ion. The group

f inally  agrees on inc lud ing the color of the s loth 's eyes in the descn ptio n.

W hat is happ ening here is that through col labo rat ive inte ract io n, the s tu-

dents are able to br ing in dime nsio ns of var ia t ion tha t m igh t otherw ise be

neglected if students are not  afforded  the oppor tuni ty to in te rac t am on g

themselves.

W e are not

 denying tha t

 the

 space

 of

 learning

 may in

 part,

 or

 ent ire ly,

 be

const i tuted in the interact ion between students . However , the point we

w ould l ike to m ake is that reference is  f requent ly  m ade to interact ion (s tu-

dent centeredness, activity approach, task-based learning, etc.)

  as

  some-

thing that  is  inherently conducive  to  learning, without relating  the  inter-

act ion to what is learned. And that the l ived object of learning is—as a

ru le—not expla ined  in any precise way. Interact io n, s tude nt centeredness.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 52/255

1 .  T H E S P A C E O F  L E A R N I N G  39

and so on, are assumed to be good for learning in

 some

 very general , but

rather vague sense,

  and for

 their

 ow n

  sake.

In this

 book ,  we try to

  establish

  the  precise

  l ink between learning

  and

condi t ions of learning.

 This

 link is established by m ean s of the relat ionsh ip

between the object of learning and the space o f learning, where the enac ted

object

 of

 learning

 is

 identical

 to the

 space

 o f

 learning, both

 of

 w hich refer

 to

the ob jec t of learning as it real ized in the classroom .  It is the ob jec t of learn-

ing  that the studen ts encounter, and the ob ject of learning th at they ca n pos-

sibly

  learn,

  o r

  learn about , making

  it

  into

  a

 part

  of

  themselves .

  If we are

interested in how

  students learn

  to see

  certain things

  in

  cer tain ways,

  w e

m ust ask  o urselves w hat c r i tical features of the ob jec t o f learning s tudents

can

 possibly discern in a part icular classroom si tua tion . N obo dy can learn to

see

 cer tain th ing s

 in

 cer tain w ays w i thout experiencing cer tain pat terns

  o f

var iat ion.  This s tatement  is  true  in  general  as far as  this kind  of  learning

( learning to see

 som eth ing

 in a

 cer ta in wa y)

 is

 concerned,

 and it

 im pl ies that

very

  specific

 pat terns of variat ion are necessary condi t ions for the learning

o f—o r  learning abou t— specif ic objects  of learning.

Having

 said that,

 we do not

 w ish

 to

  im ply that

 i t

 does

 n ot

 m at ter whether

th e

 teaching

 is

 wh ole class,

 or

 whether students work

 in

 groups; w hether stu-

dents are  engaged  in  task-based  or  problem-based learning,  or in  project

work;

 whether teaching

  is

 student centered; w hether

  IT is

 used;

 or

 whether

there is a joy fu l  atmosphere in the classroom or not . Differences  in the way in

w hich the co nditions of learning are organized m ay u ndo ubtedly constrain or

facilitate  the

  constitution

 of the

 space

 of

 learning necessary

 fo r the

 develop-

m ent of a certain capab ility. But in order to understand how these  differences

inhibit or facilitate  the developm ent of a certain cap ability, w e have to under-

stand h ow

 they con strain

 or

 facilitate

  th e

 constitution

 of the

  specific  space

 o f

learning   necessary

  for

  developing that  specific  capability.

 A nd

  this under-

standing

  can

 never

 be

  derived

  from

  a

 general

  way of

  organizing

  the

 condi-

t ions of

 learning.

 F or

 this reason,

 we can

 never

  truthfully

  argue that

 one way

of

 organizing

 th e

 con ditions

 of

 learning

 is, in

 gen eral , bet ter than another w ay.

In

 this book we try to develop a theore tical perspective  and a conceptua l

f ramework

  for

 unders tanding learning

 in

 schools

 in

 t e rm s

 of

 w h a t

 it is

 poss i-

ble to

 learn

 in

 schools.

 I f we

 know w ha t

 the

 studen ts shou ld learn,

 and if we

know  w hat condi tions a re necessary  fo r them  to learn , then  w e co uld rea-

sonab ly

  work toward creating those necessary condit ions. I t must be re-

m em bered, however , that

 for

 each

  specific

  capabil i ty

 (of

  seeing something

in  a certain w ay ), there are necessary,  specific  con dit ions. Even  i f w e know

in   general that there m ust

 be a

 certain space

  of

  learning that

 com prises di-

mens ions

  o f

 variation th at  correspond

  to the

  cri t ical features defining  th is

specific  way of seeing a  specific p h e n o m e n o n , we do not know w hat these

crit ical

  features

 are in

 every  specific  case.

This "blessed

 ignoranc e" is a

 w eakness that

 can be

 turned into

 a

 strength

by en gag ing the teachers themselves in finding  the missing insights that are

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 53/255

40

  M A R T O N ,

  R U N E S S O N ,  T S U I

needed  fo r achieving

 different

  par t icu lar educat ional a im s , and for develop-

ing  speci f ic capabi l i ties an d par t i cu lar ly pow erfu l w ays of seeing part icu lar

phenom ena. Teachers focu s ing

 on the

 same ob jec t

 o f

 learning

 can do so to-

gether by f inding the cr i t ical features in each case, creat ing what are be-

l ieved to be the   necessary condi t ions  fo r  develop ing tha t capab i l i ty ,

check ing wha t

 th e

 stude nts leam

 an d

 w h e t h er

 th e

 co n jec tures were ju s t i f ied ,

revising  th e  fr amew ork if neces sa ry, check ing aga in , docu m en t ing th e pro-

cess  as we l l as the  ou t com es , and sharing  it

 w i th

 o ther t eachers .

Teachers could

  in

  th is

 w ay

  par t i c ipa te

  in the

  co l lec t ive cons t ruc t ion

 o f

professional   knowledge , not on ly a s a par t of t he i r ow n profe ss ional devel -

opmen t  but also as  scientif ic  research that m ay y ie ld new  ins igh t s . Severa l

examples

  o f

  such

 an

  enterprise

 are

 ou t l i ned

 in

 c h a p t e r

 8 of

  th is

 book .

E N D N O T E S

'T o be

  fu l l y  a ccu r a t e ,

  o f

  cour se ,

  w e

  shou ld say , "deve lop ce r t a in capab i l i t i e s

and  values However , a l though

  not

  w i s h i n g

  to

  p l ace m o r e i m p o r t an ce

  on one

r a ther than

  th e

 other,

  th e

  f o c u s

  o f

  th i s book

  i s on the

  d e v e lo p m e n t

  o f

 c ap ab i l i ti e s

r a ther than va lues ,

  a s

  th is

  h a s

  been

  th e

  f o c u s

 o f o u r

  s t u d i e s .

2

The pron oun "he"  is used  in a sex n e u t r a l s en se  to a v o id a w k w a r d use o f "he o r

she."

3

A

  reed re lay

  is a  device th a t w i l l cha nge  a  weak e lec t r i ca l cu r r en t in to a  very

st rong cur rent when   it is  connec ted  to  e l e c t ri c c i r cu i t s . R eed r e l ay s  a re  u sed  in

m any e lec tr ica l a pp l i ances tha t r equ i r e l a rge cur r en t s  fo r o p e r a t io n , su ch a s e l eva -

tors, motors, and so on.

4

A   light

  d iode

  resistor  ( L D R )  ca n  p r even t  a n  e l ec t r i c cu r r en t f rom pass ing

th rough   a n e l ec tr i c c i r cu i t becau se  of i t s g rea t res i s t a nc e . L ig h t d iode r es i s to r s  a re

sensi t ive  to  ligh t . W hen exposed  to b r i g h t l i g h t , th e r e s i s t an c e r edu ces , t h u s a l l o w -

in g the  e lect r ic cur rent  to pass th rough .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 54/255

 

O n

  Learning

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 55/255

  his page intentionally left blank

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 56/255

2

Variation

  and the  Secret

of  the

 Virtuoso

Po

  Yuk Ko

Ference Marton

MAKING

  SYSTEMATIC  USE OF

 VARIATION

In chapter

  1 , we

 argued that whatever object

 of

  learning learners,

  try to ap-

prop r iate , cer tain cr i t ical features

 o f

 that objec t have

 to be

 d iscerned, that

 is ,

learners must learn   to see  cer tain s i tuat ions  in  cer tain ways.  B u t  learners

can

 never discern anyth ing w ithout expe r iencing var iat ion. T he  exper ience

of

 pat terns

  o f

 var iat ion

  specif ic

  fo r different

  objects

 of

 learning

  is a

 neces-

sary condi t ion   for  appropr iat ing those objects  o f  learning.  If  this  is  true,

then

 success fu l

  teachers mus t

 b e

 good

 a t

 con st i tu t ing such necessary con di-

t ions

 in the

  classroom, that

 is, the

  specif ic

  pat terns

 o f

 var iat ion.

 One of the

m am

 po in ts o f th is book  is tha t w e have m anaged  to identify  a cr i t ical  factor

in

  teach ing , a

  factor

  tha t d is t inguishes

 be tween

  teach ing tha t

 m a k es

 a cer-

ta in

  learning possib le and teach ing that

  fails

  to achieve this . Th e em pir ical

compar i sons  in chap ter  3 and  chapter  4  demonstrate th is  in greater detai l .

In

 any

  ins tance

 o f

  teach ing ,

  there  are

  th ings that

  are

  repeated

  and

  there

are

 thing s that vary.

 On e of the

 fea tures

 of

 good pract ice

 and

 pow er fu l peda-

gogy is the

  eff ic ient

  use of var iat ion and repet i t ion, whether i t be con-

sciously

  or

  less consciou sly .

 Let us

  look

  a t an

  example .

K w a n ,

 Ng, and

 C hik (2002)

 offered  a

 por trayal

  of an

 innovat ive teache r 's

w ay  of  t r ansform ing  the  intended object  of  learning into an enacted ob jec t

of learning in the course  of a double lesson  in Chinese ( the class was a pri-

mary Grade

  2

 class

  in

 Hong Kong) .

T he  intended object  o f  learning could  be  split into  tw o  in ter twined  as-

pec ts that were n ot to be dealt with sepa rately, but at the same t im e. T he s tu-

43

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 57/255

4 4 K O A N D

  M A R T O N

dents were expected  to become better  at the  fo l low ing sk i ll s : (a )  describing

animals , and (b) using the four language ski l l s in an in tegra t ive manner .

T he  doub le

 lesson

 b u i l t on the  previous lesson  in w h i c h th e s tudents h ad

learned to discern

  di f fe rent

  aspects o f a pan da, such as i t s app earanc e (body,

head, four l im bs, color) and its movement .  A s the po in t  o f departure fo r the

double lesson,  a 3-m inute  film  sequence w as shown  to the c lass . In th e f i lm

sequence ,  th e s tudents  s aw a young s lo th , w h o according  to the na r ra to r h ad

lost

  its

 m other.

  T he

  s tudents

  saw the

 s loth c l im b down

  f rom a

 tree

  in

 search

of a new   home, swim

 across

 a nearby river, and c l im b u p ano ther tree w here

it encountered

 another sloth—which  w as  luck i ly  f r iend ly—and  t hus  the

young s lo th found a new home.

The s tudents f i rs t watched the v ideo wi th the narra t ion, then watched i t

a ga i n wi t hou t

  th e

  na r ra t ive ( see ing , l i s t en ing ) . N ext they foc used

  on the

sloth ' s appearance by discu ssin g it s var ious fea ture s (body, he ad,  four

l imbs,

  color)  and its movement

  (speaking, l i s tening) .

 Then  the

  s tudents

w rote down th e i r descr ipt ions

  on

 overhead t ranspa rencies

 and

 showed them

to the re st o f the c l a ss (w r i t ing , r ead ing) . F ina l ly they appo in ted a m em ber

of the

 group

 w ho

 c a me

  to the f ront  of the

 c lass

 a n d

 ac ted

 a s

 na r ra to r

 a n d

 read

a loud the i r desc rip tion w hi l e the m uted f ilm seque nce w as rep layed on the

video  (speaking, l istening ).

Dur ing

  th e

 lesson,

  a

 h igh ly co m plex pa t te rn

  o f

 va r i a t ion

 w as

 j o i n t l y c o n-

st itu ted by the teache r and the s tu den ts . Let us look a t some inte res t ing pa t-

terns  o f var ia t ion  and  invar i ance .

To begin w i th , the s tudents were expected to

 sh i f t

  the i r a t t en t ion be tween

tw o

 aspects

 of the

 an imal :

  its appearance and its

 m ovement.

  W e

 cannot

  say

t h a t these

  tw o

 aspects var ied ,

 but the

 s tudents them se lves were a sked

 t o

 vary

the foc us of the i r a t ten t ion between the two

  di f fe rent

  aspects and the  four

di f ferent  par ts of the animal . This show s tha t va r ia t ion can th us be exper i -

enced by exp er i enc ing va r i a tion genera ted by the env i ron m ent ( e .g . ,

  focus-

ing on

 d i f ferent animals)

  or by

 varying

 one's

 at tent ion

  o r

  imaginat ion (e .g . ,

f oc us ing

 o n d i f ferent  a spec t s of the same a n im al ) . Fur the rm ore , va r i a t ion is

a lway s exper ienced aga ins t the backgro und of wh a t i s invar i an t . So the re -

fore  w e

  speak

  o f "patterns o f

 va r i a t ion

 and

 invar iance ."

 In our

 spec i f ic  case

w e   have  th e  fo l lowing :

Variation  Invariance

di f ferent

  a spec ts same an im al

and

Variation

  Invariance

di f ferent  par ts same aspect (app earance )

B y genera t ing two k inds of varia tion in re la t ion to the same an im al ,

 differ-

en t

 aspects

  and

 different  parts

 can be

 discerne d.

 B ut is

 th is specif ic

  to

 th is ani-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 58/255

2.

  V A R I A T I O N  A N D T H E  S E C R E T  O F T H E  V I R T U O S O  45

m al? In order to generalize the acts of discernm ent, they have to be applied to

m ore than

 o ne

 an imal .

 As the

 same exercise

 w as

 also carried

 out in

 relation

 to

a

 pa nda dur ing the previous lesson, th is condi t ion w as ac tual ly m et .

Variation   Invar iance

dif fe rent

  an im als same ac ts o f d iscernm ent

(panda and s lo th) (be tween aspects , and between par t s )

So fa r w e

 have been dea l ing w i th

 t he  first  of the two

  in tende d ob je c ts

 of

l ea rn ing :

 be ing ca pab le o f describ ing an an im al . W hat about the second in-

tended object

 of

  learning, using

 the  four language

  skills

 in an

  mtegrative

m a n n e r ? A s a  m a t te r  o f

  fact ,

  the two  in tended ob jec ts  o f  learn ing  can be

b r o u g h t

 toge the r

 b y

 app ly ing them

 to the

 same task : tha t

 is ,

 desc r ib ing an i -

m als in

 speech

 a nd in

 w ritten

  fo rm,  and

 tak ing par t

 in the

 desc r ip tion

 by

 l i s -

t e n i n g   a n d r e a d i n g . T h e s t u d e n t s t a l k e d a b o u t t h e a p p e a r a n c e a n d

m ovem ent o f the s lo th , they wro te down the ir observat ions , they read eac h

other s ' observat ions , they com m ented on the fi lm sequence wh en the sou nd

w as

  o f f ,

 and  they

  l i s tened

 to

  each others' c o m m e n ts .

Variation

  Invar iance

different  same thing

 described

com m unica t ive ac ts ( the s lo th)

W e  can see here the double funct ions  o f var iat ion, that is, tha t  d i f ferences

between the

  different

  ac t s can be d iscerned when the i r

 objec t

  i s the same .

This

 is

 w h a t

 w e

 refer red

 to as separa t ion  in the

 previous chap ter .

 It is the di-

m e n s i o n o f co m m unica t iv e ac ts t ha t is be ing separa ted from other aspects o f

the relat ionship between the descr iber and the descr ibed, that is , between

the

  s tudents and the s tory about the s loth . But because the object of these

ac ts is the sam e, com m onal i t ies a re b rought ou t as wel l : w ords , express ions ,

and sen tences .

 Dif ferent

  acts are d iscerned and broug ht together at the sam e

t ime. A

 th ird pat tern

 o f

 var iation presen t

 in the

  lesson m akes

 it

 possib le

  fo r

the

 s tuden ts to ar r ive at the ins igh t that the sam e task can be c ar r ied out in

dif fe rent  bu t

  e qual ly correc t w ays . However,

  th e

  different  w a y s

  o f

 d o ing

som eth ing on ly becom e v is ib le

 if

 they

 a re

 perceived

 as

 being

 different

 w a y s

of doing

 t he

 same

 thing

 (otherwise

  different

  acts and

 different

  tasks co-vary

and can not be separa ted from each o ther).

 This

 is exa ct ly the c ase in the les-

son

 d iscussed here ;

 the

 sloth

 i s

 described

 in

 different  w ays , t ha t

 is, by

 m e a n s

o f d i f ferent  communica t ive ac ts .

The teacher in th is example is a Chinese teacher   from  Hong Kong . I n

fact,

  a lmos t

 all of the

 examp les

  in

 this

 book are

 abou t Ch inese teachers ,

 and

m ost o f them are

 from

  Hong Kong. St igler and Hieber t (1999) emphasized

that

  teach ing is a cu l tural act iv ity a nd argued tha t teachers  in different  cu l -

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 59/255

46 KO AND M A R T O N

tures follow  different  scripts. If so , is it the case that there is a spe cific Chi-

nese  pedagogy, a Chinese  script that  is  characterized, among other things,

by

  an elaborate  use of patterns of variation  and  invariance?

Is it reasonable  at all to  expect  a  speci f ic Ch inese pedagogy? Trying  to

find

  out

 whe ther there

  is one or not

 wou ld

 be an

 im possibly large undertak-

ing because

  of the

  sheer vastness

  and

 va riabil i ty

 of the

 Chinese cu l ture.

 A n

easier question   to address—and  one  tha t for our purposes  is ac tua l ly more

interes t ing— is this : W hat

 are the

 pedagog ical pract ices considered

 to be ex-

emplary

  in

 C hina? This

 was the

 question addressed

  by one

 me mb e r

  of our

team (Ko, 2002),

  and the

 fo l low ing sect ion bu i lds

 on her

 work .

A

  L E S S O N

  O N

  S E M A N T I C S

In th is sect ion, we present a lesson tau gh t by an expert teach er in the Peo-

ple ' s Re publ ic of Ch ina (PR C; the lesson w as referred to br ief ly in cha p. 1) .

The  t eacher  is a  "Special Rank Teacher,"  and the  w inner  of a pres t ig ious

s tate-granted tea chin g aw ard.

 T he

 do uble lesson

  is a

 dem onstrat ion lesson

that

  w as

  audiotaped, t ranscr ibed,

  and

 pu bl i shed (Qian , 1985) ,

 in

 order

  to

dissem inate the teac hing expert ise of the teac her as a m odel lesson for his

counterpar t s

 to follow.  It

 a l low s

 us to see

 w h a t

 is

 considered

 to be

 good ped-

agogy

  in the

  P R C .

The  sample lesson  is a  reading lesson based  on a prescribed text ca l led

"Semantics ," wh ich is a piece of exposi tory w ri t ing abou t the lexical m ean -

ings of w ords. In the lesson, the teacher a im ed to introd uc e several

 l inguis t ic

concepts   on  semant ics , including th e

 scope

 of m e a n in g o f words;  the level

of

 generali ty

 of

 words ; homonym s, synonyms,

  and

 an tonym s,

 and

 thei r

 us-

age. In the fol lo w ing , w e first describe the lesson and then a na lyze the varia-

t ional pattern.

S u m m a r y  of the  L e s s o n

Instead

 of

 in t roducing

 th e

 l inguis t ic know ledge direct ly ,

 the

 teacher started

w ith a s tory aim ing to i l lus t ra te that the m ean ing of words can vary a ccord-

ing to the co ntex t , and that i t is im portan t to be aware o f the co m plexity of

the me a n i n g o f  words .

Afan t i

  was a

  hairdresser. There

  was one

  cus tomer ,

  A h u n g ,

  w ho

  a l w a y s w e n t

  to

A f a n t i ' s

  p lace

 to

 have

 his ha ir cut but

 never paid

  for the

 service.

 It

 made

  Afan t i

  very

angry.

 He

 wan ted

 to

 p lay

  a

 t r ick

 o n  A h u n g . One day  A h u n g

 c a m e

 t o  Afan t i ' s  aga in .

Afan t i  f i r s t  cu t

 A h u n g ' s

 hair .

  T h e n

 he

  began

 to

 s h a v e A h u n g ' s

 face,  and

  asked,

  "Do

you  w a n t

 you r

  eyebrows?"

 A h u n g

 rep l i ed ,

 "O f

  course

W hy

  ask " Then,

 qu ick

  as a

flash, Afan t i

 shaved

 off

 A hu ng 's eyebrows  and said, "You wan ted your eyebrows,  so I

w i l l

  g ive them

 to

  you "

 A h u n g was too m ad to say

  any th ing ;

 because

 he had

 indeed

said

 he

 "wanted"

 his

 eyebrows.

 M e a n w h i le Afan t i

 asked,

 "Do you

 w an t your

 beard?"

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 60/255

2.

  V A R I A T I O N

  AND

 THE

 S E C R E T

 OF  THE  V I R T U O

A h u n g

  had a

  beau t i fu l

  beard ,  and he  imm ed ia te ly sa id ,  "M y  beard? No, no I d o n ' t

w a n t my beard "  B u t aga in

 Afan t i

  proceeded  to shave off his beard .

 A h u n g

 s tood up

and saw an  egg- l ike head  in the m ir ror . [Both  the  teacher  and

  s tuden ts

  l augh . ] Fur i -

ously, A h u n g  reproved Afan t i ,  " W h y did you  shave of f my e y e b r o w s and beard?" "I

w as  on l y

  fo l lowing

  y o u r

  orders, s ir "

  Afan t i

  a n s w e r e d  calmly.  There  w as

  no t h i ng

A h u n g  cou ld

  say to

  tha t

After  tel l ing the story, the teacher asked questions to help the studen ts to

unravel

  the

  ambivalence

  of the

  mean ing

  o f

  "want"

  ( in

  Chinese,

  y a o

(Pu tonghua)

2.1

T :

  Af ter

  l is tening to this story, do you know what kind of tr ick

Afant i

 played on

 Ahung?

Ss:  Afan t i  played a trick using the word "want."

T :

  W hat did A fanti m ean by asking "Do you

 'want'

 your eyebrows?"

Ss: It

 meant

 "to give."

T :

  Exact ly

It

 mean t

 "to

 give."

 You

 w ant them, then

 I

 shave them

 off

and

  give them to you. Then  what i s the meaning of the word

"want"

 in "Do you

 w ant your

 beard?"

Ss:

  "T o

  keep."

T :

  Yes,

 it

 me a n s

 "to keep." But how did

 A hung interpret

  th e

 w ord?

H e

 st il l though t

 i t was to

 "give"

 h im h i s

 beard.

 That's w hy he im -

media te ly answe red, "I don ' t wa nt it." A nd he  fell  in to the t rap.

S o  you

 see,

 the

 word

 "want" has tw o

 meanings .

 It can

 mean this ,

or it can

 mean tha t. Afant i  exploited

  the

 am bigui ty

 of the

 m e a n-

ing of "want" to trap A hun g. This story tel ls us i t is im portant to

master the m ean ing of a word a nd its scope of m eaning [Teacher

writes o n the  blackboard: meaning, scope  of m ean ing ] .

In  the

  story,

 the

  word

  "want"  (yao

  (Putonghua)  [-lc])

 had two

  mean-

ings

 — ei ther "to keep" (liu (Pu tonghua) [ ̂ ]) or "to give"  (gei (P u tonghua)

[ £«•]) acc ording to different  contexts. Afant i  exploited the am bigui ty of the

mean ing

  o f "want"

 (homonyms)

  to

  t rap Ahung. When Ahung interpreted

"want" as meaning "to keep," A fan ti deliberately interpreted the

 "want"

 as

m ean ing "to give," and vice versa. A s a result , Ah ung lost his eyebrow s and

his beard. In the teache r-led discussion that im m ediately fo l low ed the read -

ing, the teacher did not u se m etalan gu ag e (i .e., the term

  hom on y m s )

  to illus-

trate the semantics concept, but simply highlighted the complexity of the

mean ing

  of

  words .

Nex t ,

  the

  teacher showed

  the

  class some books

  and

  asked students

  to

name them.

 

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 61/255

4 8 K O A N D   M A R T O N

2 2

T : . . . I first

 w a n t

 you to

  look

  at

  some th ings

 a nd

 nam e them

[holding

 up a

 book] . W hat

  is

 this?

Ss: A   Chinese book.

T :

  [hold ing

 u p

  another book] W hat

  is

 th is?

Ss : An  E ng l i sh book .

T :

  [holding

 up two

 books tog ether]

 I f w e put

 them toge ther, wh a t

 do

w e  cal l  them?

Ss:  Books .

T:  Narrow down  th e  scope.

Ss:

  Textbooks.

T: [holding up a dic t ion ary] W hat is this?

Ss: A dic t ionary.

T: Is i t a book ?

Ss:  Yes,  it is.

T: W hat k ind

  o f

 book?

S s:

  Re fe rence

 book?

T: W hat are these three a l l togeth er?

Ss:  Books.

T :

  That's

  right.  Chinese book, textbook, book,  the  scope  of the

meanings becomes b igger  an d b igge r  .. .

In  this excerpt, we see that the teacher started with  specif ic  k inds of

books and

 then asked

 the

 students

 to

 provide

 a

 general w ord that could cover

both  kinds  o f books.  B u t w e  also  see tha t the  teacher tried  to he lp the s tu-

dents

 to see the

 differenc e

  in

 scope

 o f

 m e a n in g

 b y

 showing

 how one

 k ind

 o f

book subsum es an other kind of book, such that lan gu ag e books are a kind of

textbook and textbooks are in turn a kind of book, jus t as a reference  book is

also  a kind  o f  book.

On one

 hand,

  th e

  teacher cleverly juxtap osed  books

 at the

  same level

 of

specificity—a

  Chinese languag e book

  and an

  English language book—and

showed that it is only necessary to go one level up in order to su bsum e both

books,

 name ly

 by

 us ing

 th e

 word

 "textbook."

 Hen ce, there

 is no

 need

  to

 sub-

sume them both under th e very general term

  books .

  T he  teacher then jux ta-

posed textbook and reference book, which are also at the same level of

specificity,  and showed that the next level up to subsume both would be

"books." T he

  class were therefore presented w ith three levels

 o f

 generality,

and the  relationship between  the examples w as a hierarch ical structure, that

is, with one subsum ing the other.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 62/255

2 . V A R I A T I O N   A N D T H E  S E C R E T  O F T H E  V I R T U O S O  49

In  order to help the students see the relat ionship b etwe en w ords of  differ-

en t  levels  of  generality, which  is  denoted  by the  word relat ion known  as

h y p ony m s ,  the teach er varied the com bination of the spe cifics and the levels

of ge nerali ty. A s pointed out , th e com bination of the Chinese l anguage book

and  the

  Engl ish language book

  can be

  subsumed precisely under " text-

books," w hich is at a m ore gene ral level , but less precisely under

 "books,"

w hich is a t a yet

  more general level . However, when

  the

 co m bination

  is of

var ious

 types

 of

 books (i .e. , text books

  and

 reference books) ,

  the use of the

word

 "books"

 is the m ost app ropriate.

The teacher helped the s tudents unders tand the semant ic features of a

book by con sidering w hat i s not a book ( i .e ., by us ing

 con tras ts,

 m en t ioned

in

  chap .

  1 ):

2 3

T :

  ...

  [holding

 up

 pieces

  o f

 new spaper]

  Is

 this

 a

  book?

S s:  N o ,

  that ' s newspaper .

T: W hy is i t not a

  book?

S s:

  B ecause there 's

  no t

  enough [pages] .

S :

  Eve n a pi le of new spaper ca n ' t be cal led a book. A book has a

cover,

  a

 new spaper do esn ' t .

S:  N ot exactly. If I have a book, and I tear off the cover, the book is

 still

a

 bo ok. Pictorials also have covers,

 b ut

 they aren't c alled books.

S:  A

 book

  is a

 com pos i tion boun d

 by a

 cover.

T :

  [ho lding

 up an

  exercise book] This also

  has a

  cover , hasn ' t

  it?

B ut

  w e

 do n ' t c al l that

 a

 book .

  A

 book

  is a

 l iterary com posi t ion

bound by a

 cover.

 You can see

 that w i th dict ionaries , r ight? [s tu-

dents nodded thei r heads]

 T o be a

 book,

  it has to be

 bound

 by a

cover, and a t the

  same t ime

  it ha s to

  fulfi l  another requi rem ent :

that

  o f be ing a compos i t ion . Books are bound compos i t ions .

This

  is the

 definit ion

  of the

 word.

 Now can you

 give

 me the

  defi-

ni t ion of a

 textbook ?

S :  A

 textboo k

 is a

 composi t ion used

 for

 t each ing , bound

 in a

 cover .

T :

  R ight Then please give

 me the

 definit ion

 o f

 th is C hinese book .

S:

  A

 com pos i tion used

  for

  teaching Chinese, bound

  by a

 cover.

T :

  V ery good

W e put in

 m ore l im i ts here. From

  the

 exam ple above

w e

 can see that ,  from "book" to "textbook" to "Chinese  book,"

w h a t

 happe ns to the scope of m eaning?

Ss: I t

  becomes narrower.

T :

  Then

  how

 abou t

 the

 m eaning? Does

  it

 become m ore

  specif ic , or

m ore genera l?

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 63/255

50 KO AND M A R T O N

Ss:  Specif ic

T: A gain , you have discovered a rule: the sm aller the scope of the

mean ing ,

  the . . .

  w h a t

  . . . the

 mean ing becomes?

Ss : The

 m ore  specif ic

  the

 m ean ing becomes .

T :

  Then

  how

 about ,

  th e

 b igger

  th e

  scope?

Ss: The   more general  the  meaning becomes .

The teacher varied the examples of "not a

 book"

 and looked at the fea-

tures they had in comm on  and those that

 differed.

 Having a cover,  for exam-

ple,

  is

 c er tainly

  not a

  cr i t ical feature becau se a l thou gh

 a

 m agaz ine

  and an

exercise book both ha ve covers

 an d a

 new spaper does no t ,

 a ll

 three

 o f

 them

are not books. The   defini t ion  of the sem ant ic features of a book paved the

way for def ining

  more

  specif ic

  k inds

 o f

 books,

  fo r

 example ,

  the

 l anguage

textbook.  B y  getting students  to  def ine what  a  language textbook  is, the

teacher demonstrated tha t

 as the

 scope

  o f

 m ean ing ge ts narrower ,

 t he

 m e a n-

ings

 of the

 words

  get

 m ore  specif ic .

The teacher also t ried to relate scope of m ean ing to levels of gen eral i ty .

From book to textbook to langua ge book, the scope becom es sm al ler and

smaller. From

 book to

 textbook

  to

 langua ge book,

  the

 level

 o f

 gene rality

 be-

comes m ore

  and

 m o re  speci f ic .

T he

 teach er used several

 d i f ferent

  contex t s

 to

 i l lus t rate

 how

 words

 a t

 d i f -

ferent  levels

 of

  generali ty should

 be

  used

  in

 relat ion

 to d i f ferent

  contex t s :

2.4

T :  S o, I 'd

  l ike

 to

 ask, should

 we be

  more speci f ic

 or

 more genera l

w h e n w e

  make choices

 o f

 words

  in

 w ri ting

  an

  essay?

S :

  Somet imes

  it is

 better

 to be

  specif ic ,

 bu t

  somet imes

 it is

 better

 to

be general .

T :  A ny

  examples?

S: I f

 somebody asks , "How m any books

 do you

 have?"

 A nd you an-

swer,  "I  have one m a th , one Chinese,  one  Engl ish ,  one phys ics

and  on e chem istry book, so five book s in total ." That w ou ld be

very clumsy.

  W e

 w ould just say,

  "I

 have

  five

 books."

T :

  Any o ther com m ents?

S :  Sometimes w e should be more sp ecific. For exam ple,  if that is a

rabbi t ,

 w e

 should

 no t

 ju s t g ive

 a

 general answer

 and

 only

 say

 tha t

it  is an a n i ma l .

T:

  V ery good.

  In

 w riting

  and

  speaking,

  how

  should

 w e

  choose

  our

words? More  specific  ones

 or

 more general ones?

  It

 depends

  on

th e need  of  individual expressions.  B e  specific when necessary.

T he   same applies to being general. Your b iggest problem w hen

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 64/255

2.

  V A R I A T I O N  A N D T H E  SECRET  O F T H E  V I R T U O S O  5_[

choosing words

 is

 that

 you

 tend

 to be too

 general

  and

 vague when

you

 need

 to be specific. You saw a

 rabbit,

 but you say you saw an

animal .

 S o

 people don't know what

 y ou

 actually saw. Could

 you

give m e

 some examples

 of us ing

 words with vague meanings?

  ...

Afte r

 dealing with levels o f generality,  th e teacher dealt with another

type

 of

 word

 relation:

 synonyms.

 To bring out

 this kind

 of

 word relation,

the

 teacher contrasted

 it

 with homonyms, which

 had

 already been covered

in the story of

 A fan t i.

 Although

 homonyms

 are

 words

 with two meanings,

synonyms

  are the opposite; that is , the same meaning denoted b y

  d i f f e ren t

words.

 In

 other

 words,

 synonyms

 are

 understood

 in the

 context

 of the

 rela-

t ion

 between meaning

  an d

  word; synonyms being

  o n e

 kind

  o f

 meaning

word relationship

 and

 homonyms being another kind

 of

 meaning word

 re-

l a t i onsh ip .

2.5

T :

  . .. So , now w e come  to another question. In the story w e men-

tioned, the word "want" used by Afanti has two meanings. But

there

 are

 other situations where

 one

 meaning

 can be

 expressed

by

 a

 number

 of

 words.

 Can you

 give

 me

 some examples?

S :  For example "see," we sometimes use d i f ferent words like look

up

 at, glance at,

"look

 down, etc.  All these basically mean

th e same as

 "see."

T:

  Very good. These

  are the  d i f ferent

  interpretations

  of the

 word

"see." Now, I'd like you to find some

 more

 examples of  different

words representing the same meaning.

S :

  Father, daddy, papa.

T:

  Y ou

 learned

 a

 noun

 in the

  Poem

  of

 Mulan recently. What

 was i t?

S:

  Pater.

T:  Father, daddy, papa,

 and

 pater

 are a ll in the

 same semantic scope.

L et

  u s illustrate the scope  o f meaning by circles. F o r example,

book, textbook,

 a nd

 Chinese book [teacher draws

 a

 circle

 on the

bo ard] . If this circle indicates

 "book,"

 then where should w e p u t

the word textbook?

Ss:

  Inside

 the

 circle.

T :

  [drawing another circle] Then,

 h ow

 about

 the

 circles

 o f

 pater,

 f a-

ther , daddy, and papa?

Ss:

  They

 are of the

 same size.

T: Al l

 these  four circles

 are

 overlapping.

  W e

 call these  four words

synonyms.

  W e

 also consider them equivalent

 as

 they share

  the

same meaning....

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 65/255

52 KO AND  M A R T O N

Because

 the

 teacher contrasted these different examples,

 the

 students

 be-

came aware of the two  different  re la t ionsh ips be tween w ords and m eanings ,

that

 is, the

 dif ference be tween hom onym s

  and

  synonyms.

Follow ing th is , the teac her drew the stude nts ' at tention to the use of syn-

onyms .  Different  examples were used

  to

  i l lustrate  t ha t

 d i f ferent

  synony-

mo u s

 words

 should

 be

 used

  in

 differen t contexts

  for

 exam ple: form al versus

informal  ("father"  vs. "daddy"  or

 "papa");

  t echnica l versus nontechnica l

("sodium

 chloride"

 vs. "salt"); and different degrees of politeness ("wrinkly

old fel low" vs. "Grandpa").

The teacher then revisi ted the

 different

  levels of gen eral i ty.

2 6

T: S , please read  out the first and  second paragraph. [The student

reads

  it

 ou t ] W hich sen tences have

 you

 m a r k e d?

S :

  I

 have m arked

 the

 las t sentence

 in

 bo th paragraphs .

 The two

 tha t

talk abou t the concepts of "ship" and "paper."*

[*The last sentenc es

 in the two

 paragrap hs are : "The conce pt

 tha t

'ship'

 as a kind  o f  marine transportat ion vehicle  is  generalized

from   the comm on propert ies tha t all sh ips share," and "The c on-

cept of

 'paper'

 is a lso ge nera l ized

 from

  the com m on proper t ies

shared  by all  paper."]

T: Now I ask you, how does the general concept of "book" come about?

S: I t c o me s f rom the com m on proper ti es tha t are shared by  d i f ferent

kinds of books .

T :

  Then w ha t a re the com m on proper t ies o f books?

Ss:

  They

  are

 b o u n d

 by a

 cover.

T :

  A n d . . . ?

Ss: They are com posi t ions bou nd by a cover.

T:  These

 are the

 com m on properties that every book has . W ith these,

w e can generate the concept of book. The con cepts of

 "ship"

 and

"paper" a re also g enerated l ike this . Thu s, we can unders tand the

creation

 of a

 concept.

  S ,

 could

 you

 please read

 th e

 following para-

graph? [co nt inues to read, from "Comrade M ao Zedong" to

"these tw o books"] W hat sentences have y ou m arked?

Ss: [most s tuden ts] This one: "In select ion of w ords , one m us t be

clear about the meaning and the scope of meaning in order to

choose

  an

 accu ra t e

 and

  sui tab le word."

T :

  Have

 you

 no t iced wha t Com rade

 M ao

 said? This rem ark

 is

 in ter-

est ing. A ccording  to our theory, have you

 seen

 th e word "men?"

S s:

  [some] Yes,

 w e

 have, [some]

  N o , we

 h a ve n ' t,

 no t

 acco rd ing

 to

the

 book .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 66/255

2.

  V A R I A T I O N

  A N D T H E  S E C R E T  O F T H E  V I R T U O S O  53

T :  N o n e  of you  have seen  the  word

 "men."

  "Men"  is an abs t rac t

concept . W hat we see are Cheung , Lee , W ong, Ch iu , and so on .

They are concrete , real people. W hat i s the relation between the

concept "men"

  and all

 real people?

S:  A ll

 real people have general propert ies .

T :

  — The com m on proper ti es genera te a concept . Have you seen

the word "building?"

Ss:

  N o.

T: W e see the

  words "teaching block,"

 "classroom," and

 "our

  ow n

flats," w hich are our

 "homes."

 A ll these are the solid "bu ildings"

that

 w e can rea lly touch .

In

  t h i s exce rp t , t he t eache r r e f e r r ed back t o t he d i s cus s ion abou t

"books"

 a n d

 a sked s tuden t s

 to

 n a m e

  th e

 c o mm o n p r o p e r t ie s

  o f

  d i f fe ren t

kin ds of book s . Then the teac her gave a

 fu r ther

  i l lu s t r a t ion o f the con cep t

of the

 d i f fe ren t

  l evel o f ge nera l i ty o f w ords . For exam ple , "bu i ld ing" i s a t

a h ighe r l evel  o f ge ne ra l i ty  o r  abs t rac t ion than "teaching block," w h i ch

r e fe r s  to a  s p e c i f ic b u i l d i n g .

  Af te r c la r i fy ing

  th e  c o n c e p t ,  th e  t e a c h e r

g u i d e d

  the s tudents to apply the knowledge to an exerc i se in the t ex t -

b o o k .

In

 the

 las t par t

 o f the

  lesson,

  the

 teacher in t roduced another w ord re la-

t ion

  in

  semant ics , tha t

  is ,

 antonyms.

  H e

  first  asked s tudents

  to

  s u g g e s t

w ords w i th cont ras tive m ean ings (em inent -m ediocre , c lever-stup id , a r -

rogant-modest). B y

  search ing

  fo r

 examples

  to

 e xem pl i fy an tony m s , s t u -

dents foc used on the cr i t i ca l fea ture s o f these types o f wo rds— con t ras t in

m e a n i n g .

2.7

T:  Now we w i ll d i scuss another phenom enon in semant ics— con-

trast

  in

 m eaning . Could

 you

 please tel l

 m e,

 w h a t

 is the

 term

 for

words wi th contras t ive meaning?

Ss:  [all ] A ntonym s.

T :  Can you g ive m e some examples o f an tonym s?

S:  Em inent and m ediocre .

S:

  Clever and s tupid.

S:  Arrogant and m odest .

T: I t seems qui te easy for you to give examples  for an tonym s . W ha t

I real ly want you to   focus  on i s how m uch un ders tanding an t-

onyms

 can

 help

 in presenting our ideas? For

 example,

 when we

grasp the pair "arrogant" and "modest," h o w c a n we present our

unders tanding  o f  these antonyms? Comrade  M a o h a s  stated  ...

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 67/255

5 4 K O A N D   M A R T O N

S :

  "Modesty helps

  you

  improve, arrogance makes

 you

  decline."

T: Y ou see, w e express our ideas from both the posit ive and neg a-

tive p oint  of view. W hat  is the advantage  of doing this?

S:  W e can present our ideas com prehensively.

S:

  Thoroughly.

T: Co m prehensive, thorough, are both correct . A n addi t ional point

I

 w a n t to m ake : L anguage presented  in a thorough w ay requires

our own

  thorough th inking.

  It is

  better

  to

  consider

  a

  problem

from   both the posit ive and negative sides. Thu s, ma stering rich

antonym s can a lso help us to consider a problem thoroug hly. I

wil l

 w ri te som e w ords below

 and I

 w a n t

 you to

 tell

 m e

 their ant-

onym s. Then, please use these antonym s to com pose a sentence

in

 order to  show  th e meaning  of  these words.

[Teacher writes praise, arrogant , good .  Students state the ant-

onyms:

 condem n, modes ty , bad ,

 and the teacher w rites them down

on

  th e

  board.]

T :  Now , please  use  these words  to  make sentences. Remember,

present them

  in a

  thorough way.

S:

  It is

 good

 to be

 praised. How ever,

  it

 wo u l d

 be bad if we

  became

arrogant .

T: Good Then w hat should w e do after  being pra ised?

S:

  The

 m ore praise

 w e

 receive,

  th e

 more mo dest

 w e

 should

 be . W e

not

  only learn

  from

  criticism,

  w e also

 appreciate praise.

T: You  see,

 after

  learning these anton ym s, we can tackle a problem

thoroughly, and present our ideas met icu lous ly . OK, we wi l l  stop

here . I hope w e have a good com m and of

 an tonyms— help ing

 u s

toward a deta i led presenta t ion; the more antonyms we master ,

the  more complete  will  be our  meaning.  After  school,  please

read the rest of this intellectual short essay, "S em antics 1 ," and

m ark the im portant sentences.

T h e   Sys tema t i c

  Us e o f

  V a r i a t i o n

  in the

  Lesson

A t the beginning of this chapter, w e proposed that a systematic use of varia-

t ion m ight

 be a

 specific

  feature

 of

 Ch inese pedagogy,

 o r at

 least

 o f

 wh a t

 is re-

garded as good teach ing in Ch ina. The lesson "Sem antics," w hich was taught

by a  state-selected expert teacher, exem plified that the teac her use d variation

system atically to  create  an  optimal space of  learning for the students.

There

 are several

 different patterns

  of

 variation-invariance

 in the

  lesson.

For example, the story of  Afanti  highlighted a variation between w ord and

m eaning: tha t is , the concept tha t one word ("want")  s ignif ies  t wo m e a n -

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 68/255

2.

  V A R I A T I O N  A N D T H E  S EC R ET  OF T H E  V I R T U O S O  55

ings

 ("keep" and "give") was

 illustrated

 by

 making

 the

 word

 ("want")

 invari-

ant while varying the meanings ("keep" and

 "give").

 This allowed students to

discern

  a split between word  and meaning (cf.

 "separation,"

 mentioned  in

chap.

 1).

 When introducing

  the

 concept

 of

 synonyms,

 the

 variation

 of

 word

and meaning

 w as

 used again.

 T he

 concept

  of synonyms w as

 highlighted

 b y

keeping

 the

 meaning (e.g., "see") invariant

 and

 varying

 the

 words used

 to re-

fer to this meaning (e.g., "see," "look up, glance at, "look down ), which

al lowed students to see that th e same meaning could be represented by differ-

ent

 words. B y varying the relationship between word and meaning, students

were guided

 to

 discern

 the

 complex relationship

 a nd

 also

 to

 discern

 th e differ-

ence between homonyms and synonyms.

Homonyms:

Variation   Invariance

meaning word

Synonyms:

Variation   Invariance

word meaning

T h e

 awareness

  of the

  level

  o f

 generality

  o f

 words

 w as

 highlighted

 b y

another

 set of

 variation, namely "books"

 as a

 category,

 and

 d i f ferent  types

o f  books (e.g., Chinese book, reference book, and textbook). When the

teacher guided students to compare  the lexical meaning of "book" with

categories that were within

 the

 scope

 of

 meaning

 of "book,"

 students dis-

cerned that there were  d i f ferent  levels o f generality within th e same cate-

gor y o f words.

Variation

  Invariance

level of generality category ("books")

Another pattern

 of

 variation

 was

 used

 to

 illustrate

 the

 relationship

 be-

tween words  an d contexts. T h e teacher drew students' attention  to  d i f fer-

en t synonyms

 and

 their level

 of

 generality. This

 was

 done

 by

 keeping

 the

words (the

  d i f fe ren t

  synonyms) invariant  a n d varying  th e  contexts. Stu-

dents' attention

 was

  drawn

 to

  focus

 on the

 match

 or

 mismatch between

words

 and the

 d i f ferent contexts,

 an d

 they were also made aware

 o f the im -

portance

 of the

 usage

 of

 words

 in

 relation

 to the

 contexts. What

 is

 interest-

in g   i s the w ay in which th e teacher tried  to simultaneously bring into th e

students' awareness what

 was

 covered

 in the

 previous episode, that

 is, a

k ind

 of

 word relation that pertained

 to

 levels

 of

 generality,

 and the

 critical

semantic features of a word or a phrase.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 69/255

5 6 K O A N D

  M A R T O N

Variation

  Invar iance

con text synonym s ( i .e ., one synonym at a t im e)

When

  the

 dist inct ion between

  the

  synonyms

  w as

  clar if ied

  by

  m e a n s

  o f

showing their appropriateness to one context  but not to another,  the  teacher

also dem onstra ted that the appropria te synon ym s var ied w ith con text . This

is an example o f "fusion" as described  in chapte r  1

 (i .e., fusion

  between syn-

onym and context ) .

T he

  teacher constructed

  the

  lesson coherent ly

  by

  m e a n s

  o f

  correlated

var ia t ion. The l inguist ic concepts in this lesson were not introduced dis-

cretely;  but were  closely linked together  by the teacher's use of the  same

word

  as

  example

  or

  coun te r example .

 F or

  ins t ance ,

 the

 word

 "books"

 w as

used

  to

  i l lustra te concep ts th at have d i f ferent

  bu t

  re la ted  m e a n i n g s .

 I t w as

also used to il lustrate the   difference  be tween nar row and wide scope of

meaning (Chinese book, reference book, and books, respect ively) . And in

the la t ter par t of the lesson co ncerning synonym s, the exam ples book,

  text-

book, and Chinese book were referred  to again, a s a contrast  to a new set of

exam ples, pater ,  father,  daddy, and papa ,  in order to

  i l lustrate

 tha t the latter

shared the same scope of m ean ing. Likew ise the w ord "want" w as used in

the

 story

 o f

 Afan t i

 to

 i l lu stra te that

 th e

 w ord mean ing r e l a tion

 can be

 a m b i g -

uous , and was la te r used aga in as a coun te rexam ple of synonym s.

This

  analysis shows that

  the

  teacher used var ia t ion constant ly

 and

  sys-

tematically  to raise students' awareness of the

 richness

 of the meaning  of

words,

  as

 w e l l

 as the

  higher order structure

  o f

  words, such

 as

  h o m o n y m s ,

narrow-wide  scope  o f m eaning, spec if ic -genera l m ean ing, synonyms,  ge-

neric and

  specific

  re fe rence , and antonym s.

A N O T H E R

  TH E O RY

  OF VARIATION

In chapte r

  1 , we

 presented

  a

 theory

 o f

 learning tha t revolves aroun d

 the

 con-

cept of

  var iat ion.

  In this chapter, we described two  d i f ferent  examples of

teaching carried

  out by

 Chinese teachers

 w ho

 bo th made pedagog ica l ly

 so -

phist icated use of varia t ion. B asing our assessment  on the a fo rement ioned

theory, both teach ers  are doing a good job and can be expected  to m a k e u se

of var ia t ion in pedag ogical ly po w erful way s. I t shou ld be noted that another

theory

 o f

 learning based

  on

 variation

 w as

 presented long before

 our

 own,

 b y

a mathemat ics educa tor

  from

  Shan ghai , Gu Lin gy uan (Gu, 1 991) . Interest-

ingly,  unl ike our theory, which was der ived  from  our character izat ion of

learning in the edu cat iona l context , Gu developed his theory indu ctively by

observing cases  o f  good practice, that is, cases  o f teach ing tha t resul ted in

good learning. So , in a way, Gu 's theory  o f variation  is itself a depict ion of

features  o f good

 Chinese

  pedagogy.

Gu's theory

  is

  more psychological ly or iented than ours (which

 is an ex-

per ient ia l theory) , insofar as Gu is character izing learning—notably the

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 70/255

2 . V A R I A T I O N

  A N D T H E S E C R E T O F T H E V I R T U O S O 5 7

learning o f m athem at ics  in term s  of

 three

 consecutive, h ierarchically orga-

nized levels of functioning, or educational goals. The following brief ac-

count

 of som e parts of his theory is adm ittedly colored by the

  fact

  that one

theory (Gu's)  is  here seen through  the  lenses  of  another theory (our own).

W hat fo l low s is our understanding of Gu 's theory on the way in w hich the

m astery of the levels of m athem atical insights can be achieved by m eans of

consti tuting   different  patterns

  of

  variation.

1 .

  U n d e r s t a n d i n g

  th e  P r o c e d u r e s  o r

  Pri nc i p l es

This section

 is

 about

 th e

 ways

 in

 w hich

 it can be

 m ade possible

 for

 students

 to

discern

 th e

 critical features

 of a

 certain concept

 or

 principle.

 Due to the

  fact

that  Gu developed his theory within the field of geometry and only subse-

quently

 gen eralized it to other fields of m athem atics and to other school sub-

jects ,

 w e use the geom etrical i llustrations from G u's original pu blication.

a.  Widening  of  the concept , b y blocking o ut  eatures erroneously assumed

by  s tudents  to be  cri t ical features  of the  concept .  For

  instance, s tudents

most ly think in terms of s tandard f igures , tac i tly (and erron eously) assum -

ing that th e difference s between s tandard a nd nonstandard figures represent

critical

  feature s (see F ig. 2.1) .

In th is case, the variation builds on the teacher ' s understan ding of the s tu-

den ts ' understanding of the concept prior to the inst ruct ional sequen ce.

b. M a k in g

  it

 possible

 for the

  s tudents

  to

  discern what

  is

  taci t ly under-

s tood,  by

  me a n s

  o f

  contrast ing noninstances.

  F igure 2.2 is an example .

c. M aking i t possible  or the s tudents to m ake d is t inct ions be tween cases

that they t reat

 as

 m e m b e rs

  of  the

 same se t .

 Here , contrast ing cases

 are

 intro-

FIG. 2 . 1 .  Dist inguishing b etween essential an d nonessen t ial features by contrasting

standard

  and nonstandard

  figures.  From

 L . Gu  (1991) , Xuehui Jiaoxue

  [Learning

  to

teach],

 p. 68.

 Beij ing, PRC : Peop le 's Educat ion Press .

 Copy right ©

  1991

 by Gu

 L ing

Yuan.

  Reprinted with perm iss ion.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 71/255

58 K O A N D   M A R T O N

duced

  in

 order

 to

 make

  the

 dist inctions

  possible. T w o

 examp les

 are

  shown

in Fig. 2.3.

A ll  three of these  different  ways of contributing to make i t possible for

the students to discern the cri t ical features of the concept or principle in

quest ion,

 presuppose

  that

 the

 teacher

 has a

 good unders tanding

 of the

 stu-

dents 'preconce ived ideas and of their hab itual w ays of deal ing w i th the con-

cept. T he first pattern of variation corresponds  to gen eralizat ion  and the two

other patterns  to

  contrast

  as  defined in  chapter 1 .

2.

  U n d e r s t a n d i n g

  the

 Process

 of

  Fo rmi ng

 the

 Procedures

or

  Pri nc i p l es

This

 sect ion h as to do w ith Gu 's observation tha t s tudents

 f requent ly

  have

difficulties   discerning the figure-ground  structure o f geom etric form s, that

is,  w ith the qu est ion of discernm ent of wh oles and parts of pa rt icular in-

stances ra ther than the discernment of features across instances. This is

very m uch l ike the discernm ent (or del im ita t io n) of the deer and i ts parts

discussed

  in

 chapte r

  1 .

The dis tinct ion between the discernm ent of

 features

 and the discernm ent

(o r

 de l imi ta t ion)

 o f

 parts

  and

  wholes reminds

  us

  that

 the

  lat ter

 w as

  intro-

duced

 b y

  Svensson (1976)

 to

 characterize

 differences

  in the

 unders tanding

of the

 sam e text. (Svensso n arg ued tha t readers differ

  as to how

 they de l imi t

the

 w holes

 a nd

 parts

 a nd

 therefore  ma k e different senses

 of the

  same text.)

FIG.  2.2.  D i s t i ngu i sh i ng be tween

  the

 presence

  and

 absence

  o f

 essent ia l f ea tures

 by

contras t ing conceptua l  and  n o n c o n c e p t u a l

  f igures .

  From  L . Gu  ( 1 9 9 1 ) ,

  Xuehui

Jiaoxue   [Learning to teach] , p . 69. Be i j in g , PR C: People ' s Edu ca t ion Press . Copy-

r igh t  ©  1991 by Gu  L i n g Y u a n . R e p r i n t e d w i t h pe rmiss ion .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 72/255

2 . V A R I A T I O N   A N D T H E  S E C R E T  O F T H E  V I R T U O S O

59

F IG.  2 .3 . Foc us ing

  on

  e s se n t i a l f e a t u r e s

 by

  om i t t ing t he m . F r om

  L. Gu

  ( 1 9 9 1 ) ,

Xuehui

  Jiaoxue  [Lea rn ing  to  teach] ,  p. 70. B e i j i ng , PR C : P e op l e 's E du c a t i on P r e s s .

C o p y r i g h t

  ©

  1991

  by Gu

  L i ng Y ua n . R e p r i n te d w i t h pe r m i s s i on .

This dis t inct ion between the tw o form s of discernm ent i s discussed in m ore

detail in chapter 3.

T he

 k ind

 of

 variat ion th at

 Gu

 proposes

  to

 make

 i t

 po ssible

  for the

 s tudents

to

 discern the

 part-whole

 (and figure-ground) structure, is the trans form a-

t ion  o f the

  same

  figure

  through rotat ion

  and

 otherwise (see F ig. 2.4).

The s tudents can in th is w ay discern how com plex f igures are com prised

of s imple  f igures.  Another example  can be  seen  in F ig. 2.5.

FIG.  2.4. Se para t ing geom etr ica l ta rgets  f rom  c om p l e x b a c kg r ounds b y va r i a t i on .

F r o m L .

 G u

 ( 1 9 9

 \) ,

 Xuehui

 Jiaoxue

  [Lea rn ing

 t o

 t each] ,

 p . 73.

 B e i j i ng , PR C : P e op l e ' s

E duc a t i on   P r e s s . Copy r i gh t ©   1991 by Gu  L i ng Yua n . R e p r i n t e d  w i t h p e r m i s s io n .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 73/255

60

K O A N D

  M A R T O N

F I G .  2.5. Sep ara t ing com po nen t par ts o f geom et r i ca l  f igures b y

 s tr u c tu r a l \ a n a t i o n .

F r o m L . G u (199 ) ,X u e h u i J i a o x u e   [ L e a r n i n g to teach] , p. 74.

 B e i j in g ,

 P R C : P e o p l e ' s

Educa t ion

  Press . Copyr igh t

 ©

  1991

 by Gu

  L i ng Y ua n .

  Rep r i n t ed

  w i t h p e r m i s s i o n .

3.  D i s c o v e r i n g New

  Procedures

 and

  P r in c ip l e s

by  t h e S t u d e n t s Themselves

This is the  third form of variation in the  sense  that  here  th e  students produce

variation,

 or

 open their m inds

 to

 variation (rather than enc oun tering va riation),

within constrains

  posed  by

 questions. This type

  of

 variation

  is

  illustrated

 in

chapter

 3,

 when

 w e

 look

 at the

 open-ended problem

 of

 "the p ostm an's route."

VARIATION

  W I T H

  REPET I TI ON — TH E

  F E A T U R E

O F  G O O D TE AC HIN G  IN CH I N A

Unti l

 recently,

 teaching

  in

 m ainland China

 w as

 m ostly described

  as

 conser-

vative,

 textbook -oriented,

 and

 charac ter ized

 by

 teache r-centered rote learn -

ing

 (e.g. , Cleve rley, 1985; Se ybolt , 1973). Som e resea rch ers, su ch

 as

 L e u n g

(1991),  claimed that the conservat ive features mainly s temmed  from  tw o

tradit ions:  the C onfu cian pedagogica l t radi tion , and the Sovie t m ode of in-

struction. This resulted in a hybrid pattern of

 t each ing

 and l e a r n i n g

 tha t

 w as

more

  or less textbook-based, teacher-centered, and

  con ten t -over loaded .

T he

  predominance

  of

  drilling

  and

  repet i t ion

  in

  Chinese c lass rooms sug-

gested

  that Chinese teachers believed that repeti tion skil l dev elo pm en t

should precede interpretat ion (Gardner 1989).

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 74/255

2.

  V A R I A T I O N

  A N D T H E

  S E C R E T

  O F T H E

  V I R T U O S O

  6 ^

Som e studies, as represented by Pa ine's (1 990) work, described the dis-

t inctive

  teaching pract ice

  in

 modern China

  as

 being

  a "virtuoso model"—

hen ce the t i t le of this chapter. In Ch ina, direct w hole-group instruction an d

teacher ta lk were found to be prevalent in classrooms. Paine (1990) ob-

served that

  "It is

  this f lair—a special t iming,

  an

  elegance

  of

  language ,

  a

power

 of

 expression— that dis tinguishes

 the

 great teach er

 from the

 ordinary

one '

1

  (p. 69).

S he

  recognized

  the

  shortcomings

  of

  this model

  in the

  sense that

  the

teacher plays an active role whereas the students have a passive role; the

teacher i s the actor, the s tudents the audienc e. She com m ents that

A l t h o u g h t e ache r s in C h i n a f r equen t ly v o iced conce rn abou t r e ach ing th e s tud en t s ac -

ademica l ly , t h e

  l i te ra ture

 and in te rv iews w i t h t e ache r s sugges t tha t th e m a i n i n t e l l ec-

t u a l

  th rus t

  o f t each ing  c e n t e r s  on the  t e a c h e r s ' p e r f o r m a n c e  an d  m i n i m i z e s o r

i nadver t en t ly neg lec t s

 th e

  in t e rac tiona l  p o t e n t i a l

 o f

 c l a s s room expe r i ence . A da p t i ng

t e ach in g  to par t i cu la r  aud iences , t hou gh c l ea r ly th e m a r k  of a  good teach er , is no t a

necessa ry  r e qu i r e m e n t of the  f ine  c l a ss room pe r fo rm er , given  the d o m i n a n t co n c e p -

t ion

  o f the

  t e ache r ' s

 role .

 ( Pa in e , 1990,

 p. 6 8)

However, Paine admits that despite

  the

  negat ive impl icat ion

  o f

  th is

mo de l ,  the  emphasis  on  teaching  as a  vir tuoso performance incorporated

both a

 recognition

 o f the importance of

 know ledge

  and an

  acknowledgment

of the  role  of  personal , humanis t ic qual i t ies  o f  aesthetics,  af fect ion ,  and

com m itm ent in t each ing .

Cheng (1 992)

 had

 similar view s

 on the

 pract ice

 o f

 t each ing

 in

 China ,

 and

used  the metaphor of "drama"  to describe it:

So m e t im es , I  t h i n k ,  to  t e ach  a  lesson  in  C h i n a  is to be  l ike  an  a c t o r p e r f o r m i n g a

dr a ma .  E v e r y t h i n g s h o u l d be p l a n n e d in deta i l ; ev e ry ac t ion shou ld fol low  the  scr ipt .

T h e r e

 is no

 room

 for any

 m i s ta k e s .

 T he

 lesson plan

 is

 j u s t

 l ike

 the scrip t of a

 d r a m a ,

 (p .

107,

  in

 C h in es e ,

 ou r

  t rans la t ion )

Cheng (1992) believed that the emphasis on strict lesson planning may

have

  an

  undesirable

  effect ,  as it

  reduces

  the

  opportunit ies

  for

  students

  to

contribute to the plan ning o f the lesson. H ow ever, Cheng points out that the

emphas i s

  on

  serious lesson planning reflects

  the

  conscientious at t i tude

 of

teache rs in China that contributes to m aintainin g the quali ty of the teac hing

profess ion

  in the

 country .

It

  sho uld be noted th at there is also other— m ostly more recent— litera-

ture that portrays

  the

 Chinese classroom

  as

 interactive

 and effective.

  These

works argue that

  the

  whole-class ins t ruct ion method commonly

  found  in

Chinese classrooms al low s each chi ld to have the m axim um opportunity to

benef i t

  from  the

 teacher,

 an d to

 enhance conceptual unders tanding;

  and

 tha t

it is this that contributes to the excellent perform ance of Chinese students in

international academic campaigns (Stevenson   &  Lee, 1997). These  re-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 75/255

62 KO AND  M A R I O N

searchers have also found that Chinese classrooms are active,  and  that stu-

dents participate  in though t-provoking tasks o r ques t ion ing (Co rtazzi 1998;

Cortazzi

  &

  Jin, 2001). Some  researchers

 are

  also interested

  in

 t racing

  the

historical origin

  o f

  some

  of the

  dist inct ive perceptions

  o f

  t e ach ing

  and

learning in Chinese c lassrooms. They  ident i fy  the impact of  Conf uc i a n

views of teaching and learning on educat ion in C hin a and some o ther East

A sian coun tr ies , as the  C onfuc ian-her i t age-cu l tu re  (CH C) l ea rne r phenom -

enon (e.g., Biggs, 1996;

  Ho,

  1991).

 These

 researchers

  try to

 associa te som e

of the dom inan t learning approaches adopted by studen ts in the East A sian

countr ies w ith the C onf uc i an  cul ture . They p oin t out tha t a l th ou gh Ch inese

classrooms

 are

 apparently

  full  o f

 rote learning,

 the

 repetit ion

  is

 used

 by the

learners

  as a

 means

  to

 enhance unders tan ding. These researchers a rgue tha t

the emphas is on repe t i t ion and memor iza t ion should not s imply be d is-

m issed as conservative , as Ch inese teachers a lso em ph asize reci ta t ion w ith

ref lect ion,

 and

 m em orizat ion w ith un derstan ding (B iggs, 1 996; Lee , 1996) .

B y

  us ing

 a

  combina t ion

 of the

  variation theory

  o f

  lea rn ing presented

  in

chapte r  1 , and  Gu 's theory o f  variation as a con cep tua l f ram ew ork , Hu ang

(2002) recently character ized mathematics teaching in China (more pre-

c ise ly in S hangha i ) as be ing both h ig hly teacher -cente red and h igh ly s tu-

dent-centered ( if we use "teacher-centered" in the sense of the enacted

object

  of

 learning

 being close to the

  intended object

  of

 learning,

  and

  "stu-

dent-centered" in the sense of the s tudents owning the space of learning,

that is , tha t they par t ic ipate in br inging it abou t) . Those lessons

 tha t

 a re con-

sidered

  to be

 good

  in

 China

 are

 p lanned, cho reographed,

  and

 w e l l t h o u g h t -

out  lessons.  B ut  their e laborate design never theless  stil l offers  p lenty  o f

space  for the  s tuden t s ' ow n  independen t  and  spon taneou s ideas .  T he  s tu-

dents

 a re

 h ighly ac t ive

 and

 they

 pay

 a t tent ion

 to the

 teacher .

 In a

 s imi la r way .

the teacher, having thoroughly orchestrated the lesson, can   fully  focus  on

the var ious ideas the s tudents com e up w ith. B y usin g her previou s exper i-

ences

  and her

 know ledge

 o f the

 w ays tha t

 the

 s tudents often  th ink a b o u t

 the

specific objects  o f  learning,  the teacher  is able  to  ant ic ipa te wh at  the  stu-

dents

  are

  going

  to

  come

  up

  wi th spontaneously .

  T he

  m a them a t ic s le sson

from

  a  Shanghai c lassroom descr ibed  in  chapte r  3. is ano the r example o f

this

 way o f

 teaching.

 A s w e

 a l ready m ent ioned , Pa ine (1 990) a rgued tha t

 in

the Chinese c lassroom , the teacher is the perform ing actor w hereas the s tu-

dents m ake up the pass ive audience . A more accu ra te m etaphor w ou ld be ,

w e believe,  to see the teacher as the director,  and the s tudents as the actors

playing

  in accordance

 w i th

  a

 script  that they ha ve never seen.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 76/255

3

D i s c e r n m e n t

an d   th e Question,

W h a t

  Can Be

  L e a r n e d ?

U l l a   Runesson

I d a   A. C. Mok

In

 chapter 2, the pr imary focus was on variat ion. In this cha pter, th e

 focus

 is

m ain ly on

 d iscernm ent .

  In

 accordance w i th

 the

 theory

 p ut

 forward

 in

 chap-

ter 1 , we argue that a cer tain w ay of learning, of unders tand ing or "seeing" a

part icular  phenomenon,  means that certain  cri t ical features must  be  dis-

cerned a nd held in our awareness s imul taneous ly . Hence , the possib il i ty for

the

  learner

 to

 discern

 o r

 focus

 on

 these

  features  is a

 necessary c ondi t ion

 fo r

lea rn ing som eth ing in a cer tain w ay. W e i l lus t rate th is theory us ing  different

examples

  from

 mathematics lessons.

DISCERNMENT,  E X P E R I E N C E ,

  AN D

  M E A N I N G

In

 chapter 1 , exam ples w ere given that i l lus t rated how different profess ion-

als m ust pay at tent ion or mu st be sensit ive to cer tain aspects o f a s i tuat ion in

order

 to

 handle

 the

 s i tuat ion

 in a

 cer tain w ay.

 T he

 ability

 to

 handle

 a

 si tua-

t ion

 c ou ld be descr ibed in terms o f the w ay in wh ich the s i tuat ion is seen or

exp erienced ; in other wo rds, the origin of

 pow erfu l

 w ays of ac ting is power-

fu l  w a y s o f  seeing.

The w ay in w hich a s i tuat ion is seen or experienced depends on the fea-

tures of the  situation that are discerned. In every situation, we can attend to

all

 the

 different  aspects

 of the

 si tuation . How ever, this

 is not

 wh at happens .

W e

 do not at tend to al l these asp ects and w e do not at tend to them at the sam e

t ime .  Ins tead, we pay part icular a t tent ion to some aspects—that i s , some

63

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 77/255

64  R U N E S S O N AND MOK

features

 of the si tuation are discerned, w herea s othe rs are not . Th e  features

that w e  discern  are in the  fore of our  at tent ion, or put  differently,  they are

held

 in our

  focal

 awareness; and

 they

 are

 held

  in our

 awareness

  at the

  same

t ime.

 W e can say

 therefore, tha t

 a

 certain

 w ay of

 exp er iencing could

 be

 cha r -

acterized  in  term s o f the  aspects that a re discerned s im ul tane ous ly .

T he

 par t icular

 way in

 w h i c h

 the individual

 experiences

 a

 s itua t ion— or

 a

phenomenon—is

  the way the

  individual unders tands

  it.

 E x p e r i e n c in g

 im -

plies experiencing   the  object  as  som eth ing ,  t hus  experiencing  a m e a n in g .

When

  we use the

  concept

  a way o f exper ienc ing someth ing  and

  describe

how

 som eth ing

 is

 experienced,

  w e

 describe

  the

 m ean ing t ha t t h i s som eth ing

has for the individual. That w hich appears to be the same thing could have a

different

  mean ing

  for

  different

  individual s .

 It

  could also have

  different

mean ings  for the same individua l at different  t im es . W ha t m ean ing it has for

the individual cou ld be unders tood in terms of wh ich aspects of the object

are

  discerned

  and

 held

  in the

  focal  awareness s im u l t aneous ly .

  One way of

experiencing impl ies  the  s imu l t aneous d i s ce rnmen t  o f  cer tain features ,

w hereas another w a y o f exper iencing im pl ies th e s imu l t an eo u s d i s c e rn m e n t

of

 other features .

W e

 leam  to experience by  discerning aspects  of the object  to w h i ch w e

direct

 o u r

 aw areness.

 In

 order

 to

 gain

 a

 cer ta in un ders tan ding

 o f a

 p h e n o me -

non ,

 w e

 m u s t

 be

 able

 to

 discern cer tain cr i t ical features . Fo l low ing this l ine

of reasoning, if w e, as educators, w ant our students  to gain a p ar t icular w ay

of unders tanding, o r to develop a par t icular ca pabi l i ty , w e m u s t m a k e it pos-

sible

 f o r

 them

 to

 discern feature s that

 are

 cr i t ical

 fo r

 tha t par t i cu lar l earn ing .

D I S C E R N I N G C R I T I C A L F E A T U R E S

  O F  WHAT  IS TAUG H T

Wh a t

 do we

 me a n

 by

 crit ical features '?

  And how can d i f ferent

  w a y s

 of

 h a n -

dling

 these cr i t ical feature s

 in the

 l earn ing s i tua t ion provide

 d i f ferent

  possi-

bilities

  for

 learning?  Learning what

  a

  square is—and what

  it is

  no t—for

instance, takes

  the

  di scernment

 o f the

 cr i t ical featu res

 o f

  that geom etr ical

shape. Consequent ly ,  if the teacher a im s a t m a k in g the  learner un ders tan d

wha t

 a square is, he o r she

 mu s t start wi th w hat

  it

 takes

  to

 know

  and recog-

nize

 the

  cri t ical features

 o f a

 square,

  in

 order

  to

 m ake learn ing poss ib le .

 In

this case,

 the

 cri t ical fea tures

 are the

 size

 of the

 angles ,

 th e

 n u m b e r

 o f

 sides,

and the relat ions betw een them , and i t m ust be possible for the learners to

discern these features . How ever , s im ply po int ing

 ou t

 these cr i t ica l features

to   the learners  is not  enough .

W e i llus t rate th is po int w i th a t hou gh t experimen t . Le t us  i ma g i n e tw o

dif ferent  c lass rooms wi th

  tw o  dif ferent

  t eachers , bo th in tending

 t o

  he lp

their students learn w h a t a square is. They both ta lk about the  characteris-

t ics of a square and refe r to a pictu re of a square. In that respect the lessons

a re

 s imi lar .

 But le t us

 im ag ine t ha t

 th e first

  t eache r s im p ly po in t s

 to the an -

gles, stating that they   are four righ t angles; po ints to the  fou r sides, show-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 78/255

3.  D I S C E R N M E N T

 A N D

  " W H A T

  C A N B E

  L E A R N E D ? "

  6 5

ing the

  relat ion  be t w e e n

  th e

  sides ( i .e. , equal);

  an d

  f inal ly  g ives

  a

defin i t ion  o f a

  square .

  T he

  second teacher s imi lar ly points out—or

  fo-

cuses on—the ang les ,

 th e

 sides

 and the

 re la tion be tween them,

 b u t

 also

 in -

t roduces a variat ion of these aspects . F or ins tance, she draws a pictu re of a

rhombus bes ide

  the

  square

  an d

 draw s

  th e

 s tuden ts ' a ttent ion

 to the  differ-

ence be tween   th e  angles o f the  square  and the  angles  o f the r h o mb u s  re -

spect ively.  Next she compares the number o f s ides in a square wi th the

number of ' s ides

  in a

 t r i ang le .

  In the

  same way,

  she

 con tras ts

  the

 re la t ion

betw een the opposi te s ides of a square and a rectan gle. F inal ly , the teac he r

points to squares of d i f ferent  sizes.

I f w e

 take

  a

 closer look

 a t how

  that w hich

 w as

  t augh t

 w as

  handled,

 and

w h a t

 i t was

 po ssible

  to

  learn,

  w e

  find

  that

 the

  same cr i t ical features

 of the

square w ere fo cuse d on in both lessons (ang les , num ber of s ides , and the re-

lat ion

 betw een them ). How ever, w hereas

  the

 second teacher indica ted that

the

 size of the angles, the num ber of sides, and the relat ion betwee n the sides

cou ld  be  different  be tween what  is a  square  and  w h a t  is not a  square, her

counterpart

  did

  not .

  In

  th is way,

  a

  variat ion

  of the

  cr i t ical features

  w as

broug ht to the at tent ion of the s tudents by m eans of contras ts . The teache r

also gave exam ples

 o f

 different  squares ( i .e., squares with different  area) .

 In

this

 way ,

 the

  geometrical shape

 w as

 con stant,

 but the

 size

 of the

 shape

 w as

varied. In the lesson taugh t by the

 first

  teacher, how ever, such variation w as

no t in t roduced. I t w as not possible for the learners to experience the fact that

the

  size

 of an

 angle c ou ld vary. N either

 was i t

 possible

  for

 them

  to

  experi-

ence  the

  fact

  tha t  the  same shape could have

  different

  sizes,  but  still  be

cal led

  a square .

Accord ing

  to the

  theoret ical f ramework laid down

  in

  chapter

  1 ,  tha t

w h i c h  varies

 is

 likely

 to be

 discerned.

 F or

 ins tance ,

 it is

 necessary

  to

 know

w h a t

 a

 right angle

 is

 no t ,

 in

 order

 to

 learn w hat

 i t

 ac tua l ly

 is. And it

 takes

 the

experience

 of (at

 leas t )

 tw o

 different

  examples

 of

 squ ares ( i .e. , variat ion

 of

the  area)

  to

  unders tand what

  a

  square

  is. So, in

  other words ,

  the

  space

  of

var iat ion

  afforded

  o r

  const i tu ted—part ly wi th

  the

  learners—is cr i t ical

 fo r

the po ssibil i ty to discern these cri t ical fe ature s. A certain pattern o f varia-

t ion  is

 necessary

 fo r a

 certain learning

 to

 happen .

  So , in

 answer

 to the

 ques-

t ion , "How  can learning best be promoted  for m y studen ts?," w e w ould say ,

"Take

 as yo ur point of departure the ca pab i l it ies you wa nt them to develop.

W hat do you w ant you r students to  learn? W hat  is cri t ical for th is learnin g

and  fo r

 this

 way o f

 u nders tanding? Make

  it

 po ssible

  for the

  learners

  to

 dis-

cern those features that

  are

 cri t ical

 for

  that learning."

However , we

  must s t ress that

  we a r e no t

  say ing tha t what

  is

  t a u g h t

 is

necessa r i ly w hat i s learned by the s tuden ts . From our point of view, there

is no

 cau sa l i t y be tween t each ing

 an d

 l earn ing . That

  is ,

 there

 is no

 gua ran-

tee

 t ha t a par t i cu lar l earn ing w i l l t ake p lace s imply beca use t eachers ac t in

a ce r ta in way , o r  s t ructure  th e  learning s i tuat ion in a par t icu lar w ay . B ut ,

w h a t

 t eache r s

 can

 accom pl i sh

 is to

 create

 t h e

 pos s ib i l i ty

 o f

 learn ing som e-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 79/255

66  R U N E S S O N  AND MOK

thing  in a cer ta in w ay. It is not

 possible

 to say co nc lus ive ly how the t e ache r

af fects  what s tudents

  leam

  s imply by looking a t the way in which the

teacher teaches  th e

 lesson.

 How ever ,  it is po ss ib le  to say w ha t s tuden t s are

afforded

  to learn in that pa r t icula r learning si tuat io n to acc ou nt for

  d i f fer-

ences  in pos sibi l i ties for learning , and to show h ow th ese are ref lec ted in

what s tudents ac tua l ly lea rn .

STUDENTS'

  A C H I E V E M E N T S

AN D  POSSIBILITIES FO R L E A RN I N G

In   1992, Stevenson  and St igler published some rem arkable findings in their

book,  The

 Learning

 G a p.

 These findings indicated more  or less tha t previ-

ous

 specula t ions— tha t

  is ,

 tha t

 a

 lea rner 's achievem ent

 is af fected  by  differ-

ences

 in

 w h a t

 it is possible to

 discern—seem ed

  to be

 correct .

 In tw o

 studies,

Stevenson and St igler (1992) com pared achievem ent in m ath em atics in pr i-

m ary Grade 1 and pr imary Grade 5 s tudents  from  a num ber of schools in

Sendai, Taipei,

  and

 M inneapolis ;

  and in

 Sen dai , Taipei , B e i j ing ,

  and

  Chi -

cago, respec t ive ly . Al though the Amer ican s tudents a l ready se r ious ly

lagged behind  the  Asian s tudents  in  pr imary Grade  1 , by the  t ime they

reached primary Grade

 5, the gap had

 w idened

 so

 m u c h

 tha t

 there

 w as

 bas i -

cal ly no overlap;  the per form ance  in the bes t A m er ican schools w as w e a k er

than the perform ance in the w eakest o f the A sian sch ools .

Several other comparative studies (e.g. ,  the Third In te rna t iona l M athe-

ma t i c s  and  Science Study [TIMSS], 1999;  see  M u l l i s  et al ,  2000) hav e

demon stra ted that Japanese s tudents ,

  and

  Chinese s tudents

  from

  H o n g

Kong and Ta iwan, do m uch be t te r in m athe m at ics than Am er ican s tudents .

There

 were ser ious a t tem pts to acco un t for such dif ferences .  On e of these

was the TIMS S-video s tudy, in w hich a great nu m ber of ma them at ics les-

sons in Germany, Japa n, and the Un ited Sta tes were recorded  and in tense ly

scrutinized. Stigler and Hiebert followed up   The Learn ing G ap  (Stevenson

&

  Stigler, 1992)

  by

  publishing their book,  The Teach ing Gap,

  in

  1999

(Stigler

 &

 Hie bert, 1999). Th eir

 w a y o f

 describing

 the

 typic al Japan ese les-

son and the typical A m erican lesson   offers  a potent ia l exp lana t ion of the ob-

se rved d i f f e r ences in ach ievement be tween Japanese and Amer ican

students.

In a typical Japanese lesson,

  af ter

  having reviewed the previo us lesson,

the

 teacher introdu ces

 the

 problem

  of the

 day:

 a

 p rob lem comp lex enough

 to

be  used  as a  f ramework  for the  entire lesson  and for e labora t ing  d i f ferent

ideas and procedures.  A s a rule,  the students first work on the problem indi-

vidual ly

  and in a num ber of different  w ays , and then cont in ue the wo rk in

groups. Eventual ly  different  groups  present their solut ions

  and

 these

  are

compared .  T he  t e ache r—and  the  s tudents  as  w e l l — c o m m e n t  on the

strength an d weakn esses of dif ferent  approaches.  Final ly , the teacher sum-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 80/255

3.

  D I S C E R N M E N T

  A N D

  " W H A T

  C A N B E

  L E A R N E D ? "

  6 7

m arizes the work and points to the most pow erfu l  ideas that have come up

during the lesson.

In a

 typical Am erican lesson,

 the

 teac her also starts

 by

 review ing

 the

 pre-

vious

 lesson.

 Then

 the teacher m ay int roduce

  definit ions,

  term s and, above

all , a method for solving a certain k ind of problem . Af ter the dem ons t ra tion ,

the

  s tudents are given a relat ively large num ber of problem s of the same

kind

 to pract ice on. After they have prac ticed these, another type o f pro blem

m ay

 be

 in t roduce d.

 T he

 m ethod

  fo r

 solving this kind

 of

 problem

  is

 demon-

strated

 by the teacher, and the students are given a further  set of problem s of

this

 other kind

  to

 pract ice

  on .

M a t h e m a t i c s  lessons

  in

  China have been described

  in

  s imi lar terms

  as

Stigler

 and

 Hiebert

 described

 the

 Japanese lesson

 (see

 Stevenson

 &

 Stigler,

1992).

 An d to a

 certain extent ,

 it

 wou ld

 be

 true

 to say

 that

 th e difference s

 j u s t

i l lustrated   could potent ial ly provide  an  explanation  of the  differences  in

achievement

  between Asian

  and

  American s tudents .

If w e

  s impl i fy  th ings somewhat ,

  we can

 argue that

 an

 im por tan t  differ-

ence  betw een Chinese an d Japanese m athem at ics lessons on one hand, and

Amer ican

 lessons

 on the

 other,

 is

 that

 in the

 former,

 the

 s tudents most ly

  face

one

 problem

 to

 w hich they

 are

 asked

 to f ind different

  solut ions , w hereas

 in

the latter, the students are presented wi th one method fo r solving one kind of

p rob lem,

  which they then prac t i ce

  by

  solving

  different

  problems

  of the

same kind.

In

 each case ,

 t he

 s tudents were  af forded  dif ferent  poss ib i l i t ies

 fo r

 w h a t

cou ld b e

 learned.

 In one

 type

 o f

 lesson, s tuden ts w ere given

 the

 poss ibi l i ty

to

 learn

 how to f ind

 solut ions

 to a

 p rob lem , w hereas

 in the

 other , they w ere

af forded  the po ss ib i l i ty to learn ho w to m ake use of a pa r t icula r solut io n.

In

  these s tudies , comparisons were made using lessons wi th

  different

mathemat i ca l

 co ntents . Thus, the comparison w as m ade o n a general

  level.

In

 the

 fo l lowing

  example ,

 tw o

 lessons tha t aim ed

 to

 teach

 the

 same

  specific

conten t  are

 com pared

  in

 relat ion

  to

 w h a t

 w as

 possible

  to

  learn.

L E A R N I N G

  TH E  OPERATOR

 ASPECT

O F TH E FRACTION AL

 C O N C E PT

R une sson (1999) s tudied f ive m a them at i c s le s sons , a ll t each ing  f rac t iona l

num bers an d percen tages . In many respect s , the l essons w ere very s imi lar .

F o r

  i n s t ance ,

  four  o f

  them used

  the

  same textbook,

  and the

  c lass room

w ork was orga n ized in a very s imi lar way . The a im of Ru nesso n ' s  s tudy

w as to

  invest igate

 th e

  var ious ways

  in

  which teachers handle conten t ,

 in

th is case,  f ract ional  n u mb e r s

  an d

 percen tages .

  Le t us

 take

 a

 closer loo k

 a t

tw o of the

 teachers.

In both lessons, the content being taught was the operator aspect o f the

fract ional co ncept (e.g. , how to f ind 1 /3 of  12). In both cases, the discussio n

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 81/255

68

R U N E S S O N

  A N D M O K

took place

 in a

 whole-class

 setting. The lessons were  audiorecorded and

transcribed verbatim.

 T he

  d ia logue

  is

 p resen ted a longs ide

 o ur

 analys i s :

3.1

  [Mathematics

  Lesson

 /

 F ract ions/

  Secondary Grade

  1 ]

Teacher

  A

T: OK.  Here  I  have  a  piece  of

string. It's

 90

 cent imeters long.

[Teacher holds  up a  piece  of

string]  Three  people have

  to

share that equally. How do you

go about that? Fair share? Tell

m e,

  Sylvia .

S 1 :

  Wel l  ...  divide by  three.

T :

  Yeah, each

 o ne

 w i ll

 get a

 th ird .

B ut let's say one of them wants

to have more than   the  others .

The str ing is

  stil l

  90 cen t ime-

ters  long  and I  want 2/3. H ow

cou ld we f igure tha t out? 2/3 of

a

 piec e of str ing tha t is 90 cen-

timeters long?

 Thomas?

S 2:

  [ inaud ib le]

T:

  R ight. First

  you

  f igure

  out the

length

  o f a

  third

  and

  then take

another

  one . . . and

  together

that makes

  ... ?

 W h a t

  d id you

say?

  60

  cen t im eter s? Yes.

  So,

first  you

  have

  to figure out the

length   of  1/3. Measure that,

an d

  then t ake ano the r one .

[The teacher

  first

  marks 1/3 ,

then 2/3 of the whole length  o f

the  str ing] .

T: OK.

  Let ' s take

  a

  look

  a t

  th is

piece

  of

  string. [The teacher

  is

hold ing   up a

  shorter piece

  o f

str ing]  This  is  on ly  40  cent i-

meters long. I would l ike to

have

  one  fifth  of 40

  cen t ime-

ters . [Wri t ing   on the  b l ack -

board:  1/5 of 40 cm]

The  teacher  introduces  the

problem.

 A man ipulative aid is

used.

 A

 s trategy

 fo r

  solving

  the

p r ob l e m   (1/3

  of 90) is

  intro-

duced.

The

 nom inator  is changed;  1/3

is   changed  to 2/3  (2/3  o f  90).

The

  teacher  elucidates  the

strategy   an d  illustrates with

the  manipulative aid. (1/3  o f

90   =  90/3  =  30)

A

  ne\v

 problem

  (1/5

  of 40) is

introduced.  A  manipulative

aid is

  used.

Writ ten

  representation

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 82/255

D I S C E R N M E N T  A N D  " W H A T  C A N B E  L E A R NE D ' . ' "

69

S 2:  8 cen t im eter s .

T :

  Yes, each  fifth is 8  cen t ime-

ters .

 B u t le t 's s ay we wan t 3/5.

How do you f igu re   that out?

Tell  m e, L i s a .

S 3:  Th r ee t imes 8.

T : O K .  Firs t  w e  m u s t  f igure out

how   m u c h  1/5 is. So, you di-

vide  40 by 5, and you get 8.

A n d

  three f i f ths m ust be three

t i m e s

  a s m u c h .

  Three

  s u c h

p i e c e s . Th at ' s 24. B ut le t 's say

t ha t  the piece of str ing is 60

c e n t i m e t e r s   in s t ead . [Wr i t e s

60 cm on the board] . One of

y ou

  should have 3/5,

  and an-

other  o n e

 2/5.

 H o w m u c h w i l l

the person  w h o  gets  3/5 have?

...

  O K . H o w d o w e g o  abou t

th is? T h e w ho le p iece o f string

is

 60.1 shou ld have 3/5, then

 I

m u s t

  f i g u r e  s o m e t h i n g

  o u t

f i rs t , w h a t  .. . ? M ar tin .

S4: 5 d ivided by 60

T :  Wel l , now you  said it the other

w ay

 around— 60 d iv ided by 5.

W hat ' s t ha t?

S5: 12

T :  OK. 12 . So now we know tha t

1 /5   is 12. Then  how  m u c h  is

3/5?

The

 n o m i n a to r  is changed (3/5

of  40).  The

  teacher asks

 for an

appropriate

  s t ra tegy.

The  teacher elucidates

  the

s tra tegy .

The

  who le

  is

  changed

  (3/5  o f

60).

  W ri tt en represen ta tion

The

  teacher asks

  for the ap-

propria te s t ra tegy.

I f we

 an alyze this data

 in

 term s

 o f the

 aspects focus ed

 on , it is

 apparent

 tha t

the

  focus

  was on the

  strategy

  fo r

  solving

  the

  problem ( i .e . , calc ulat ing

 the

length   of a fractional part of a piece of str ing) .

 This

  is the aspect that the

teach er tr ied to draw the students ' at tention to. B ut only one strategy or proce-

dure

 wa s presen ted, and hence the strategy w as not varied. This particular as-

pec t was foc use d on, but w as kept invariant. However, the teacher did chang e

the  param eters  in the problem .  After  introducing  the first problem  and pre-

senting an appropriate solving strategy, the teacher ch anged the length of the

piece of siring (i .e. , the wh ole) a s we ll as the size of the fractional part (1/5 o f

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 83/255

7

R U N E S S O N

  A N D M O K

40).

 In the fo l lowing

  example ,

  the

 num era to r

 w as

 changed (3/5

 o f

 40),

 an d fi-

nally  in the last example,  the who le ( i .e ., the length of the string) w as changed .

Thus,  the

  strategy

  w as

  invariant , while

  the

  numbers var ied

  in a

  sys tema t ic

way.

 (1 /3

 of 90, 2/3 of 90, 1 /5 of 40, 3/5 of 40, 3/5 of 60)

Let us now  contrast th is approach w i th that o f Teacher  B , wh o se t each in g

also deal t wi th

  th e

  operator aspect

  o f  f rac t ional

  n u m b e r s .

  Th e day

 be fo re

this lesson, the

 students

 had been working on a

 p ar t icu lar

 problem in

 w h i ch

they had to mark 3/7 of a recta ng le w i th a size of 7 x 8 squares , as shown in

Fig. 3.1 .

3.2 [Ma them at ics /Frac tions /Pr im ary Grade   6 ]

Teacher  B

The

  rectangle

  is

  shown

  on an

O H T T o

 begin w i th

 the

  teacher

asks Lena to

  tell

  the class how

she

 marked

 3/7 of the

 rectangle.

SI: If you just take seven squares

from the

  whole,

  and

  then take

three of those ... If you count

"one, two, three,"

 and

 mark them.

T:

  W h y ?

SI:  Well  it is 3/7 of the  small  pile.

And then

  I

  continue: one, two,

three, four,

 five,

 six, seven,

 go on

like that.

 I

 keep counting

 to

 seven

and

 m arking three of them .

A

  manipulat ive

  aid is

  used .

In   each group  of seven squares ,

three are m arked .

The  teacher asks

 fo r an

  argu-

ment.

The  s tudent i s explaining her

s tra tegy .

FI G.  3 . 1 .  T he  grid  fo r m a r k i n g 3/7 of 56.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 84/255

3.  D I S C E R N M E N T  A N D

  " W H A T

  C A N B E

  L E A R N E D ?"

71

T:  Oh yeah . I unders tand You

counted one, two, three,

  four,

five   six, seven,  and  then  you

marked th ree

  o f

  t h e m.

  A n d

t hen one , two , t h ree , fou r ,

f i v e ,

  s i x , s e v e n ,  a n d y o u

m arked them . In o ther words ,

you do it

  l ike this [pointing

 a t

the pro jec t ion on the board]

one, two, three,  four, five, six,

seven.

  You can

  ma r k

  the

  las t

ones l ike tha t . How do you go

on? In the same w ay?

S I:  Yes .

T:  Wel l  did  anyone  do it  differ-

ently?

  Did you all do it  like

that?

Ss:  N o.

T :

  Well  what  about you ...

Sophie?

S 2:

  We l l ,

  I

  j u s t d iv ided

  it

  in to

seven  parts .

T: OK. You

 jus t counted

  all the

squares and divided them into

seven. OK, Maria what about

you?

S 3:  Wel l  I  tried

  different

  numbers

like that unt i l I got seven parts .

The  teacher e lucidates Lena 's

s tra tegy.

A  manipula t ive  aid is  used .

The

  teache r asks for a l terna-

t ive s tra tegies .

Another

  s tudent expla ins

  her

strategy,  which

  is

  different

f rom   the

 previous

 o n e .

The

  teacher asks for a l terna-

t ive s tra tegies .

Yet

  another s tudent exp la ins

her

  s tra teg y, which

  is  different

f rom   the prev ious one .

Here, Teacher

  B

  focused

  on the

  solving strategy

  in a

  s imi lar

  way to

Teacher A. Thus, the foc used aspects w ere the same in both lessons . B ut , w e

can

  observe that Teacher B asked the students to come up with

  different

solving strategies to the sam e problem (3/7 of 56). The variation in the stu-

den ts ' solving s t rategies was m ade exp l ici t w hen the teacher asked: "Did

you al l do i t

 l ike that?"

 T he

 studen ts ' responses show ed that they

 h ad

 c o me

up w i th  different  w ays of solving the problem. Lena solved the problem by

count ing seven squares in each column and then marking three of them.

So phie divided the 56 squares into seven parts (and m ult ipl ied six by three ).

A nd f inal ly,

 M aria tried

 different

 num bers, which m ul tip l ied

 by

 three equ als

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 85/255

72 R U N E S S O N   A N D M O K

56. So, in B 's  lesson,  the solving strategy varied, w hereas  the param eters o f

the problem were  the

  sam e

  (i.e., 3/7 of 56),  and hence  were invar iant .

T he

  different  w a y s

 in

 w h i c h

 the

 studen ts chose

  to

 solve

 the

 problem

  im -

ply

 that

 they had

 differe nt interpretations

  of the

 operator aspect

 of fractional

num bers. Th e way tha t Lena solved the problem im plie s that she interpreted

3/7 of 56 squares by a rranging the 56 squares in to groups of seven an d then

taking three ou t of seven in each group (s tre tcher /shr inker interp reta t ion ;

B ehr, Harel, Post, & L esh, 1993) as i l lus trate d in Fig. 3.2.

The other two stra tegies both imp ly another interpreta tion, na m ely divid-

ing 56 squares in to seven group s and then tak ing three groups out of the seven

groups (dup licator/partit ion -reducer interpretatio n)  as

 illustrate d

 in F ig. 3.3.

In

 Lesson

 B , the

 var ia t ion

 in

 so lving stra tegies that were presented

 by and

to the

 s tudents a lso in volved

 a

 var ia t ion

 in the

 sem ant ic in te rpre ta tion

 of the

operator aspect  o f f ract ional  num ber s . Thus ,  in th is s i tua t ion , a var ia t ion o f

the semantic interpreta t ion of the concept was introduced.

N ow le t us take a c lose look a t the two lessons. W hat is the m ain   differ-

ence be tween the two teachers ' w ays of dea l ing wi th the same conten t ( a

FI G. 3.2. Le na 's strategy  for  f ind ing  3/7 of 56.

F I G .

  3 .3 . A no the r s tud e n t ' s s t ra t egy fo r  f i n d i n g  3/7 of 56.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 86/255

3 . D I S C E R N M E N T  AND  "WHAT  CAN BE  L E A R N E D ? "  73

strategy

 for

 solving a/b

 of c)?

 Teacher

 A

 introduced

 a

 m e thod

 for

 com put ing

the  fraction  of an integer that basically involved dividing the integer by the

denomina tor

  and

 m ult iplying

  the

  quot ient

 by the

  nominator . This method

w as

  first

  applied

 to one

 problem, then

 to

 another,

 and

  finally

  to a

 th ird, tha t

is, the method w as the same but the problem s

  differed.

Teacher B  offered  the students one problem only but invited them to find

different  ways of solving i t . Three  different students found three  different

w ays of solving the same p roblem. Hence, the problem w as invariant but the

solving strategy varied. This w as the oppo site of w ha t w e observed in the

case of teacher A , w here the me thod w as invariant and the problem varied.

A  com parison of the tw o lessons is i l lustrated in Table 3.1.

From Table 3.1,

 it is

 apparent that there

  is a

 system atic

 difference

  in the

aspects that the two teach ers varied and those they kep t invariant. Som e as-

pect s varied in one lesson, w hereas they w ere invariant in the o ther and vice

versa.

 In the first case (T eacher A ), w e find a variation in the a lgo rithm ic so-

lu t ion,

 w hereas

 in the

 second case (Teacher

 B ),

 there

 is a

 variation

 in the se-

mant ic  interpreta tions o f fraction  numbers  as operator.

Seeing

  the two

  lessons

  in

  terms

  o f

  possibi l i t ies

  for

  d iscernment  (i .e.,

w h a t m i g h t be discerned), the two lessons are also  different; different n u m -

bers are plugg ed into the  formula in one lesson, and different  wa y s o f inter-

preting and solving the problem, in the other lesson.

Focused  Aspects  an d  D i m e n s i o n s  of V a r i at io n

For every aspect of the ob jec t o f learning , there is a corresponding dimen -

sion of

 varia t ion.

 B y

 that

 w e

 mean tha t

 a

 part icular aspect co uld

 b e

  some-

th ing

 different .  For instance, in the exam ples ju s t c i ted , the calcula t ion

 a /b

o fc  could be done in different  ways , and thus the ca lcula t ion corresponds to

a  dimension of variation in the solving strategies. One way of solving the

problem is a

 "value"

  in the dimension of solving s t ra tegies , whereas an-

other way of solving is another

  "value"

  in the same d imens ion. When

TABLE   3 .1

The Space of  V a r i a ti o n

 Constituted

 in Lesson A and

 Lesson

 B,

  R e s p e c t i v e l y

S o l v i n g

  s t ra tegy

Paramete rs o f ope ra t ion

Rep r e s en t a t i o n

S t ude n t s ' unde r s t a nd i ng

Semant i c in t e rpre ta t ion

Teacher  A

Inva r i an t

Var i ed

Var i ed

Invar ian t

Invar ian t

Teacher B

Var i ed

Inva r i an t

Inva r i an t

Var i ed

Var i ed

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 87/255

74

  R U N E S S O N

  A N D M O K

studying   the lessons f rom the po in t  of view  of how the "enacted object  of

learning" is con stituted, w e  find  that the aspect focus ed on in each lesson is

varied

  and we

 describe tha t

 in

 term s

  of the

 opening

  o f a

 d imens ion

 of

 varia-

tion

 o f

 that par t icular aspect .

 F o r

 instance,

 in

 Lesson

 B ,

 w he re

 the

 focus

 w as

on the solving strategy, and different  solving stra tegies w ere dem on stra ted,

a

  dimension

  o f

  tha t aspect ( i .e ., m etho ds

  o f

 so lv ing)

 w as

 opened.

  In

 addi-

t ion ,

 we saw

 that

 the

  teacher presented  different  in te rpre ta t ions

 of the

 con-

cept and different  w ays of un derstan ding , and thu s that these aspects were

also opened

  as

 d im ensions

  o f

 var ia t ion.

 In

 other wo rds,

  the

  solving strate-

gies , the interpreta t ion of the concept , an d the s tudents ' unders tan ding were

dimensions of var ia t ion that were a l l opened in Lesson B. In Lesson A,

other

 aspects

 varied (i.e. ,

  the

 parameters

  o f

 op era t ion

  and

 representa t ion) ,

and therefore other dimensions

  o f

  var ia t ion were opened (see Table 3.1) .

Thu s, the spaces of var ia t ion con st i tuted in the lessons w ere  di f fe rent .

DISCERNING PARTS

  AN D

  W H O L E S

So far in

 this chap ter ,

 w e

 have discuss ed

 how dif ferent

  aspects

 o r

 fea tures

 of

an object of learning are discerned. B ut the ob ject of learning is a lw ay s s i tu-

ated  in a context , a nd it has to be  discerned

  from

  tha t par t icu la r context in

order to re la te i t to other con texts and to other instan ces and by doin g so dis-

cern

  its

  fea tures . Fur therm ore ,

  the

  ob jec t

  o f

  l e a rn ing

  is a

  whole and .

  as a

rule , dis t inct par ts within  i t can be  discerned.

 These

 par ts  can  also be de-

scribed  in  te rms  o f  their features, their re la t ions to  each o ther  and to the

whole ,  and in term s  o f  their parts.

In  chapter  1 , re fe rence w as made  to  Svensson (1 976) who  s ta ted that the

ent it ies , or the phenomenon (we w ould say the ob jec t of lea rn ing ) w e en-

counter m ust be del im ited ( i .e . , d iscerned) by the sub ject and they can be de-

l imited (or

 discerned)

 in

 different

 w ays . Such

 d i f ferences

  accou nt par t ly

 fo r

the reason  wh y we al l see t h i n g s

 differently.

  T he exam ple g iven in chapte r 1

w as the

 deer

 in the

  forest ,

 bu t

 these dif ferences

  are

 present

 in all

 cases wh en

somebody   is  learn ing something . In the exam ple s g iven in  th is chapte r , w e

can a lso see tha t m athem at ica l problems in the c lassroom have to be d is-

cerned f rom the con text . At very least , the s tuden ts m us t be able to dist in-

gu i sh be tween wh en the pa r t i cu la r p rob lem i s d i scussed and w hen

something e lse  is  ta lked about . Fur thermore,  if the problem  is the w h o l e,

there  a re  generally also parts that can be

  identif ied,

  such as separate ar i th-

metic operat ions,

  and the

 m eaning s tha t derive  from

  the

 p rob lem .

Let us look again a t the c i ted exam ple , in w h i ch tw o  dist inct ively

  differ-

ent way s of dealing w ith the operator asp ect of the

  f ract ional

  concept were

described. According

  to the

 m e thods used

  in the

  first  example (Lesson

  A )

the  s tudents cou ld ident i fy  and  learn to use an a lgor i thm for the ca lcu la t ion

a/b  of c, by

 carrying

 ou t the

 division c /b

 and

 thus ob ta in ing lib

  of c, and

 then

mul t ip ly ing

 by  b to obta in

 a/b.

 There w ere  four  subprob lems :

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 88/255

3.  D I S C E R N M E N T

  A N D " W H A T C A N B E

  L E A R N E D ? "

  75

Answer

1.  2/3 of 90 30

2.  1/5 of 40 8

3.

  3/5 of 40 24

4. 3/5 of 60 36

W hen deal ing with this part icular kind

  of

 problem ,

  the

  students

 had to

del imit  this sequence  from  the context of the lesson. By doing so, they be-

came poten t ia lly capable of relating it to sim ilar kinds of problem s in the fu -

ture.

 The

 sequence could then

 be

 considered

 as a

 part

 of the

 lesson,

 but

 also

as

  a whole in which the

  four

  subproblems could be discerned as parts.

Within  the subproblems, the three kinds of components—numerator , de-

nominator ,  and the whole—could be discerned and re la ted to each other

within   the

  same subproblem,

  or to

  corresponding par t s wi th in o ther

subproblems.

A s

 w e

 show

 in the first

 example

 in

 chapter

 4,

 this process

 of

 del im ita tion

also

 appl ies

 to the

 process

  o f

 reading

 a

 text . W hen reading texts ,

 the way in

which the

 reader

 delimits  the whole and the

 parts,

 and

 relates

 them to each

other  is of  vital  importance  for unders tanding.

L E A R N I N G

  A B O U T

  P R O B L E M

  S O L V I N G —

T H E

  POSTMAN'S  ROUTE

Th e secon d ma in data set to be used in this chapter is "the p ostm an 's route,"

which gives

 a

 further  i l lustration

 of the

 thesis presented

 in

 chapter

 2. In the

Ch inese

  lesson that we describe, the teacher carefu l ly  planned the lesson

and

 fo l low ed th e lesson p lan accordingly. W e f irs t present a summ ary of the

lesson  fo l low ed

 by an

 an a lys i s.

S u m m a r y  of the Les s on

This lesson w as a demon stra tion lesson con ducted a t the N at ional C onfe r-

ence

 for

 Open-Ended

 Que stions

 held

 in

 Shanghai

  in

 N ovem ber,  1998 (the

data  w as d iscussed in M ok, 2000, 2002). Dem onstra t ion lessons,  in w h i ch

an

  expert teacher  is  invited to  teach  a  class  in  front  of an  audience , are a

com m on prac t ice in schools in the

 Peo ple 's

 R epu bl ic of China. The c lass-

room  w as a room m uch la rger than a normal c lass room. The front  section o f

the room had the normal c lassroom set ting w ith a teach er ' s po dium , and the

back  section

 w as

 l ined w ith about

 50

 chairs

 fo r the

  audience.

 For the

 dem -

onstrat ion  lesson u nder discussion here , the audience w as made u p of par-

t ic ipants

 of the N at ional Con ference. Despi te the demonstra t ion natu re of

the lesson, i t w as a real 40-min . prim ary Grade 4 lesson, co ndu cted during

the norm al school t imetab le . There were 28 studen ts and they sat in g roups

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 89/255

76  R U N E S S O N  AND MOK

of four. B efore the lesson, the teacher gave the au dienc e a one-page lesson

plan . It w as s ta ted in the plan tha t the obje ct ives of the lesson w ere to de-

velop students ' problem-solving stra tegies  by  so lv ing a  pos tman 's prob-

lem, and to develop stude nts ' creat ivi ty and divergent th ink ing via problem

solv ing.

 In the

 lesson plan ,

 i t w as

 exp l ic i t ly s ta ted tha t

 the

 t e ache r wo u ld

 ap -

proach the problem by asking students to design rou tes for the postm an in a

real-l i fe  context . Fol lowing th is ,  the  t e ache r wou ld he lp s tuden t s to  con-

sider th e problem in a m athematical context , by asking them to look  for reg-

ular i t ies exhibi ted in the routes th at they had com e up w ith , and to thin k

about the possible transform ation  of the

 shapes.

 (The routes becam e shapes

once the direct ion arrows w ere taken aw ay.) To beg in wit h, the teache r ex-

plained

  the

 problem

 by

 holding

  up a

  sample worksheet (see Fig. 3.4). Here

is a transla t ion of the problem sta teme nt that w as show n on the black bo ard

and explained by the  teacher verbal ly:

There are nine dots on the paper. The dot surroun ded by a t r iangle in

the  left  u pper comer represents the post  off ice.  The pos tman needs to

start at the post

 office

  deliver a letter to each of the e igh t p lace s , and re-

turn to the post  off ice .  W hat could be the po s tm an 's route?

T he

  lesson consisted

  o f

  four  phases

  in

 w h i c h

 the

 teach er he lped

 the

 s tu-

dents to tackle the problem according to the lesson p lan . Th e

  four

 p arts are

now   illustrated:

Part

 1:

 Students Designing   Their

  O w n  Routes .

  The

  teacher asked

  th e

students to work in groups an d exper iment w ith as m any routes as possible .

Each student designed his/her  ow n route o n the pieces of paper provided  by

the

 teacher,

 and the

 designs

 o f all

 group m embers w ere

 put in a

 p i l e .

 T he

 s tu-

den ts w ere very

 eff ic ient

  and

 only ta lked w i th the i r ne ighbors occas iona l ly .

W hen they h ad f in ished  al l the papers o n the i r desks , they ra ised the i r han ds

for  more .  T he  act ivi ty lasted about  1 0 m i n .

F IG .

  3.4.

  T he  workshee t .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 90/255

3.

  D I S C E R N M E N T

  A N D  " W H A T  C A N B E  L E A R N E D ? "

77

Part 2:

 Evaluating

  Their  O wn  D es igns  Selectively.  During this phase,  th e

teacher resumed the at tention of the whole class. She posted some of their

designs

 on the

 blackboard

 and

 asked

 the

 class

 to

 j udge

 whether the

  designs

w ere correct . One group produ ced 18 designs and the teache r asked the stu-

dents to discuss with their group members whether this group's designs

were correct (see Fig. 3.5).

After  the discussion, the whole class agreed that a ll of the designs were

correct . Then the teacher pointed to a   faulty  design produced by another

group,

  and

 asked whether

  i t w as

  correct (see Fig. 3.6).

Th e s tudents pointed o ut that the arrows w ere m iss ing from this design,

and that there w as no indicat ion of how the postm an cou ld return to the post

office .

  Fol lowing this,

 the

  teache r asked

  the

 class

 to

 think

 o f

 w a y s

 t o

 deter-

m ine wh ich design w as the best . After a few exchanges of ideas between the

class and the teacher, they agreed that the shortest route w as the best . Then

the

 teacher guided

 the

 class

 to

 compare

  the

 n u mb e r

 of

 straight

 a nd

 d i agona l

F I G .

  3.5.  The 18 rou te s .

FI G. 3 .6 . The w rong route .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 91/255

78

R U N E S S O N A N D M O K

segments

 in the

 designs,

 an d to

 determine

 the

 shortest rou te based

 on

 this

 in-

formation

 (see Fig. 3.7).

  The teacher removed the longer routes, leaving

seven

 on the

  board.

Part

 3:

 Group Interchange

  in

 Order

 to

  Supplement Additional Best Routes

After

  this,  the  teacher asked  the  students  to  supply more  of the  shortest

routes f rom their own piles of designs. They handed in 5 m ore, w hich made

a tota l of 12 on the blackb oa rd. The teacher g uided the class to remove the

repe titions and to disregard the arrows. E ven tua lly only 8 designs rem ained

on

 the

 board (see Fig. 3.8).

Stra ight

segments

Diagona l

segments

F IG . 3.7.

  T h e

  four  pat terns

  and the

 n u m b e r

 o f

  l ine  segments .

F I G . 3.8.  The 8 pat terns

  after

  neg lec t ing the a rrows . N ote tha t (1 ) has the m iss in g

part  fac ing  left

  w he r e a s  ( 2) has the  m i s s ing pa rt

  fac ing

  up .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 92/255

3.

  D I S C E R N M E N T A N D "W H A T C A N B E L E A R N E D ? "

79

Part 4: Converting a   Practical

  Problem

  Into the

  Mathematical Problem

  of

Categorizing   th e Routes

 A ccording

  to

 Th eir

  Shapes.

  Once  th e  arrows were

discarded, the designs of the routes becam e geom etric shapes. In this part of

the

 lesson,

 the

 teache r asked

 the

 students

 to

 catego rize

 the

 shapes acco rding

to  the way

  they

  saw the

 relationship between them. There w ere very l ively

class discussions,  and the students gave a lot of suggestions, such as how to

rotate

  the

  shapes (see Fig. 3.9)

  or

  flip  them over (see Fig. 3.10).

Consequently,  the students  had different  ideas about the number  of cate-

gories, for example,  infinity,  2, 8, and 16.

Toward the end of the lesson,  the teache r invited the class to th ink abo ut a

related problem, which

 had the

 addit iona l constraint suggested earl ier

 b y a

student, that

  is,

 wha t

  the

  postman would

 do if

 there

  w as an

 urgen t letter.

Ana l ys i s  of the

  L e s s o n :

  T h e  P oss ib i li ty  of  D i s c e r n m e n t

  i f f e r e n t  Solutions to the  Same Problem.  At the outset, the teach er asked

th e students

 to

 work

 in

 groups

 and

 experiment w ith

 as

 many po ssible rou tes

 as

they

 cou ld think

 of. It is

 significant  that

 th e

 teacher asked

 for as

 m any routes

as possible instead of the best route. This demand for as many solutions as

possible created

 a

 dimension

 of

 variation

 in the

 possible routes. W hen

 the de-

signs were pu t on the board, the oppo rtunities for discerning alternative rou tes

became m anifold  because each  student  had to consider not  only  th e  routes

proposed

  by

  their group m em bers,

  but

  also those suggested

 by

 mem bers

  of

other groups.

F I G.

  3.9.

  A

 ro ta t ion .

F IG .  3.10.  Ref l ec t ions .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 93/255

80  R U N E S S O N AND MOK

Possible

  Versus  Impossible  Routes .

  In order  to help students discern  the

critical features of a possible rou te, the teacher del iberately focused the stu-

dents' attention o n a faulty design

 (i.e.,

  an

 im possible route),

  and

 asked

  the

class wh ether this design

 w as

 correct (see F ig. 3.6).

 In the

 di scussion about

w hether the route was correct or not , one s tudent pointed out that the arrow s

were missing, and another student pointed out that the route did not lead

back  to the post

  off ice.

In other w ords,

 by

 juxta po sing poss ib le rou tes wi th

 an

 im possible route,

the

 teacher

 w as

 able

 to

 help stu den ts discern

  tw o

 cri t ical features

 of a

 possi-

ble route. One is tha t to con st i tu te a route, there has to be an indic at ion of the

direction   of the route,  and using an arrow  to indicate direction  is  im portant.

T he

  other

  is

  tha t

  the

  rou te needs

  to

  reach

  a

  pa r t i cu l a r des t ina t i on .

 In

 th is

case, the pos tm an needed to be able to go back to the post office

  af ter

  del iv-

ering   the letters.  Discerning  these  tw o  critical features helped  the class  to

cons ider

 t he

 designs th at they

 had

 come

 up

 w i th

 and

 e l imina te

 the

 imp oss i-

b le

 rou tes .

Possible

  Routes  Versus

  Bes t

 Rou tes.

  Af ter

  this ,  the  teacher raised  th e

quest ion

  of how to

 dec ide wh ich design

  w as

 best .

  T he

  fol lowing

  is an ex-

cerpt  from

  the

 di scuss ion .

3.3. [Math Lesson/Primary  Grade

 4]

T: How

 should

 w e

 decide wh ich design

 can be

 called

 th e

 best design?

S3: Use the

  shortest route?

T:  "Use the shortest route," goo d. A ny others?

S 4:

  Do n ' t repea t

  the

 rou te .

T :

  Good,

 "Don't

 repeat  the route."  A ny  others?

S5: If

 there

  is an

  urgen t le t ter that needs

  to be

 del ivered

 first,

 w h a t

should we do?

T:  Goo d, "If there is an urg ent let ter." V ery good. An y others?

[N o

 m ore su ggest ions . ]

T :

  Let ' s

 first put S5 s

 problem aside

 an d

 assum e tha t there

 are no ur-

gent letters.

 A ll

 let ters

 are

 eq ual ly im por tan t. Then , w hich

 is the

best design?

S6: The shortes t route.

B y

 asking the s tudents to th ink about w ha t const i tu ted a

 "best

 route," the

teacher w as  trying to ge t the students  to revis i t the routes that they had de-

signed

  f rom a

 different perspective: that

  is,

 w h a t

  is

  possible versus what

  is

best. The s tuden ts 'judg m ent of the best route w as at f irs t s imp ly an intui t ive

judgm ent w i th re ference to the postman's prob lem . The c ri teria for the best

route were

 no t

 form al ized. Subsequent ly ,

 the

 teacher g uided

 the

 students

 to

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 94/255

3.  D I S C E R N M E N T

  A N D

  " W H A T

  C A N B E

  L E A R N E D ? "

  8J_

coun t the

 number

 of

  straight line segments (horizontal, vertical,

 and

 diago-

na l ;

 see Fig. 3.7). A t this point, the teacher w as in fact guiding the students to

scrutinize

  the

 routes  from

  a

 m athem at ical perspective.

U p to this poin t in the lesson, the teacher had taken an open problem , p re-

sented at a level very close to the students' everyday experience (design ing as

many paths as possible from the postman's perspective), and deliberately

guided

 the students to twice revisit the designs that they prop osed: first to pick

the correct routes a nd then to pick the best rou te. In this way, the students not

only  had the

 chance

 to see

 that

 the

 problem

  w as

 open,

 b ut

 also

 to see

 that there

w ere m any possible solutions. In other words, the problem w as the invariant

in

 the

 lesson, w hereas

 a

 dimension

 of

 variation

 w as

 created

 in the

 m ethods

 of

solving  th e problem . Because  of the  variation created, students were

  given

oppo rtunit ies to see various aspects of the problem— the m any po ssible solu-

t ions  to the  same problem;  the  difference  between possible  and  impossible

solutions; the difference between possible and best solutions; and that th e cri-

teria used

  m ay

  vary between

  an

  intuitive choice

  and a

  mathemat ica l ly

grounded ch oice.

Categorization

  of

  Shapes:

  Rotat ions and  Reflections.  In the

  fourth  par t

of

 the lesson, the tea che r asked the class to discern the regu lari t ies exh ibited

in  the eight shapes that rem ained on the board, an d to catego rize  the shapes

(see F ig. 3.8). This task gene rated

 a

 very l ively wh ole-class discuss ion.

 T he

fo l lowing

  is an

 excerp t  from

  the

  discuss ion:

3.4 [M ath Le sson/Prim ary Grade 4]

T:  N o w ,

  we are not

  going

  to

  consider di rect ion anymore. Look

  at

these shapes . I w ould l ike you to use different m e thods to ca tego-

r ize

  them. [Referr ing

 to

 Fig. 3.8.]

S 1 1 :

  There are eight

 shapes.

 A ccording  to the direction  in w hich  the

miss ing  part

  is

  fac ing,  there

  are  four

  categories. There

  are two

shapes  for  each category. Eight altogether.

S I 2 :  Move

 the

  second shape [referring

  to

 shape

  6 in

 Fig. 3.8]

 on the

second

 row

 aro und, then

 i t

 becomes

  th e

 third shape [shape

  7] on

th e  second row. [The teacher then moved the shape acc ording to

th e

  student 's instruction.]

T: Very

 good.

 S he

 found

  tha t

 af ter

 ro tat ing, these

 tw o

 shapes

  [6 and

7],

 become the same. Any m ore sugges t ions?

S12 :  M o v e

 the

 third shape

  [7] on the

 second row,

 it

 then becom es

  the

third  shape

  [3] on the first

  row.

T :

  Let m e f irs t  label these patterns. One, two,  .. .  eight [Students

said

 the num bers aloud and the teacher wrote the num bers under

the  patterns.]

SI 3 :  [ inaudible]

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 95/255

82

  R U N E S S O N

  AND MOK

T: S13

 said that rotating

  the

 sixth shape

  [6]

 w i l l resul t

 in the

 eighth

shape [8]. What next?

SI 4:

  [ inaudible]

T: S1 4 said that rotat in g the eighth shape [8] w il l aga in resu lt in the

seventh shape [7]. [The teacher wrote "6-8-7" on the board.]

S15: Turn  the  f i f t h  shape [5], and turn again . It

 wi l l

 resul t in the eighth

shape [8]. Turn

  the

  third shape

  [3]

 upward.

  No, flipping it

 over

wil l resul t

 in the fifth

 shape [5]. [Flipp ing

 the

 shape w i l l produce

an image by ref lec t ion; see Fig . 3.10.]

T: W e started by sugg esting rotat ion . No w S1 5 sugg ested flipping

over

  the

  shapes.

  A re

 there

  any

  more suggestions? [The teacher

recorded the fl ipping in another row on the boa rd; see Fig. 3.1 1 .]

After

  labeling the  shapes  1 to 8, students became very active in pu tt ing

forward

  thei r ideas for rotat ing them . The teacher put down a record of the

rotat ions

  (6, 8, 7, and

  later

  5)

  that

  th e

  students suggested (see Fig. 3.11).

How ever, rotation wa s not the only w ay to move the shapes a bou t . Som e s tu-

dents

  found that they could  also

 obtain

  matching images

  by flipping

  over

the shapes vert ical ly or ho rizontal ly ( i .e . , by r ef lec t ion; see Fig . 3.10) . The

students  suggested altogether  1 8 rotations  and  seven reflections.

Let us exam ine w hat happ ened in th is excerpt . The f i rs t student (SI 1)

suggested   that  the  eight different shapes could be put  in to four

 categories

according

  to the

 direct ion

  in

 w h i c h

 the

  "miss ing

 part" w as  fac ing,

  t ha t

 is ,

whether  it  faced

  left

  or  right ,  up or  down. This s tudent w as  looking  at the

shapes  as  static

  f igures.

  Af ter  th is , ano ther s tudent ( S I2 ) sugges ted tha t

moving

 the

 second

 on e on the

 second

 row

 (i .e. ,

 6 in

 F ig. 3.8) wo uld resul t

 in

the third one in the second row (i .e. , 7). This app roach sug ges ted seeing the

shapes as dynam ic by m oving them and looking for a m atch ing image by ro-

tat ion.

 In

 other words ,

 in

 th is exam ple ,

 the

 seve nth shape could

 be

 seen

 as an

image of the sixth shape af ter  a c lockwise ro ta t ion . In a s im i lar way , ano ther

FI G.

 3 .1 1 .

  T he

  t e ache r ' s symbo l ic

 r epresen ta tions .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 96/255

3.  D I S C E R N M E N T AND  " W H A T  CAN BE  L E A R N E D ? "  83

student (S14)

 saw the

 eighth

 and

 seventh shapes (i.e.,

 8 and 7) as

 images

 of

the sixth shape   after  consecutive rotat ions. By doing this , the shapes were

no

  longer

  different,

  they were s imply

  different

  orientat ions

  of the

  same

shape resul t ing f rom rotations and ref lect ions . In other words , the shapes

con sti tuted a dim ension of variat ion in the orientat ion of an invariant shap e.

The discernm ent of the equivalence between the shape an d i ts m atching im -

age was m ade poss ib le w hen the teacher rotated and f l ipped over the shapes

on

 the

 board fol low ing

 the

  students ' suggestion (see Fig. 3.9).

It

  is  clear that  th e students'  focus ,  in  this p art  of the lesson, was on the

t ransformation

  ( rotat ion and ref lect ion) of the shapes . They im agined the

shape turning in thei r mind s and m ade suggest ions to the teacher . W ith  trial

and error, it was no t

  difficult

  for some students to see that there were two

possible m eans of t ransform at ion, that i s , ro tat ion and ref lect ion , and  that

the rotat ional im ages could

 be

 categorized into

 tw o

 types . This categoriza-

tion

 becam e ex pl icit w hen the teacher asked a new quest ion, near the end of

the

  lesson, tha t requ ired the s tuden ts to think abou t their obse rvations at a

higher level o f ab straction.  The fol low ing is an excerpt from th e discussion:

3.5

  [Math lesson/Primary Grade 4]

T :

  Let's

 first

 look at 1 ,2 ,3 ,4 , these

 four

 shapes . W hat do w e not ice?

[Referring   to Fig. 3.8.]

S33:  [If we] keep  rotating [the shape], eventually it wi l l becom e the

first  again .

T:

  How abou t 5, 6, 7, 8? Does the sam e ru le app ly?

Ss:

  [ inaudible]

T :  Now , can we tel l how m any categories there are al l together?

S33:

  E igh t .

S34:  Infinitely  many .

T:  "Infinitely

 many"

 refers to the designs. Bu t we said at the begin-

ning there are eight best des igns . I f w e go back to the problem of

the postma n del ivering let ters , how m any categories of bes t de-

signs  are  there?

S35:

  Infinitely

  many .

S36: Two.

S37:  Eigh t .

S38: 16.

T:  W h y ?

S36: B ecau se there

 are

 eight shapes.

 Af ter

 changing

 the

 directions

 (of

the  routes), there wil l be 16.

T:  Good. Then,  how did S36's  suggestion  of  " two" come up?

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 97/255

84

  R U N E S S O N

  AND MOK

S38: Because  all these shapes  can be obtained  by rotating tw o

  differ-

ent

  shapes.

T :  Good.

 These

 are the different  w a y s  o f categor izat ion. Here ,  w e

conclude

  f rom the

  simple problem

  of the

 postman.

 There

  were

1 6 best routes.  In the earlier part  of the  lesson,  S5 suggested  th e

problem of an urgen t letter . Teacher gave yo u this to think about

as a postlesson problem. If we

 need

 to

 send

 an

 urgent

 letter to the

third

 poin t , how m any types  o f best design  are  there? Don't for-

get to

 tell

 the

 teacher you r

 findings.

 This

  is the end of the

 lesson.

Bye, c lass [Class dismissed] .

In the above excerpt, the teache r requested the studen ts to

 ref lect

  o n the

rela t ionship between shapes,

  and

 asked

  for the

 n u m b e r

 o f

 categor ies

 o f the

shapes. When   the s tudents were u nable to come  up wi th the correct answ er,

the

  teacher brought

  the

 c lass back

  to the

  everyday context

 of the

 pos tman .

A s we can see, this con textual

 shif t

  enabled som e s tuden ts to come up w ith

the correct an sw er. It is hig hly likely that as soon as these sh apes w ere seen

as

  the postman's routes , the cr i t ica l features of possible routes and best

routes th at were discussed ear l ier in the lesson, that is, the

  indica t ion

 o f di-

rect ion  and  shortest distance respectively, came to the

  fore

 of the  s tudents '

awareness. The  fact  that there were eight shortest routes and  tha t  the post-

m an cou ld e i ther go c lockw ise or cou nterc lock w ise , as po inted out by S38,

led to the answ er of 16 routes . Here w e see tha t the s tude nts ' answ ers w ere

once  again focused  on the

 postman's

 routes,  bu t that this t im e the  answers

were enr iched wi th

 a n

 u nde r s t and ing

 of the

 m a t h em a t ic a l m e a n i n g s .

Contextual

  Variation.  When  w e  look back at the lesson  as a w h o l e , it is

no t  di ff icul t  to see tha t th e problem w ent through a shi f t o f context from real

life

  to ma the m atica l , and vice versa (see Fig. 3 .12) . Th e problem w as f i rs t

introduced   in a real-l i fe  context tha t w as  eas i ly access ib le  to the s tudents .

T he

  different  solut ion s were exam ined

  in

 te rm s

  of

 fea s ibi l i ty  ( w h e t h e r

 the

routes were correct

  o r

 not )

  and the

  o p t im u m r e q u ir e m e n t ( w h i c h

 one was

the best) . In both cases, the use of the postman's route as a con text for under-

standing

 possible rou tes a nd best rou tes was a

 power fu l

  one . I t was easy f o r

students

  to

  re la te

 to the

  possible routes

  and the

  best routes

  from  the

  post-

m an's perspect ive: that is , he m us t be ab le to re turn to the s tar t ing po int , the

post

 office,

  and he

 should

 be

 able

 to finish his

 m ail delivery

 a s

 eff iciently

  as

F I G .  3 .1 2 . C o n t e x t u a l

 va r i a t ion .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 98/255

3 . D I S C E R N M E N T  A N D

  " W H A T

  C A N B E  L E A R N E D ? "  8 )

possible, that

 is, no

 part

 of the

 rou te should

 be

 repeated unne cessari ly.

 T he

teacher  shifted  into

 a

 mathem at ica l con tex t

 b y

  invit ing

 th e

 students

 to

 cate-

gorize the routes . The s tudents then examined the

  different

  solutions and

thei r

 relat ion s, using rotation and reflect ion. W hat is interest ing is the way

in  w h i c h

  the

  teacher exploited

  the

  contextual  shift  back

  to the

  pos tman ' s

rou te when

 the

 students

 h ad diff icult ies

  coming

 up

 w i th

 the

 correct answ ers

fo r

 c atego rizing the routes. Even m ore im portant , the teach er referred to the

postm an's problem again at the end of the lesson, and co ncluded the discus-

sion by

  aff i rming  that there were

  1 6

 best ro utes.

 This

 conc lusion

  w as

 buil t

on the comparison between the m any po ss ibil it ies , different  rotat ions and

ref lect ions , an d the

 directions

 of the

 routes .

 A t the end o f the

 lesson,

 after the

various stages of explorat ion, the way in which the students discerned the

postman's problem

  w as

 u ndoubtedly  different from  thei r unders tanding

 o f

this

 problem at the outse t of the lesson. T he lesson provided a rich ex peri-

ence  o f  problem solving reinforced  by the  teacher wi th

  careful

  p l ann ing .

However ,

 the

 s tudents also played

 a

 cru cial role

 in the

 con st rual

 o f the

 ob jec t

of

 learning. For exam ple, when the class wa s discuss ing the best route, one

s tudent

 imagined  the si tuation of an urgent letter. This show ed tha t the stu-

den t 's reflec t ions on the solution

 were

 very m uch in areal- l i fe context a t th is

stage.

 T he

 teacher  skillfully  kept

 the

 s tudents '  focus  according

 to her

 or igi-

nal

 p lan ,

 and put

 this nove l idea aside

 fo r

 that par t icular m om ent . However ,

after

  examining

 t he

  solutions

 of the

 problem

  f rom a

 m athem at ica l perspec-

t ive, the idea of "an urgent letter" became a m ean in g fu l  al ternat ive problem .

T he

 problem

  was the

  same type

  of

 problem

  as the

 original one,

 bu t

 w i th

 a

var iat ion,

 and as such, it w as an opp ortunity  for the students to put w h a t they

h ad

 ju s t experienced into pract ice.

This example provides

  a  wonderfu l

  illustration

  of the way in

  w h i ch

 a

t eacher

 can

 provide studen ts w ith

 a

 rich un ders tanding

 of the

 ma n y

 facets o f

problem solving even with the use of one single problem. The lesson also

suppor ted  th e  teacher 's lesson objective  "to  enhance s tudents ' p rob-

lem-solving abil i ty

 and

  strategies"

  in an efficient  and

  s t im ulat ing wa y.

WHAT

  C O UL D  BE L EARNE D?

W e

  s tar ted this chapter by saying that unders tanding something as some-

th ing

 im pl ies discerning  features ,

  o r

 aspects,

  o f

 that which

  is

  experienced.

Certain learning takes  the s im ul taneous di scernm ent  o f certain c ri t ical a s-

pec ts of the o bjec t of learning . That is , the cri t ical asp ects are held in the fo-

cal aw areness a t the sam e t im e. How ever, an aspect can on ly be discern ed if

it

  is expe rienced as a dime nsion of variation. From this assum ption, we ar-

gue that those dim ensions of variat ion that are presen t in the learning en vi-

ronm ent are cri tical for learning w ha t is possible to learn . In other words, i f

an

 aspect is presented as a dim ension of variat ion to the learners, i t m ake s i t

poss ib le

 fo r the

 learners

  to

 discern tha t pa rt icular aspe ct .

 And i f

 several

 di-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 99/255

86

  R U N E S S O N

  A N D M O K

mens ions

  o f

 variat ion

 are

  open s imul taneous ly ,

 it

 ma k e s

  it

 poss ib le

 for the

learners to discern al l of these aspects s im ul tan eo usly . So, wh at i s varying,

w h a t

  is

  invariant,

  and

  w h a t

  is

 varying

  at the

  same t ime

  are

  impor tan t

  fo r

w h a t

  is

 po ssible

  to le a m .

W e

 have an alyzed three m athem at ics lessons f rom the point of view of

the pattern of variat ion that was consti tuted during the lesson, and have

identif ied  the pattern of variant and invariant aspec ts in the three lessons re-

spectively.

 From this w e can draw

  some

 co nclusio ns about the poss ib i l i t ies

to leam   that were provided for the learners.

In  the f irst tw o

 exam ple lessons ,

 in

 w h i c h

 th e

 object

 o f

 learning

 w as f rac-

t ional

 n u m b e rs

 as

 operator,

 w e can see

 tha t there

 was a dif ference  in

 w h a t

 it

w as possible to learn . Teacher A provided the learners w ith the possibili ty to

leam

  to

 solve

 different

 p rob lem s w i th

 the

 sam e strategy, w hereas T eacher

 B

provided the learners w ith the possibil i ty to leam to solve the same p roblem

wi th different  strategies. In Teacher A 's lesson  the chang ing of the parame-

ters

  in the

  operat ion opened

  a

  variat ion

 of  different

  examples , whereas

  in

Teacher B 's lesson, the param eters were invarian t and the s t rategy varied

(F ig . 3 .1 ) . In addi t ion , in Teacher B 's  lesson, the learners had the po ss ib i l i ty

to

  discern

  a

  semant ic aspect

  of fractional

  numbers ; th i s

  did not

  occur

  in

Teacher

  A 's

  lesson.

  On the

 o ther hand,

  the

  s tudents

  in

 Teacher

  A 's

  lesson

were presented w i th

 a

 variat ion

 of how a f ract ional

 num ber cou ld

 be

  repre-

sented,

  and

  thus

 the

 possibil i ty

  to

 discern this pa r t icula r aspect .

It

 is

 obvious that these

 tw o

 lessons were very s im i lar

 on one

 level ,

 for in-

stance in the  t each ing me thods  and the  a r r angem en t  of the  l earn ing s i tua-

t ion.

 How ever,

 o n

 an other rather m ore su bt le level (but

 one

 t ha t

 is

 im por tan t

from

  the

 po in t

 of

 view

 of the

 s t uden t s ' po t en t i a l

 fo r

 l earn ing) , they were very

different. W hen com par ing tw o lessons as we ha ve done here, i t seems na tu-

ra l

 to ask, "W hich one is the best?" However, there can be no gen eral answ er

to

 tha t ques t ion .

 T he

 answer

 to any

 assessm ent

 o f

 th i s k ind

 wi l l

 a lw ays need

to  take into con siderat ion th e ca pab i l i t ies the t eache r wan ted th e s tudents to

develop,

  and

  t hus

 the

  in ten ded object

  o f

  learning .

The lesson on the postman's routes showed how the teacher very

though t fu l ly ,  and j o in t l y w i th her s tuden ts , con st i tu ted a pa t tern of variation

that w as

 very

 rich in

  several aspects.

 T he

 learners ,

 in the

 s ame

 way a s

 those

in  Teacher  B 's  lesson, were

  afforded

  the  opportunity  to discern that there

can

  be

  many poss ible solut ions ( routes)

  to the

  s ame p rob lem .

  A s

  we l l

 as

this ,

  they were provided

  the

  opportunity

  to

  evalua te ( scru t in ize)

 th e

 so lu-

t ions

 in different

  w a y s ,

 in an

 everyday contex t

 and a

 m a them at i ca l con t ex t ,

and

 in

 term s

 of

 w he ther

 the

 solut ion s were feas ib le (poss ib le

 o r

 impo ss ib le)

and

  optimal (best) .  They  were also required to discern the character of a

shape

 by the

 variat ion

 in the

 o r ientation

 of the

 sam e shape

 af ter

  rotat ion

 and

ref lect ion. Tha t is ,

 w i thin

 th e

 m athem at ica l con tex t ,

 th e

 same shape

 c an

 pro-

duce many  different  possible routes by rotat ion  o r ref lec t ion.  Rotat ion and

reflection   were tw o

 w a y s

 b y

 w h i c h

 the

 s tudents cou ld

 see

 os tens ib ly differ-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 100/255

3.

  D I S C E R N M E N T

 A N D

  " W H A T

  C A N B E

  L E A R N E D ? "

  87

ent shapes

 a s the

 same. Fu rther,

  th e

 students w ere able

 to see the

 sam e prob-

lem a nd its solutions differently,  before and af ter  scrutinizing the solut ions

in

  a m athem at ical context.

A l though our

 focus

 has been o n the impo rtance of the ob ject of learning,

w e do not wish to imply that c lassroom arrangemen ts are unim portant . A s

w e  have seen  in two o f the  examples (Teacher  B 's  lesson  and the  Chinese

demonstrat ion lesson)

  the

  students contributed very much

  to the

 co nst i tu-

t ion  and the widenin g of the space of variat ion. It is reasonable to assum e

that

 this variation

 w as a

 resul t

 of the

  group discussions that took place

 b e-

fore the w hole-class sessions. In both of these lessons, the studen ts w ere al-

lowed, and even enc oura ged, to discuss the problem w ith their peers. Th us a

certain

 arrangement (group work)

 facilitated

 th e

 opening

 of

 variation. How -

ever, w ha t w e w ant to stress is that i t is not a certain arrangem ent in i tself that

m ake s a certain learning possible, but rather that the possibil i ty to learn is

provided

 by

 w h a t

  it is

 possible

  to

  discern.

In th is chapter w e have fo cu sed on the poss ib i l i t ies to learn, rather than

w hat the s tudents actua l ly learned. W e have also descr ibed the en acted ob-

j e c t

  o f

  learning

  as a

 po ten t ia l

  fo r

  s tudent learning seen

  from  the

 po in t

  o f

view

 o f the researcher. How ever, we w ould no t a rgue tha t what w as poss i -

ble

  to

 experience

  w as

  indeed wh at

 th e

  s tudents actual ly experienced.

  It is

l ikely tha t

 th e

 s tuden ts learned d i f ferent  t h ings . W ha t

 w e do

 argue

 is

 tha t

is

  l ikely that the s tudents learned cer tain th ings in one lesson, and other

th ings  in  another .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 101/255

  his page intentionally left blank

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 102/255

4

Simultaneity  and the  Enacted

Object  of  Learning

P a k e y

  P . M .  C h i k

M u n   L i n g  L o

In the

 three previous chapters,

 the

 im portant role

 of

 variation

 in

 opening

 up

possibil i t ies  for a

 particular learn ing

 (or the

 deve lopmen t

 o f

 certain cap abil-

i t ies) to

 take place

 w as

  described

 and

  exem pl i f ied .

 W e

 also i l lustrated tha t

the way in

  which

  w e

  experience

  or

  understand som ething depends

  on

which  fea tures of it we are aware of and can discern s im ultaneously. Thu s,

w h e n   different

  aspects

 of the

 same th ing

 are

 discerned

 and

 focused

 on ,

 d i f -

ferent

  ways of understanding wil l resul t . More powerful ways of under-

standing  a m o u n t

  to a

  s imul taneous awareness

  of

  those fea tures tha t

  are

crit ical  to

 ac hieving certa in a ims.

In

 this chapter ,

 we

 pr im ar ily focu s

 on

 describing

 one of the two

  fo rm s

of  s im ul t ane i ty d i scussed

  in

  chapte r

  1 :

 synchron ic s imul tane i t y ,  w h i c h

r e fe r s

 to

 expe r i enc ing d i sce rned fea tu re s

 at the

 sam e time , w he re

 th e

 dis-

cerned features

 may be

 he ld

 in two

 differe nt types

 of

 re la tionship. W hen

th e

 d isce rned fea tu re s a re seen a s aspec ts o f s o m e t h in g , w e r e f e r to re l a -

t i onsh ips

 o f

 th i s k ind

 a s aspect-aspect re lat ionships.  F o r

 e x a m p l e , w h e n

we see a person

 w ri ting

 an

 essay

 on a

 com pute r,

 we may

 exp erience

  it as

som eone eng aged in an ac t of essay w ri t ing as we l l a s an ac t of word pro-

ces s ing .

 T h e other re la t ionsh ip invo lves see ing t h e discerne d parts of the

wh o l e ,

 and the

  whole tha t

 is

  del imited from

 a

 context ,

 at the

  same t ime .

For exam ple , one may d isce rn the eyes , m uzz le , and l im bs a s   d i f fe ren t

p a r t s

  of the

  body

  o f a

  deer , which

  is in

  turn de l imi ted  f rom

  th e

  w o o d s

(Mar ton & B ooth, 1997). The kind of  l inkag es

  established

 between  an

ob jec t  or a phen om enon and it s pa r t s be ing d iscerned a re re fe r red to as

part-whole  re lat ionships.

89

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 103/255

90  C H I K  A N D L O

Things m ight or migh t not be experienced s im ul tane ou sly , and in teach -

ing, various things

 m ay o r m ay not

 come

 to the

 fore s imu l taneo usly .

 In

 this

chapter,

  w e

  compare lessons that

 differ  in

 this respect; th at

 is, the

 in tended

object

  o f

 learning

 is the

 same,

 but the

  enacted object

  differs,

  as

 does

 w h a t

the students leam  (i.e., the

  l ived object  o f  learning) .

Let us,  however, start with  a study  in which  th e  learners were facing the

sam e situation, but the w ay they discerned and experienced the relation s be-

tween

 parts, an d

 wholes

 differe d. Saljo

 (1982)

 carried  out a study  to  investi-

g a t e t h e d i f f e r e n t w a y s i n w h i c h u n i v e r s i t y s t u d e n t s i n S w e d e n

comprehended   the  same text . He

  found

  tha t a l though these s tudents were

reading

  the

 same text (w hich was,

 by the

 w ay, about

 different

  perspectives

 of

learning),

 the

  students

  saw

 different

  meanings

  in it. Tw o

 dis t inct w ays

 of un-

derstanding

 the

 text were identif ied.

 In the first

 way ,

 th e

 students

 saw the

 text

as

 having

 a

 sequential s tructure, with

 different

  perspectives

 o f

 learning being

described,

  but

 bearing

  no

  relationship

  to

 each other.

 In the

  second way,

 the

students discerned

 a mam

 t hem e

 (the

 f o r m s  of  learning)  i l lustrated

 b y a

 n u m -

ber of

  subthemes  ( d i f f e r e n t

  perspect ives  o f  learning) .

  They

  saw the

  text

 as

having a

 hierarchical structure w ith clear

 part-part

 relat ionships (between

 the

subthemes)  and part-whole  relationships (between  th e  subthemes  and the

m ain theme).

 In

 both w ays

 o f

 com prehending

 the

 text ,

 all the

 parts contained

in  the  text were distinguished,  but these

 parts

 w ere seen  as occupying  differ-

ent structural positions. S aljo (1982) also  found  that th e students w ho under-

stood

 th e  text  in the hierarchical  way had a

 more

 organized  and m ean ing fu l

understanding, and were better able to grasp the mam idea of the text than the

students

  w ho

  understood

  the

 text

 in the

  sequent ial way.

It is not

 diff icul t ,  therefore,

 to

 i ma g i n e

 w hy i t is

 tha t

 in any one

 par t i cu lar

classroom si tuat ion, d i f ferent  s tudents w i l l have

 d i f ferent

  levels o f under-

s tanding even thou gh they  all are given the sam e presen ta t ion by the teacher .

W hat s tudents

 gam

 from

  a

 lesson,

 and how

 w el l they unders tand tha t l esson ,

depends

  on the w ay

 they com prehend

 the

 s t ructure

 of the

 presen ta t ion ; th i s

in turn dep ends on w ha t t hey  focus on and w hat recedes to the background

(or is

 taken

  fo r

 granted) w hen they

 try to

 unders tand

 the

  lesson. Some s tu-

dents w i l l

 see no

 relat ionsh ip between

 the

 different par ts

 of the

 p resen ta tion ,

whereas others w i ll be able to comprehend  the presentation  in m ore power-

fu l

  ways (especial ly

  if

 they

 can

  s im u l taneous ly

 see

 c lear re la tionsh ips

 be -

tween  the  parts  and the way in  which these  parts  are  related  to the

presentat ion

  as a

 w h o l e ).

 It

 then fo l low s tha t s tudents wi l l leam m ore

  effec-

tively if the teacher is able to consciously structure the presen tat ion in such a

w ay as to

 bring

  out

 clear ly

 the

 cr i t ical features

 of the

 ob jec t

 o f

  learn ing ,

 as

w ell as

 thei r relationships

 to the

 object

 o f

 learning

 and to

 each o ther.

 In

 other

words ,  the way the lesson  is structured w il l have a n impor tan t  inf luence on

s tudent learning .

W e

 now

  provide three sets

  o f

  c lass room data—compri s ing

  six

 pa i rs

 of

lessons—to i l lus t rate th e w a y s in w hich teachers can s t ructure the i r lessons

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 104/255

4.

  S I M U L T A N E I T Y

  A N D T H E

  E N A C T E D O B J E C T

  O F  L E A R N I N G  9 ±

to

 enable their students

 to

 discern

 the

 aspect-aspect

 ( or part-part) and part-

w hole relat ionships  of various fe atures o f the object o f learning. I n par t icu-

lar,

  we look at w hat i t is possible to learn regarding

  specific

  obje cts of learn-

ing

  (i.e.,

  enacted objects  of  learning)  and  w h a t  is  actual ly learned  by

studen ts ( i .e. , l ived objects o f learning ).

The

  first  pair

  of

  mathemat ics lessons

  is

 drawn

  from

  Runesson 's (1999)

study

  and is

 used

  to

  i l lustrate

  the

 aspect-aspect  relat ionship.

  The

  second

and the

 third pairs

 o f

 lessons

 are

 taken  from

 a

 2-year research p roject un der-

taken

  by a research team in Hong Kong,

1

  and involve a pair of primary

Grade

  2

  Chinese language lessons

  and a

 pai r

  of

  pr imary Grade

  1

 E ngl i sh

l anguage  lessons , respect ively ( for detai led informat ion and discuss ions

abo ut other aspects

 o f

 these

 tw o

 pairs

 o f

 lessons,

  see

 C hik, 2002;

 L o &

 Ch ik ,

2000, and M ok et al ., 1 999). These three p airs of lessons  wil l be m ain ly used

to

  i l lus t rate part-whole  and

 aspect-aspect

  relat ionships . Each pai r  o f  les-

sons was o n the same topic, but taug ht by two  different  t eachers .

The

 analysis

 is

 based

 on the

 audio

 o r

 video recordings

 of the

 lessons

 and

the  subsequent transcript ions. In order to explore the impact of the struc-

tural

 difference s

  between the lessons on s tudent learning outcom es, the les-

son

  analyses

 of the

  second

  and

  thi rd pai rs were also com pared against

 the

student data , w hich include the w orksheets c om pleted by the s tudents im -

media te ly

  after

  each lesson.

A  P A I R   O F

  MATHEMATICS LESSONS

The two m athem at ics l essons  focused  on teach ing two

 different

  aspects of

fractional  numbers :  the part-whole  aspect  and the  division-quotient  as-

pec t , where

  the

  former aspect involves adding

 up

 parts

  to a

 w hole (e .g . ,

 1 2

qua rters add up to 3) and the lat ter involves dividin g up a who le in to par ts

(e.g. , 1 2 divided by 4 is equal to 3). The  different  w ays in w hich these two

aspects and thei r relat ionsh ip are s im ul taneo usly brough t to the s tuden ts

1

focal

  awareness

 are

  com pared .

Des cr i p t i o n

  of  L e s s o n  A

This lesson con sists of three main e pisodes.  In Episode  1, the teacher drew

the

  students ' at tention

 to the

 part-whole

 aspect

 of

  fractional

  numbers .

 S he

used an ico nic represe ntat ion to show six pieces of 1 /3 of a pizza and invited

the s tudents  to  represent th is wi th mathemat ical symbols

2

  ( i .e. , symbolic

represen tat ion) . The fol low ing suggest ion was arr ived at by the s tudents :

—   or 2

3

(6 pieces

 of

 pizza, each

  1/3 of a

 whole, equal

 2

 whole p izzas)

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 105/255

92  C H 1 K A N D L O

In Episode

  2, the

 teacher focused

 on the

 division-quo tient aspect

 of frac-

tional

 numbers

 (6 divided by 3), and asked the  students, "How  can this be

represented  by  (mathem at ica l ) sym bols?" The same represen tat ion w as ar-

rived

  a t

 again

 by the

  students:

—   = 2 (6

  divided

 by 3

 equa l s

 2)

T he teacher  further illustrated this with  an  example  of  dividing 6 by 3

(e.g.,  6 p ieces  of pizza  are divided am ong  3 persons) :

In

 Ep isode 3, the teacher broug ht the two aspects together by ex pla inin g

to  the

  class:

T: ... the   resu l t  is the  same,  but it is  said

  differently.

  Somet imes I

will say.  "20 divided by 4" and som etimes  I w ill say,  "20 quar-

ters"

  . . . the resul t i s 5 anyway. But you should know that a l -

thoug h these are actual ly qu i te different  [concepts] , the r e su l t is

th e  same.

Des cr i p t i o n o f L es s o n B

This

 lesson can also be divided into three m ain episodes. In Episod e 1, the

teacher drew a picture of 12 apples on the b lackboard. H e sa id that 3 per-

sons were to share them equ al ly ( the divis ion-quot ient aspect of  f ract ional

num bers w as fo cuse d on) , and asked the class to th ink in s i lence how m an y

apples  each person  would get .

Meanwhile ,

  he

  drew

  a new

  picture—a bowl with

  a lot of

  triangles

  in it

(where each triangle was supposed to be 1/3 of an app le)— and asked the class:

...

  If you

 have

 to

 pick

 up as

 man y p ieces

 o f

 apple

 from  the

 bowl

 as you

have there (m eaning the same as the total of the problem ju s t g iven) ,

how

 many p ieces

  of

 apple

 do you

 have

 to

 p ick

 up?

Again ,

  the

  students were instructed

  to

  th ink abo ut

  the

 answer

  in

 si lence.

In th is way, the teacher in t roduced both the divis ion-quo t ient aspect and

the

 part-whole

 aspect

 by

 usin g icon s ( i.e.,

 the

 p ic tures

 of

 apples)

 a nd

 m a d e

the  difference

  between

  the two  aspects  visible  by  asking  the  students  to

m ental ly solve two p roblems that resul ted in the sam e num ber ( i .e . , 4) .

In  Episode 2, the teacher referred back to the  first  example (12 apples

shared

  equally

  by 3 persons)  and

  asked

  h is

  s tudents

  to

  represent

  it

 u s ing

mathemat ical symbols . He also asked for the resul t of the operat ion. The

teacher then  wrote

 the

  student 's  suggestion

  on the

  b lackboard:

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 106/255

4.

  S I M U L T A N E I T Y

  A N D T H E

  E N A C T E D O B J EC T

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

93

12

—   =4  (apples)

N e x t ,

  the teacher referred to the second example ( th i rds of apples) and

asked , "How m any thi rds

 of an

 apple

 did w e

 need

 to

 pick

 u p, and how

 could

that

 be wri t ten w i th symbols?" T he teache r wrote dow n wh at the s tudents

sugges ted :

12

—   (i.e.,  12 x 1/3  apple)

A g a i n , both aspects w ere focu sed on s imu l taneously in th is episode.

In

 Ep isode 3, the teach er asked the stu den ts to consider and com pare the

tw o

  examples . After

 som e

 questions

  and

  comments f rom

 the

  students,

  the

teacher

 f inal ly conc luded:

.. .

  1 2 divided by 3, and 12 thirds, the resu l t  is the  same.  So w h e th e r

you  have  1 2 apples shared by 3 or you have  1 2 thirds, you w i ll get the

same resu l t .

Aga in , the teacher l inked the two aspects of fract ional num bers by con c lud-

ing  tha t they corresponded to the same nu m ber (4).

Thus,

 thro ugh out this lesson,

 the

 teacher

 ha d

 kept

 the two

 aspects toge ther.

Aspect-Aspect  R e l a t i o n s h i p

  S h o w n

  in  D i f f e r e n t L e s s o n

O r g a n i z a t i o n s

A lthough bo th lessons were teaching  the same content , the part-whole as-

pect

  and the

  divis ion-quot ient aspect

  of

  fract ional

  num bers , t hey

  differed

signif icantly  in term s o f wha t

  aspects w ere

 kept  in focu s, w hat varied, and

whethe r

 the aspec ts were varied at the sam e t im e (see Fig . 4 .1) .

In Lesson

 A, the tw o

 aspects

 of fractional

  num bers were b rough t

 up one

after

  the o ther ( i .e. , f i rst, the

 part-whole

  aspect and then the division-quo -

t ient

  aspect ) .  I t was  only  in the  last episode that  the two  aspects were

b r ough t  together

 as two

  aspects

 of a  fractional

  num ber . Thus ,

  a

 consc ious

effort

  to set up the

 condi t ion

 for the

 s tudents

 to

 discern

 th e

 a spect-aspect

 re-

la t ionship

 betw een the two aspects s im ul tane ously was observed in the f inal

F I G .

  4 . 1 .

  The focused

  a spec t ( s )

 i n

 d i f f e ren t  e p i sode s

 o f

  Lesson

 A and

  Lesson

 B .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 107/255

94  C H I K  AND LO

episode  of Lesson A. In comparison, the teacher of Lesson B

  focused

  on

both aspects in each of the three  episodes. He

  first

  asked the class to men -

tally

 work

 o ut the

 solutions

 of tw o

 problems, each po int ing

 to one of the as-

pects

 o f

 fractiona l

  num bers and resul t ing in the same n um ber (4) . Then, he

asked the students to represent their solutions in term s of m ath em atic al

symbols.

  He

 concluded tha t

 the

 same num ber could

 be

 arrived

 at by

 m e th -

ods

 that reflected

 the different

 aspects

 of

 fractional num ber s.

 In

 th is way .

 the

relationship between the two

 aspects

 perm eated the who le lesson and the

condition   for the exp erience  of the relations hip w as co nsciou sly created all

through the lesson. This is an exam ple of the pat tern of variation refe rred to

as

  us ion

  in

 chapter

  1 : The two

 aspects vary together

 to

 br ing abo ut

 a

 s imu l -

taneous awareness

  of

 both

  in the

  learners.

The two lessons il lustrate how two aspects o f the sam e direct object of

learning

 (fractional

 num bers )

 can be

 brou ght in to students '

 focal

 awareness

simultaneously .

  In the

  fol lowing sect ions,

  we try to

  invest igate

 how

 stu-

dents ' learning outcom es are affected  by the w ay in w h i ch a lesson  is struc-

tured—from

  the point of view of the part-whole relationship s.

A   P A I R   O F  PRIMARY  GRADE  2

C H I N E S E   L A N G UAG E

  L ES S O N S

Tw o pr imary Grade 2 Chinese langu age lessons tau gh t by two   different

teachers  from

  the

  same school were selected

  for the

  study. B oth lessons

were th e first of a senes o f lessons dealing w ith th e teach ing of vocabulary

in a

 text. Both classes

  (2 A and 2B)

 consisted

 o f

 s tudents

 w i th

 s imi lar

 abili-

ties.

  T he

  title

 of the

  text

  was "A

 polite l i t t le guest."

  T he

  com ple te tex t

 is

translated into E nglish and reproduced in Table 4.1 .

Description of Lesson  2A

The lesson  can be roughly divided into

  four

  episodes. In Episode 1, the

teacher introduced

  the

 main them e

 of the

  text

 as

  "being polite." This

 w as

TABLE   4.1

The

  Text Used

  in

 Both

 Lesson 2 A a nd Lesson 2 B

Lesson

  10: A

 Pol i te Li t t le Guest

Father  intended

  to

 visi t

  a

  fr iend  w i t h

 us. He

 asked , " W h a t m u s t

 on e do to be a

  pol i te

little guest?"

 M y

 y o u n g e r  sister said, "When

  we are

  ea t ing,

 w e

  should

  no t

  turn  food over

again  and again to  select  food  in the dish." M y elder sister said, "W e should  look at the

person

  we are

 ta lk ing

 to, no t

  look  from  side

 to

 side.

 W h e n

  other people

 are

 t a lk ing ,

 w e

should not

  interrupt ."

 I

 said,

 "On

  leaving,

 we must say

 goodbye."  Father

  w as

  very

pleased   after

  hea r ing  these , he prom ised to  give us the  c ha nc e  to  show ourse lves  to be a

pol i te guest .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 108/255

4 . S I M U L T A N E I T Y

  A N D T H E

  E N A C T E D O B J E C T

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  95

done by asking  the students to first nom inate  the  classmates that they con-

sidered

 to be

 poli te,

 to

 give their

 reasons, and

 then

 to

 read

 the

 text

 in

 si lence

to f ind out w h a t  w as mean t  by being  poli te.

In

  Episode 2, the teacher taught the at tributes (e.g. , forms, pronuncia-

t ions,

 and me anings) of seven words

3

  that had been p re-iden tif ied

 from

  th e

text. M any teaching aids were used to show the m eaning of each word (e.g. ,

drawings

  and

  real objects) . References were also made

  to

  show

  how the

mean ings

 o f

 these wo rds con tributed

 to the

 t hem e

 of the

 text— being pol i te .

With each word they studied,  the  teacher posted  up on the b lackboard  the

corresponding word card.

In E pisode 3, the teacher asked the students to f ind the sen tence in the text

where

  each word that

  they

  had

 studied

  w as

 used,

  and to

 read

  the

 sentence

out  loud.

 The

  sentences  were then p osted

  up .

 W hen

  all the

  sentences (each

involving  on e or tw o of the

 w ords) were found,

 the

 teacher told

 the

 students

that

  if

 they

 did all of the

  th ings that were  described

  in the

  sentences, they

wou ld

 be

 po l i te chi ldren. The phrase "polite children" [  ^ = f ̂ |^ ^j ̂   -f- ]

w as

 a l so pos ted

 up . The

  s tudents

 w ere

 then asked

 to read the

 text together .

In  th is way, the teacher focused the s tudents ' a t tent ion on the words as

parts

 o f the

 sen tences , w hich were

  in

 turn parts

 of the

 text

 an d i ts

 them e

 of

"being   poli te."

In   Episode 4, the teacher taught the students how to write some of the

characters .

4

Des cr i p t i o n

  o f  L e s s o n  2 B

There are five m ain episodes in the lesson. In Episode 1, the teac her gave

the stud en ts an overall idea of w hat the text w as abou t by asking a few ques-

t ions

 to

  in t roduce

  the

 content

  of the

 text .

In E pisode 2, the teacher foc used on teaching the form s of a num ber of

charac ters that she thoug ht her s tudents migh t f ind

 difficult.

  Individua l stu-

dents were invited

 to

 come

 o ut and try to

 w ri te each character

 o n the

 b lack-

board.

  T he

  other students were required

  to pay

  at tention

  and to

  j u dg e

whe the r the character was correc tly writ ten or no t . I f not , the teacher w ould

invite othe r studen ts to try unti l the charac ter was co rrectly writ ten. In some

cases,  the

  teacher also posted

  up a

  word card highlighting

  the

  structure

 of

the character.

In E pisode 3, the teach er identified a set of w ords

 from

  the text and taug ht

the m e a n in g

 of

 each wo rd. From t im e

 to

 t im e,

  the

 studen ts w ere invited

 to

give a verbal explanation of a certain w ord o r to act out i ts me an ing.

In

  Episode 4, the teacher focused on teaching the pronunciat ion of the

words in the  text. This w as done b y  asking different  groups of  students  to

take

  turns reading them. Ins tant feedback

  w as

  given when

  the

  students

could  not pronounce  the w ords correctly.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 109/255

96

C H I K .  A N D L O

In

 Ep isode  5, the  teacher introduced  a m a tch ing game . In th i s gam e, the

teacher showed  the  students three phrases one by one and  asked them,  in

grou ps, to m atch the phrases by finding a w ord in the text tha t convey ed the

same meaning.

Part-Whole

  R e l a t i o n s h i p  i n t h e  H i era rch i ca l S tructure

of Lesson

 2A

Lesson

  2A was

  structured

  in a

  hierarchical

  w ay

  that showed clear

 part-

whole re la t ionships between the word a t t r ibutes (each as an aspect of a

charac ter), the ch aracters (each as part of a wo rd), the w ords (ea ch as part of

a

 sentence),

 th e

 sentences

 (each

 as

 part

 of the

 text)

 and the

 text

 (a s

 con tribut-

ing to the u nderstanding of i ts them e). Figure 4.2 i l lust ra tes the part-who le

re la t ionships  in the hierarch ical organ izat ion o f  Lesson  A .

For

 example,

  th e

 character that

 has the

 meaning

 o f "insert"

 [}ff

 ] w as first

learned  on i ts own  wi th a  part icular form, pronuncia t ion, a nd  m e a n i n g . It

w as

 then referred

  to in the

 context

  of the

 word "interrupt"

  [} §

 * % ] , which

 is

m ade up of two characters ,

 "insert"

 [}fj] and "m outh"  [°%]. The charac te rs

now   took on a different  m e a n i n g in com bina tion . In th is wa y , their re la t ion

w as

 such that they were

 no

 long er

  tw o

 separate p arts

 pu t

 together,

 bu t

 con-

stituted a

 who le,

 in

 w hich each

 was a

 part . Hence, w hen

 the

 wo rd "interrupt"

[ t§ "H ] was focused on, the characters "insert" [ ̂ ] and "mouth" [ °% ], as

w ell as their relatio n to each other , w ere broug ht

 s imul taneous ly

 to the s tu-

 

FIG. 4.2.

  A n

  i l lus t ra t ion

 of the

 part-whole re la t ionship shown

  in the

 h ie r a r ch ica l

structure

  of  Lesson  2A .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 110/255

H H

den t s ' focal  awareness . Nex t , the word was seen again in a sentence, "W hen

others

  are

 ta lk ing

 to

 each other,

 w e

 should

 n ot  interrupt"

  [}fj

 *$].

 Here

 the

word, w hich com pr ises  tw o ch aracters , becomes part  of a sentence  on po-

l i teness

 th at contributes

 to the

 learning

 of

 poli tene ss. Thus,

  the

 part-whole

relat ionships between   the characters and the word, as w e l l as the word and

the

 sen tence, become clear . Fina l ly

 the

  sentence, which involved

 the

 word

as a part, wa s seen in the con text of a text . Together w ith al l the other sen-

tences

 identified from the text , i t also con tributed to the und erstan ding of the

theme of the text. A gain , w hi le focu sing on the text , the sentence, the w ord,

and the charac ters were s imul taneous ly focused on to

 facili tate

  the discern-

m e n t  of the

 par t -w hole

 relat ionships as we l l as the relat ionships betw een

each par t .

In this w ay, Lesson 2A dem onstrated h ow the parts (e.g., different  attrib-

utes of a charac ter , an d

 different

  characters that m ake up a word) were em -

bedded in the wholes (e.g. , a character is embedded in a word; a word is

embedded

 in a

 sentence) . Therefore

  it

 provided

  the

 s tudents w i th

 a

 s imu l ta -

neous

  experience (and hence, discernment)

  of

  these

  par t -w hole

 relat ion-

ships throu gh ou t the lesson. The s tudents cou ld refer  to the word ca rds, the

sentence

 stops,

 and the text throughou t  the lesson, thus making

 possible

 the

discernment

  through synchronic s im ul taneity .

Part -Whole

  R e l a t i o n s h i p

  i n t h e

  Seq uent i a l S truc ture

of  L es s o n  2 B

T he teacher of Lesson 2B spent m ost of the

 class

 t im e o n vocabulary teach -

ing,

 w hic h she organized unde r the three at t r ibutes of w ords ( form , pron un -

cia t ion, and m eaning ) and presented in a sequ ent ial m anner (see Fig . 4 .3) .

In this lesson, the

 different

  attributes of a character were taugh t in  differ-

ent episodes, but no at tempt was made to relate the   different  at tributes of

each character

 to one

 another ( the aspect-aspect relat ionships) .

 A s a

 resul t ,

some s tudents m ay have unders tood the characters as to tal ly discrete ent i -

t ies.

 Let us take fo r exam ple, the word "interrup t" [$|  °^], which consists o f

two cha racters , the first one m eaning "insert"  [ j j ] ,  and the second m eaning

"mouth" [°^]. Instead

 o f

 teaching

 the

 form s

  of

 these

  tw o

 characters

  at the

F IG . 4.3.

  T he

 se quentia l organizat ion

  of

 Lesson

 2B .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 111/255

98

C H I K   A N D L O

TABLE

  4.2

A

 Comparison of the Structures of Lessons

 2A

  a n d 2B

Lesson

  2A

Lesson

  2B

Word  1 -»

form;

p ro n u n c i a t i o n ;

m e a n i n g

Word 2 -»

fo rm;

p ro n u n c i a t i o n ;

m e a n i n g

Word

  3 -» ...

form;

p r onunc i a t i on ;

m e a n i n g

Form   •>

w or d  1 ;

word 2;

word 3;

P r o n u n c i a t i o n  ->

w or d  1 ;

w o r d  2;

w or d

  3;

M e a n i n g

w or d  1 ;

word 2 ;

w o r d

  3;

same t im e

 as

 teaching

 the way in

 w hich they combine

 to

 form

  the

 wo rd ' 'in-

terrupt" (as the teacher  in Lesson 2A did)  th e teacher  of Lesson  2B taught

the

  form s

 of the first

 character

  on i ts ow n in

 Episode

 2, and the

 p r o n u n c i a -

tion

  of

 this character

 as part of the

 word

  "interrupt" in

 Episode

  4.

 F urther-

m ore in Episode 5, the word "interrup t" w as given a

 defini t ion

  (" joining in

others ' conversation"  [fa   ̂ #J A ^ |£ f£ ]) tha t was ne i ther s i tuated in

 the

con text of being po li te, nor re lated to the text .

Thus,

  in

 each

 of the

  three con secutive episodes

 of

 Les?on

 2B, one o f the

word at t r ibutes ( form, pronunciat ion,

 or

 m e a n i n g )

 w as

 kept invariant ,

 and

thus became superordinate , whi le the characters /words   tha t  varied were

used to illustrate  the attribute. B y con trast,  the words were  the focus of Les-

son 2A, and each w ord becam e supe rordinate w hi le the three at t r ibutes (or

aspects) varied  at the sam e t im e, in order  to enhance  th e s imul taneou s expe-

rience of the w ord at tributes. This is ano ther exa m ple of the pattern of varia-

t ion called

 fusion.

  A m a jo r

  difference

  be tween  the two classes  is w h e t h e r

at tr ibutes or w ords were supe rordinate , as determ ined by the different  w a y s

in

 w h ich

  th e

  lesson

  w as

  structured (see Table 4.2).

The

  quest ion

  w e

 need

  to ask is,

 does th is

 difference

  have

 an

  i mp a c t

 on

students ' learning outcomes?

Des cr i b i ng L ea rn i ng  Outcomes

Af te r each lesson,

  the

 s tudents

 in

 bo th c lasses w ere reques ted

 to

 com ple te

a worksheet . Thi r ty worksheet s were co l lec ted  from  Class 2A. and 31

from

  Class

  2B. The

  workshee t con t a ined

 tw o

  par t s .

  In the first

 par t ,

  the

s tudents were asked

  to put

 down w ha t they thoug h t

 w as the

 mos t impor -

tan t th ing tau gh t and/or l earned in the l esson . The second pa r t requ i red the

students to

 com ple te

 a text by fil l ing in b lanks wi th ap propriate  words tha t

had

  been t augh t

 in the

  lesson (see Table

  4.4 for the  t ex t ) .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 112/255

4.  S I M U L T A N E I T Y

  AND THE

  E N A C T E D O B J E C T

  OF  L E A R N I N G  99

What  W as

 Taught

  and  Learned From  the  S tuden t s '

 Perspective?

  In an-

sw er ing the  first par t of the quest ion, some s tudents put down m ore tha n one

response . A s shown in Table 4 .3 , the responses of both c lasses  fel l  in to two

m ain ca tegor ies . Som e s tudents r eported tha t they w ere taug ht the them e

(e .g . ,  "how  to be poli te," "being  a pol i te guest")  and  sub them es o f the tex t

(e.g.,

 "When others are

 talking

 to

 each other,

 we

 should

 not

 interrupt."),

 and

som e repor ted that they were tau gh t vocabulary. How ever,

 dif ference s

  wer e

observed in their pat tern of responses .

More students from Class 2A reported the main theme of the text or the

subthem es as the

 mo st important thing taught.

 In

 co ntrast,

 a

 higher prop ortion

 o f

Class 2B  considered vocabulary th e m ost important thing taugh t in the lesson.

W hen w e look a t the b reakdown of the s tuden ts ' r esponses , we can see

tha t

 the answers of the  students  in

 Class

 2A covered all  subthemes  of the

text , w hereas the answ ers o f the s tudents in Class 2B o nly covered two of the

sub themes .

With

 regard

 to the

 learning

 o f

 vocabulary,

 th e

 students

 of

 C lass

 2B

 m os tly

descr ibed w ha t they had learned in m ore general terms (e .g ., "using w ords to

m a k e

 sentence s," "reading w ords," etc.) . B y co ntrast , a high er proportion of

the

  s tudents

 in

 Class

 2A

 wrote down  specific  wo rds tha t they

 h ad

 learned .

What

  Were

  the

 L ea r n i n g O u t c o mes ?

  T he

 r esu l t

 of the

 secon d par t

 of the

w orksh eet is shown in Table 4 .4 . To assess the ap propr iate use of wo rds, the

s tudents ' answe rs w ere m arked cor rec t even i f they had ma de m inor mis-

takes in the w r i tten  form of the words. W ith respe ct to the w ords " inter rupt"

and

 "pol i te ," wh ich

 had

 been g iven specia l emp has is dur ing teac h ing

 in

 bo th

c lasses ,  we can see tha t Class 2A per formed much bet te r in us ing these

words

 to

 com ple te

 the

 given text

 (30 out of 30

 correct

 for

 C lass

 2 A, and

 only

9

 out of 31 for C las s 2B ) . Rega r d ing the ch arac ters

 ("insert"

  [^|]  and "ap-

p e a r a n c e "

  [ f f e ] )

  w hose form s bo th teachers h ad  spen t time teach ing , again

th e

  s tuden ts o f C lass 2 A were be t te r ab le to w r i te these charac ter s than the

s tudent s

 f rom C lass 2B . Only one m is take (on "insert") was m ad e in C las s

T A B L E   4.3

The

  S t u d e n t s '

 Responses

  S h o w n

 in the

  First Part

 of the

  Workshee t*

What  is the

  mos t impor tan t th ing

Genera l Ques t ion:

Class

  (N -  t o t a l n u mber of r e sponse s )

1

 . The  m a i n  t h em e a nd  s u b t h e m e s  of the  text

2.  V o c a b u la r y

3.

  I r re l evan t

  r e sponses

taught/ learned

  in the

  le s son?

2 A ( W = 3 5 )  2 E ( W = 3 9 )

18 10

5 22

12  7

*Some  students

 put down

  more  than

  one response.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 113/255

TABLE   4.4

Th e  Students Performances  as  S h o w n  in the Second  Part  of the W orksheet

Second Par t : Complete

  th e

  fo l low ing tex t wi th appropr ia te words /phrases

  y ou

 ha ve

learned in the lesson.

O u r  teacher often te l ls u s t ha t w he n  w e  mee t teache rs  an d  c la ssmates  in school ,  w e have

to  greet

3

  t h e m .  In the  c la ss room, when  th e  teache r  is  t a l k i ng w i t h  other c lassmates ,  do

not in te r rup t a s one l ikes ; w hen the teac he r ta lks to you , do no t look  from  s ide to s ide .

c

Afte r

  school , we have to say goodbye to our teachers and c lassm ates . Then we can be

counted

  as

  good students

 who a re

 po l i t e .

d

[English translation]

2A  2E

1 .  Use of  words

1

 . 1

 A ppropr ia te

  for all

  b lanks

  30 9

1  .2 One to  t h r e e b l a nks m i s s ing / ina pp r op r i a t e  — 22

2. Form

  of

 cha rac te r s

W r o n g l y w r i t t e n

N ot  t a ugh t  in the  le sson :

T a ugh t  in the  lesson:

" M o u t h "

"Wave"

"Watch"

"West"

"Insert"

"Appearance"

8  4

1

1   —

— 1

1   7

  4

Note .

  a

T he

 word

 "greet" [^J

  % Q °^-]

  i s m ade up of

  three Chinese characters , "beat"

T ], "wave" [ j£  ], and "ca l l" [  *f-].

word " in te r rup t"  [ | j |

  ° ^]

  is ma de  up o f two C hin ese cha rac te r s , " inse r t"  \ f f i j

  ]

 and

" mou th"

°The word

  " look

  f rom s ide to s ide" [ Jt ^ \3j St ] '

s

  rnade up of four  Ch ine se c ha r a c t e r s ,

"east" [ Jt ],

 "open"

 [ ̂  ],

 "west"

 [ gj ], and "watch" [ 5t ]•

d

The word

  "pol i te" [^ffe]

  is

 ma de

 up of two

 Ch ines e cha rac te r s , "po l ite"

  [^] and

"appearance

100

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 114/255

4.

  S I M U L T A N E I T Y

  A N D T H E E N A C T E D O B J E C T O F  L E A R N I N G  1 01

2A ,

 w hereas

  1 1

 m istakes w ere noted

  in

 Class

 2B (7 on

 "insert";

 4 on

  "ap-

pearance") .

Simultaneity

  and the

 Possibility

  fo r

  Learning.

  In

 order

 for the

 s tudents

 to

be ab le to use the w ords learned in the lesson to  f i l l  in the blank s o f a given

text,

 i t w as not enough for them simply to know how to w ri te the form of the

characters that made  up that wo rd. T he  students also had to understand  the

meaning of the words, and be able to use them in an appropr ia te context .

That

 is,

 they

 had to be

 able

 to

 discern simu ltaneously

  the

 form ,

 th e

 meaning,

and the usage of the w ord. Lesson 2 A w as s tructured in such a w ay as to a l-

lo w

 the

 students

 to be

 simultaneously  aware

 of the

 w ord attributes (form

 and

m e a n i n g )

  that are cr i t ica l to the words being used appropr ia te ly. Corre-

spondingly,

  w e

 fo und that

  the

 students

 in Class 2A

 were better able

  to

 dis-

cern these a t t ributes s im ultan eou sly than the s tude nts of Class 2B.

In Lesson

 2B ,

 a l thoug h

 the

 s tudents were a lso taugh t

 the

 same w ords ,

 the

cr i t ical a t t r ibutes o f each word were taug ht in different  episodes,  and there

w as no a t tem pt to he lp the s tudents link these aspects together . A lthou gh the

form   of the charac te r meaning "insert"  was taught in Episode 2, and the

m ean ing of the word " interrupt" (w hich consists of the two ch aracters " in-

sert"

  and

  "mouth")

  w as

  t a u g h t

 in

  Episode

  3, the

  s tudents were

  no t

  m a d e

aware s imul taneous ly

 of

  these

  tw o

  attr ibutes (form

  and

  m e a n i n g )

  of the

character "insert,"

  and may

  have considered them

  as two

  different,  unre-

la ted ent i t ies . In oth er wo rds, even if only three words were taug ht in the les-

son, if the three different attr ibutes of each word were taught as unrelated

en t i t i es—ins tead

  of

  present ing them

  as

  three attr ibutes

 o f

  three words—

some students m ay perceive these attributes  as nine unrelated e ntit ies to be

learned. The l a rge numb er o f un re la ted ent i t ies m ight cause some s tudents

to be

 confused, whereas others m ight

 not be

 able

 to

 cope w ith

 so

 m a n y

 n ew

th ings in a

 shor t t ime . This

 m ay

 accoun t

 for the

 fact

 th a t a l thoug h

 the

 teacher

of Lesson 2B spent mu ch t ime teaching the s tudents how to cor rec t ly w r ite

the  forms of the words "insert"  and "appearance," many students  still  got

them  w rong. Of course , some s tudents m ight still be able to exper ience the

at t r ibutes s imul taneously

 because o f p rev ious exper iences . How ever, the si-

m u l t a n e o u s exper ience

  o f different

  a t t r ibutes

 was no t

 b rough t abou t

 by the

co n sc io u s effort  of the teacher , un l ike in Lesson 2A , whe re the teacher con-

sc ious ly  s t ruc tured the learning exper ience s to facil i tate such an exp er ience .

A  P A I R   O F  P R I M A R Y G R A D E   1

E N G L I S H

  L A N G U A G E

  L E S S O N S

In  this section, w e further i l lustrate o u r point by looking  at two double peri-

ods of  E ng l ish languag e lessons taken  from  tw o  pr im ary G rade  1 c lasses

(Class  IB and Class  IE )  that were taught by two dif ferent teachers in the

same schoo l . B oth lessons w ere

 on e of a

 series

 of

  lessons

  on

 "Food

  in the

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 115/255

1 02  CHIK  A N D L O

superm arket ." B efore this lesson, both c lasses of s tud en ts had a lready

learned the n a m e s o f several i tem s o f food a nd d r ink . Bu i ld in g o n th i s

 pnor

knowledge ,  the teachers a im ed to develop the s tu dents ' abi l i ty to use  differ-

ent langu age i tems to describe food and drink (n oun s: e .g., c r isps , m ilk;

partitives: e.g., a bottle of, a pa cke t of; and  adjectives  describin g size: e .g. ,

large, small),  as wel l as  their ability  to extract spec if ic inform ation (prices

of  food  and  drink) through  the use of  d ia logue ("How much  is  it?"  "It is

dollars.").

The two

 classes

  were  of  similar ability  and  most  of the  students were

quite good

 at

 E ngl ish

 as far as

 speaking

  and

  listening were concerned.

Des cr i p t i o n

  o f

  Lesson

  1 B

This  lesson consisted

  of four

 m ain episodes, each

 focus ing on an

 objec t

 of

learning.

  In

 Ep isode

  1, the

 teache r revised

  th e

 names

 of

 e igh t kinds

 o f

 food

and

  drink that

  the

  students

  had

  learned

  in

  previous lessons. Pictures

  and

word cards were shown.

In Episode  2, the teacher introduced th e phrases  "a bottle of and "a packet

of to  describe  th e attributes of some of the food and drink item s shown in Ep-

isode

  1. She

  explained

  to the

  class that there were some

  food

  i tems that

needed

 a

 partitive

 and

 some that

 did

 not .

 She

 represented

 th e

 phrases

 in

 pic to-

rial,  spoken, a nd written fo rm s. A lso, w hen she exp la ined the wo rd "packet,"

which  was new to the students, the teacher made re fe rence to the more

  famil-

iar

 word "bag" that earned the sam e mea ning as "packet."

In   Episode  3, the  teacher expla ined  the use of the  words "large"  and

"small" to

 describe

  different

  sizes

 of the

 sam e kin d

 of food/drink  for ident i -

f icat ion  purposes—in

  her

  words ,

 "so

  that peop le know w hich

 one yo u are

ta lking about ." A gain, pic tures

 and

 word cards were used .

 The

 m e a n in g

 of

the word

 "large"

 was also exp la ined in term s of another word ,

 "big,"

 w h i ch

w as

 more fam iliar

 and has a

 s imilar mean ing

  to "large."

Finally  in Episode  4, the  teacher used the context of a supermarket  to in-

troduce

 the

 dialogue, "How m uch

 is

 it?"

 " It is

  dollars," w hich

 w as

 then

posted

  on the

 blackboard.

  T he

 students practiced

 the

 dia logu e

 in tw o

  differ-

ent

  tasks:

  guessing  the  prices  of  different  food  and  drink i tems,  and

role-playing   the act of buy ing in a superm arke t. In both tasks,  the  teacher

did the ques t ioning w hi le the s tudents w ere invi ted to respond by g iv ing the

"i t is" s ta tem ent e ither on a w ho le-class or  individual bas i s .

Descript ion

  o f

  Lesson

  1 E

This

 lesson

 comprised

 three interrelated

 episodes. In

 Episode

  1, the

 teacher

introduced

  the

 m ain theme

  of the

  lesson, "Buying food

  in a

  supermarket."

Specifical ly,

  the

 teacher focused

 on two

 ma in aspects , nam ely

 the

 food

 and

drink tha t could be fo un d in the superm arket, and the dialo gu e used to ask

for  the prices of food and drink.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 116/255

4.

  S I M U L T A N E I T Y

  A N D T H E

  E N A C T E D  O B J E C T

  O F  L E A R N I N G 1 0 3

In

 Ep isode 2, the teach er began by asking , "What kinds of food a nd dr ink

can

 we find in a

 superm arket?"

 Then, the

 teacher used pictures

 to

 in troduce

eight pairs

  o f

  food

  and

 drink i tem s

 one by

  one. Each pair consis ted

 o f two

dif ferent-s ized

  vers ions of the same kind of food/dr ink (e .g ., one large and

one

 sm all bot t le

 of

 w ater ).

 The

  teacher required

 the

 s tudents

 to

 n a m e e a ch

pai r of i tem s (e .g . , "a large bo ttle of water" and "a sm all bot t le of water"). In

th is way,

 the

 wo rds describ ing s ize,

 the

 p arti tives,

 a nd the

 n a m e s

 o f

 par t icu-

lar k inds of  food  and dr ink were presented to the class s imultaneously .

In   Episode  3, the  teacher to ld  the  class, "Well , here  are  some k inds  o f

food  and dr ink tha t we can f ind in a supermarket . I f we w ant to buy them ,

w h a t

  should

 we say?"

 Then ,

 she

 pos ted

 up the

 d ia logue "How m uch

 is

  it?"

"It is  dollars,"  and  in t roduced  i t as a way of f inding out the

 price

 o f

food  and dr ink i tem s in a superm arket . T he s tu dents then pract iced the d ia-

l o g u e

  by engaging in

  different

  tasks , such as gue ssing the pr ice of cer tain

i t ems of food and/or drink and pretending to buy thing s in the sup erm arket .

Part-Whole  R e l a t io n s h i p s in D i ff e r e n t

 Lesson

  O r g a n i z a t i o n s

A l t h o u g h

  the lessons bo th deal t wi th s imi lar teach ing conten ts , the two

teache rs used

 d i f ferent

 w ays of s t ruc tur ing the i r l essons , and henc e the re la-

t ionsh ips es tab l i shed s im ul tane ou s ly be tween

  the

 ob jec ts

 o f

 learn ing w ere

also  d i f ferent .  In Lesson IB , the teach er p resen ted the ob jec ts o f learn ing

( n o u n s

 fo r

 descr ib ing  food

  and

 dr ink; adjec t ives descr ib ing s ize; par t i t ives

used

 in con junc t ion w i th uncoun tab le nouns ; and a d i a logue pa t te r n ) in a se-

quent i a l w ay (see F ig . 4 .4) . For ins tan ce, in order to  foc us  on "small," the

t eacher

 showed many exam ples

  o f

 sm al l i tem s

 of  food  and

 dr ink. Then ,

  in

order to  foc us on "large," she showed m any ex am ples o f la rge item s of  food

and

  dr ink.

 A s a

 resu l t , despi te

  th e

  fact  tha t

  th e

  s ame examples

  o f

  food

  and

dr ink

  were used th roughout the lesson , on ly one ob jec t o f learn ing was

b r o u g h t to the  fore  in each ep isode . This ob jec t o f learn ing w as then  left  in

the

 backg round in the nex t ep isode w hi le som eth ing e lse w as

 h i gh l igh t e d .

Episode 1

Introduction

of

 the nouns

for different

foods

  and

drinks

b,

Episode 2

Introduction

of partitives:

"A

 bottle of

and

  "A

packet of

^

Episode 3

Introduction

of

  adjectives

fo r  describing

size:

"Large"   an d

"Small"

Episode 4

Introduction

of

 a dialogue

used  to  elicit

prices:

"Dollars"

"How  much

is

 it?"

FIG. 4.4.  T he

  sequent ial s t ructure

  of Lesson IB .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 117/255

1 4

C H I K  A N D L O

In  comparison, the teacher of Lesson IE focused on each kind of

food/drink  as an object that had different attributes (nam e, size, packaging,

and price),  and set up the l earn ing wi th the them e o f buy ing in a supermar-

ket . For ins tance, the food and drink i tems (which have thei r own name,

size, and packag ing) were presented a s the th ing s tha t can be  found  in a su-

permarket (whole)

 and

 also

 as the

 objects being referred

 to in the

 b u y i n g

 ac-

tivity. In

 th i s way ,

 th e

 same

 set of

 nouns , adjectives, par t i t ives ,

 and

 d i a logu e

were organized  in a m anner tha t re la ted them to each other as a t tr ibutes of

certain food/drink i tems,

 a s

 w e ll

 as to the

 them e

 o f

 buy ing

 in the

 superm ar-

ket as a

 w hole . Lesson

  IE can

 t hus

 be

 described

 as

 h a v i n g

 a

 h i e ra rch i ca l

 o r-

ganization (see Fig. 4.5) that ma de

  it

 possible

  for the

 students

 to

  experience

both the

 part-whole

 relat ionsh ips between thin gs and the aspect-aspect re-

lationships betw een at tributes sim ultaneo usly (see also Table 4.5).

E

pisode   1 : Introduction  of the  theme, "Buying  in a  supermarket"

Episode 2: Introduction of food an d drink that can be foun d in

su

dr

pennarket [name, size, and packaging  of a certain kind of food and/or

ink]

Episode

 3 : Introduction of the  buying activity  in  supermarket

["Dollars,"

 "How mu ch is it?" " It is  dollars."]

FIG.

 4.5.

  The

  hierarchica l  s tructure

  of

  Lesson

 1 E .

TABLE

  4.5

A  C omparison o f the Structures o f Lesson IB and L esson IE

Lesson

  IB

Lesson   IE

N o u n s

Adject ives

descr ib ing

size  •>  Partitives

Type

  of

  food/

  Type

 of food/

  Type

 of

  food/

dr ink

  1 ->  d r ink  2 ->  dr ink  3 -^ ...

food/drink  1 ;

food/dr ink

  2;

food/dr ink  3;

food/drink  1 ;

food/drink 2;

food/dr ink  3;

food/drink   1 ;

food/drink

  2;

food/dr ink  3

n o u n ;

adjectives

descr ib ing

size; w i t h

or

  w i t hou t

part i t ive

n o u n ;

adject ives

desc r ib ing

size;

 wi th

or

 w i t h o u t

par t i t ive

n o u n ;

adjectives

descr ib ing

size;

 w i t h

or  w i t h o u t

parti t ive

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 118/255

4.  S I M U L T A N E I T Y  A N D T H E

  E N A C T E D O B J E C T

  O F  L E A R N I N G

105

Because of the

  different foci

  and organization of the lessons, the two

classes also

  differed  in

  what came

  to be

  superordinate.

  In

 Lesson

  IB, the

no un s, adjectives, and part i t ives tha t described  food and drink i tem s becam e

superordinate,

  as

 each

 w as

 focused

 on and

 i llustrated w ith varied exam ples

of food/drink i tems, whereas

  in

 Lesson

  IE, the

  food/drink became super-

ordinate

 as the

 teacher focused

 on each of the

 food

 a nd

 drink

 i tem s and

 var-

ied the nouns and adjectives  used to describe them (see Table 4.5).

In Lesson  IE , how ever, w e  find another example of "fusion," tha t is, di f -

ferent  aspe cts (size, parti t ives) varying together. W hat is the im pac t of th is

structural difference  on student learning?

D e s c r i b i n g L e a r n i n g  Outcomes

To  investigate the impact of structural  difference  on student learning, 43

students in

 C lass

  IB an d 45

 s tudents

 in

 Class

  IE

 w ere invi ted

 to

 com ple te

 a

worksheet immedia te ly   after

  the

  lesson.

  T he

  worksheet consis ted

  of two

questions.  T he

  first

  question required  the  students  to put  down what they

perceived

  as the

 m ost imp ortant th ing taugh t and/or learned

  in the

 lesson.

The second question required them to fi l l in blanks in a dialogue (see Fig.

4.6

 w here

  the

  expected answers

  are

 underl ined) .

F I G .

 4.6.

  T he

  second ques t ion

 of the

  workshee t .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 119/255

1 06

C H I K  A N D L O

T he   Students Un ders tanding

  of

  W h a t

  They

  Learned

  in the

  Lesson.  A s

shown in Table 4.6, nearly a ll the students in Class  1 B reported that the most

important things taught  in the  lesson were some language items (e.g.,

names, adjectives describing size, partitives,

  and the

 dialogue). Only

  o ne

student mentioned that they

 had

 learned how

 to find the

 prices,"

 a

 concept

that should have been  one of the main  foci  of the lesson.

T A B L E   4.6

Th e

  Students Responses

  to the

 First General Question

 of the

 Written Task

1 . 1

1 .2

1 .3

1 .4

1 .5

1 .6

1 .7

1 .8

1 .9

1 . 1 0

2.1

2.2

3.1

3.2

T he

  first

  que s t i on :

(N   =

 total number o f

  re sponses;

 some students

gave more than o n e  response)

]

 .

 Language items

S om e  food

  and

  dr ink i t ems

Good s

 o the r than

  1 . 1

Size

Part i t ives

Size

  and

  par t i t ives

Dol l a r

Pr ices

  of d i f f e ren t

  food

  and

  dr ink i t ems

Pr ices

  o f goods

 other than food

  and

 d r i n k

T h e

  How m u c h ?"

 d i a l ogue

Learn ing Engl i sh in genera l

  terms (e.g.

 r e a d i ng ,

w r i t i n g

  E n g l i s h ;

 some

 E ng l i sh w or ds )

2.

  Theme

How to  find  the pr ices

C o n d u c t i n g

  th e

  d i a l ogue

  o r

  a sk ing

  the  how

m u c h ? "

 ques t ion or buy ing food and dr in k in the

contex t  o f  supermarke t  o r  t e a c h i ng s i tua t i on

3.

 Others

I r re levant

M i s s i n g

W h a t

  was the

 most

  important

t h i n g

  t aught / l ea rned  in the

lesson?

Class

  IB  Class  IE

(N  = 50) (N = 48)

48 40

26 —

— 4

4 8

1

  —

3 —

10

  6

3 —

—   4

1   1 4

—   4

  8

1   —

— 8

I Q

1

  —

— —

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 120/255

4.

  S I M U L T A N E I T Y  A N D T HE  E N A C T E D O B J E C T  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 07

There were relatively fewer responses in C lass IE (40 out of 48) indicating

language

 i tem s

 as the

 most imp ortant thing taught.

 O ne

 sixth

 o f the

 students

actually

 quo ted som e exam ples from their ow n personal experience

  of buying

goods

 at a

 supermarket; these exam ples included i tem s men tioned

  in the

 les-

son

 (e.g.,

 food or

 drink)

 as

 w ell

 a s

 some

 not

 m ent ioned

 in the

 lesson (e.g.,

 ap-

ples, m elons, a teddy bear). N one of the students from the other class did so.

Also ,

  regarding those responses that m ent ioned the them e of buy ing

th ings

 in a

 superm arket,

  the

 responses

  from

 Class

  IE

 w ere m ost ly related

 to

the context of buying in a superm arket, con duct ing the dialogue, and asking

the  "How much?" quest ion, whereas , the responses

  from

  Class IB were

main ly  abo ut el ic i t ing prices ,  an d the context w as  seldom ment ioned.

What

  Are the

 Learning Ou tcomes?

  The  second quest ion required  th e

s tudents

 to first fill in appropriate words to indicate the different  sizes of two

bags pr inted

 on the

 worksheet ,

  and

 then

 to fill in the

 m iss ing blanks

 in a

 s im -

u la ted d i a logue

 on

 buying

 and

 sel l ing.

 In

 order

 to be

 able

 to fill in the

 appro-

pr iate

 w ords in the f irst  section, the s tudents had to be  able to  different iate

large and

 sm al l objects

 and to use the

  correct vocabulary.

 In

 order

 to fill in

the  b lanks

 of the

 dia logue ,

 the

  students

 had to be

 able

 to

 recognize

  the

 sen-

t ences

 as par ts o f a dialogu e carr ied ou t in a buy ing and  sel l ing context be-

fore

  they cou ld choose

  the

  most appropriate words

  to fill in the

 b lanks .

A s

 shown

 in

 T able 4.7, nearly

 all of the

 s tudents

 in

 both classes were able

to  descr ibe  the two bags w i th appropriate w ords that indicated their s ize.

This

  show s that both classes

  had a

  good m astery

  of the

  words descr ib ing

size

 as  t a ugh t in the  lesson. However, a differenc e  w as  observed  in the two

c la sse s 'pe r fo rma nce in  answer ing the second part o f the que s t ion . W hereas

m ost of the

 s tudents

 in

 Class

  IE

 w ere able

 to

 un ders tand

 the

 dia logue

 as be-

long ing

  to a

 buy ing

 a nd

  sel l ing context ,

 a nd

  comple ted

  it in a

  me a n i ng f u l

way,

  only

 o ne

 third

 o f

 C lass

  IB

  could

 do so.

 Many s tudents

 in

 C lass

  IB did

no t

 recognize

 the two

 sentences

 a s

 cons t i tu t ing

 a

 dia logue ,

 and

 took them

 as

single  sentences unrelated to one another . F or  ins tance, some s tudents u n-

derstood

 the first

 inco m plete sentence, "Good a f te rnoo n .

  ,

 please."

  in a

c lass room

  context ,

  and f il led in the

  wo rds "sit dow n," even thou gh they

m ade sense

 of the

 second sen tenc e," dol lars , p lease."

 in the

 buying

 and

se l l ing contex t ,

 and f il led in the

 word

 "ten."

Simultaneity

  and the

 Possibility

  for

  Learning.

  One

  possible

  way to ac-

coun t

  fo r

 these d i f ferences

  in

 learning

 is the

  structural difference  observed

in

 the tw o

 t eachers ' ways

 o f

 handl ing

 the

 objects

 of

 learning.

 T he

 t eacher

 of

Lesson IE

  organized

  the

  direct objects

 o f

  learning

  in a

 hierarch ical s truc-

ture,

  us ing "buying

  in a

  supermarket"

  as a

  theme.  Different  at tributes

( n a m e ,

 s ize, pack aging, and price) o f each food/drink we re focus ed on at the

same t ime, w hich

 afforded  the

 po ssibil i ty

 of the

 s im ul taneous experience

 o f

those

  at tributes. A s a resu lt , m ore o f the students in Lesson IE expressed

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 121/255

TABLE

  4.7

Th e

 S t u d e n t s'

 Answers to the

  Sp ecif ic  Quest ion

 of the Worksheet

Second part :

  F i l l  in the blanks

(A )  A

  large/big bag.

A

  small bag.

IB

  IE

Class

  (N = 43) (N = 45)

1   Use of words to indic ate s ize:

1 . 1   Appropr ia te  39 45

1 .2

  One to t h ree b l an k s m i s s i n g /  inappropr ia te  4 —

(e.g.

  "a

  large

 of

  bag,"

  "a

  smal l

  of

  bag")

2.

 W r o ng

  spel l ing

  o f

  words ( large, smal l , bag)

  6 1 1

(B) SI :

  Good af ternoon,

  a

  large/big bag/any

  goods.

please.

S 2:  Here  you  are.

S3: How   m u c h  is it?

S4: Ten  dol lars , p lease.

1. Fil l  in  bo th b lanks appropr ia te ly

 ( e i t h e r

  u s i n g the 15 37

i n format ion

  from

  the

  lesson, i .e.

  the

  e igh t k inds

 o f

food

  and

  d r ink i t ems ,

 o r

  that provided

  in the

worksheet , i .e . two bags of

  different

  s izes )

2.   Fi l l  in one of the blanks appropriately

2.1

  ,

 please.

  — 2

2.2

  dollar s, please.

  1 8

a

  4

3.   Inappro priate answers  to

3.1   both

  2.1 and 2.2 2

b

  —

3 . 2 2 . 1

  a n d

  l eav in g

 2 .2

 b l an k

  l

c

  —

3.3 2.2 and

  l e av in g

 2.1

  b l an k

  2

d

  —

4. Did not

  respond

  5 2

N o t e .  I nappropr i a t e answers  to  2 .1 ,  fo r  example, "si t down,"  "Miss  W ." " H a v e a b i scu i t . " "many

in," "you are,"  "teacher  and,"

  "good

  night ," " thank you."

Inappropr iate answers  to, for exam pl e ,  2.1  "you," "teacher  and";  2.2 "here." "it is"

Inappropr i a t e

 answers  to, for example, 2.1 and leaving 2.2 blank ("many in") .

Inappropr iate answers to, for example, 2.2 and leaving 2.1 blank  ("it,"  "it is").

1 8

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 122/255

4 . S I M U L T A N E I T Y

  A N D T H E

  E N A C T E D O B J E C T

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  1 09

w hat they  h ad learned  in a contextual  and organized w ay. In this lesson ,  be-

cause

  th e

  food

  and

  drink items were superordinate

  to

  their attributes,

  it

seems that

 the

 students cou ld relate mo re readily

 to the

  fact  that

 the

 objects

of learning w ere

  the

  food

 and

 drink items that

 can be

 purchased

  in a

  super-

market , and that the "How

  much? "

 dialogue w as used to  inquire about price

in   a supermarket .  The  students could hence  see the relevance  of wh at they

learned, because i t w as fam iliar to their daily experience. In Lesson IB ,

however , because

  the

  words and/or adjectives describing

  the

  attributes

  of

food  and drink were su perordinate to the  food and drink i tem s, it seem s that

mos t

  of the

  students

  not

  only

 had

  difficulties  relating these attributes (the

languag e i tem s)

 and the use o f the

 dialogue

 to the

 t hem e

 of

 "buying

  in a su-

permarket,"

 but  also  in

  relating

  them to

  their

  ow n

 daily

  experiences.

 A l-

though some s tudents might have been able

  to

 recognize

  the

  relat ionship

betw een the two by draw ing on their ow n past experiences, this had been

  left

to c han ce, ins tead

 o f

 being co nsciou sly structured

 by the

 teacher.

  In

 Lesson

1E,

 on the o ther hand, the consciou s effort  of the teache r to structure the les-

son using the theme of buying in a superm arket

  afforded

  the stude nts the si-

m ul taneou s experience  of context , w hole  and parts .

Because l anguage i t ems such

  as

  part i t ives were highlighted

  and

  kept

superordinate

  in Lesson IB and not in Lesson IE, we w ould expect to see

some difference  between Lesson IB and Lesson IE w i th respect to the s tu-

dents ' unders tanding of these langu age i tem s. That i s, we should see that

some studen ts in C lass IB should be able to

 identify

  these i tems as the mo st

imp ortant th ing taught

 in the

  lesson. This

  w as

 indeed borne

  out by the

 stu-

dent da ta: Four s tudents in Class IB m ent ioned part it ives as being the m ost

impor tan t

  thing being taug ht , w hereas none

 of the

  students

  from

  Class

 1 E

mentioned part i t ives (see Table 4.6).

C O N C L U S I O N S

So far, we have discussed the possible   effect  of lesson structures on stu-

dents ' learning outcomes. This  effect  might be unders tood in terms of

whe the r

 aspect-aspect

  and/or

 part-whole

  relat ionships could

  be

  experi-

enced s imul taneous ly

 by the

 learner. Usin g

 th e

 example

 o f the

 pai r

 o f

 m a th -

emat ics lessons ,

  w e

  i l lustrated

  the

  different  w a y s

  in

  wh ich

  the

  teachers

created the con di t ions for the s im ul taneo us experience o f the two aspec ts of

fract ional

  numbers . We then gave examples of two Chinese language les-

sons

 and tw o

 E ngl ish lang uag e lessons

  to

 i l lustrate

 how the

  teachers w ere

able

 to structure their lessons to facilitate the s tude nts ' s im ul taneou s experi-

ence  o f parts  and w holes,  and  aspects versus aspects.

In the ana lysis of these tw o pairs of langu age lessons, i t w as also shown

that

 the

 differences

  in

  students' learning outcomes

  are

 related

 to the

  struc-

tural

 differences  in the way s in w hic h the teachers organized the objects of

learning, tha t is, to the differences  in the enacted object o f learning. The in-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 123/255

1 1 0  CHIK  A N D L Q

teresting  fact  is tha t in the case o f these tw o pairs o f lessons, the two teachers

used  the same  teaching material, subscribed  to the  same intended object o f

learning,  and almost certainly believed tha t they carried  out the sam e lesson

as

 w el l.

 W e c an

 presum e that both teachers w ould have said that

 the

 enacted

object  o f  learning was,  o f course,  the  same  in both classes.

How ever, this w as not the ca se— as w e have seen. T he enacted o bject o f

lea rn ing  differed  sub s tant ia l ly in both ins tanc es . This i s because w ha t stu-

dents  learn is not a

  func t ion

  of the  in tended  ob jec t of lea rn in g , but of the

enacted  ob jec t  o f  l e a rn ing ,  and  w h a t  the  s tudents  in  these  tw o  c lasses

learned c lear ly  d i f fered .  The teachers were  f ound  to have directed stu-

dents' a t tent ion  to par t icu la r aspec ts o f w h a t w as t o be learned by s tructur-

ing the

 lesson

 in

 such

 a way

 tha t

 th e

 chosen aspec ts were kept invar iant

 and

superordinate  to  those aspects that var ied.  B u t  w h a t  w as  invar iant  an d

superord ina te m ade a d i f fe ren ce to how w el l the s tude nts achieved the ex-

pected  l e a rn ing ou tcomes .

This has s igni f icant imp l ica tions for w ays of im proving the qu a l i ty of the

teaching-learning cyc le . It is l ikely that s tuden ts w il l learn bet ter  if teachers

can structure their lessons in such a way tha t s tudents are able to discern and

exper ience s im ultan eou sly par ts and w holes, and aspects versus aspects .

E N D N O T E S

'For  the backg round  o f  th is research p roject ,  see the E p i lo g u e .

2

It  i s impor tan t to no te tha t accord ing to the no ta t ion sys tem used in S w eden ,

6/3 has  dual meanings: "s ix th i rds"  and  "six divided b y  three."

3

W e use

 "word"

 here to refer  to a m ean ing fu l en t i ty in the C hinese l anguage . A

m e a n i n g f u l

  ent i ty

  can be

  s ing le -sy l labic (cons i s t ing

  of one

  cha r ac t e r )

 o r

  mu l t i -

syllabic (comprising   two o r  more characters) .

4

W e

 u se "characters"  to

 re fer

 to the w r i t ten fo rm s o f m o r p h e m e s  in the Chinese

language.  Some  characters  are m ean ing f u l on their  ow n  (s ingle-syl labic en t i t ies)

and

 some need

 to be

 co up led w i th o the r cha rac te r s

 so as to

 m a k e

 a

 mean in g fu l

  en-

t i ty (m ul t isyl labic ent i t ies) . B ecau se a l l the s in gle-sy l labic ent i t ies referred to in

this chapter have thei r ow n m eaning s , and bec ause very

 of ten

  the i r fo rm s o f w r i t-

ing

 were deal t w i th

 in the

 lesson,

 w e

 sha l l refer

 to

 these

 a s

 "characters,"

  and

 re fe r

 t o

mult isyl labic ent i t ies

 as

 "words"

  fo r

 easy re fe rence .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 124/255

 

O n

  Language

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 125/255

  his page intentionally left blank

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 126/255

5

Questions

and   the  Space  of  Learning

Amy B M

Tsui

Ference

 Marton

I d a   A. C. Mok

Dorothy

  F P Ng

Q U E S T I O N S

Quest ions  is  perhaps  the  most thoroughly researched area  of  classroom

learning.

 This is probably because it is the most distinctive feature of

 class-

room

 discourse .

 A

 lesson

 is a

 speech event w here people com e together

 and

engage in an

  activity referred

  to as

 "learning." Lessons

  are

  organized

 in

such a way

 tha t there

 is at

 leas t

 one

 person

 in the

 classroom

 who is the

 "pri-

mary   knower" (Berry, 1987)  and who is  responsible  for d i s s e m i n a ti n g

knowledge

  to the others. ' The knowledge gap between the "pr imary

knower"—that

  is, the

 t eache r— and

 the

 "secondary know er"— that

 is, the

s tudents—vests

 author i ty

 in the

 fo rm er

 in

 de termin ing

 the

 direct ion that

 the

lesson

 wi l l take ,

 th e

 act ivi t ies that w il l

 be

 conducted,

 the

 quest ions that w il l

be

  asked,

  and

  what constitutes appropriate answers

  to

  these questions.

W h e n

  a

  teacher asks

  a

 que st ion,

  the

 purpose

  is not to

 obtain inform ation

that

 the teacher does n ot have, but to check whether  the students have the

m issing inform ation indicated

 in the

 quest ion. W hen

 a

 teacher asks, "W hat

t ime

 is it,

 Johnny?"

 Johnny know s that he is supposed to tell the teacher the

t ime  even tho ugh there is a big clock on the w all that everybody can see. If

Johnn y says, "W ell, look

 at the

 clock

 on the

 w all,"

 or if he

 pu ts

 th e

 question

back

 to the teacher,

 "What

 does the clock

 say?"

 the class knows that John ny

is head ing

 for

 t rouble.

1 1 3

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 127/255

1 1 4  T S U I  E T A L .

Students also know that questions  are not asked  for their ow n sake, but that

they have

 a

 pedagog ical m otivation behind them , even thoug h

 i t m ay no t be

clear

  to the

  students exactly what that motivation

  is.

  Therefore, when

  a

teacher asks a series of qu estions, the students are supposed to try and answ er

each  one as best they can. They are not supposed  to query the purpose of those

questions

 by

 asking, "W hat

  are you

 trying

 to get

 at?"

 or

 "What

 has

 this ques-

t ion got to do w ith the preceding question?" at least not in A sian classroom s.

Qu estions serve a num ber of other purposes in the classroom besides know l-

edge checking. For example, questions can be used for classroom manage-

m ent purposes such as preventing th e students cha tting, getting the students to

focus

 on the

  lesson rather than daydreaming,

 and so on

  (see Tsui, 1995).

 In

this chapter,

  we are not

  interested

  in

  classroom questions

 per se, but in the

way the space of learning is c onsti tuted l ingu ist ical ly by the quest ion s asked

by the teacher, tha t is, the responses  that can be

 el ici ted,

 and those that are ac-

tually elicited,  and wha t  th e  teacher accepts  as  appropriate.

Q U E S TIO N S  AN D  FOCAL AWARENESS

Qu es t ions asked  a t cruc ial stages of a  lesson  can  focus  s t uden t s ' at tent ion

on the

  critical aspects

  of the

 object

 o f

 learning, c reate

  the

 con text that w i l l

help

 students

  to

 make

 sense

 of the

 object

 of

 learning,

 and

 open

  up the

 space

for  explorat ion  of an answer .

The

 data that

 we

 look

 a t in

 this chapter co nsists

 of

 data sets

 from

 science

 and

En glish classroom s

 in

 Hong Kong. Prior

 to

 presenting e ach data set,

 w e

 provide

a  brief sum m ary  of the  classroom contexts  and the objects of learning.

Physics

  Lessons:

 The  Reed  Relay

T he data set w e look at in this section con sists of two ph ys ics l essons taug ht

by the same teacher,  one taught  in Engl ish , w hich  is a  second language for

the   students (referred  to as EM I, Engl ish  as a medium of instruction),  and

one in the

 s tudents ' m other tongu e, Ca ntonese ( referred

 to as

 CM I, Chinese

as a m edium of ins t ruct ion) . The di rect object of learning in both lessons is

the sam e: the  function  of a reed relay an d how it operates. (F or a deta i led de-

scription of the background of these two lessons, see Ng, Tsui , & M ar ton ,

2001.)

 A

 brief explanation

 of the

 direct objec t

 of

 learning

 is in

 order here.

T he

 funct ion

 of a

 reed relay

 is to

 enable

 a

 w eak electric current

 in an

 elec-

tric c ircuit to start a m uc h stronger c urre nt in anoth er electric c ircuit by con-

nect ing the tw o w i th a reed sw i tch (see Fig . 5.1 for a picture of a reed relay) .

When   a w eak electric current in one circuit

 passes

 through the reed relay,

a  m agnet ic  field  is produced  and the coil becomes an electromag net, caus-

in g

 the two

 ends

 of the reed sw itch to touch one another. O nce the two ends

of the

 reed swi tch touch

 one

 another,

 th e

 c ircu it that carries

 a

 very large elec-

tric curre nt is closed a nd the electric curre nt w i ll pass throug h and operate

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 128/255

Q U E S T I O N S A N D T H E

  S P A C E

  O F  L E A R N I N G

1 1 5

FIG.

  5 . 1 .

  A

  reed relay.

the device tha t is connected to it (see Fig. 5.2b and Fig. 5.3b). In the ex am ple

l e s s o n s ,  th e  t e a c h e r  u s e d  a  d e v i c e c a l l e d  a  l i g h t - e m i t t i n g d i o d e

(LED)—which requires only  a weak cur rent to operate (see Fig. 5.2a)— and

a motor— w hich requires  a very large current  to operate (see Fig. 5.3b)—to

i l lustrate

 how the use of a

 reed relay enables

 a

 weak cur rent

 to

 activate

 a

 very

large current ,

 an d

 cause

  the

 motor

  to

 rota te .

T he s tructures of the two lessons are a lmost iden tical . In both lessons, the

teacher

  first

  explained the structure of a reed relay by showing that i t con-

sists o f a reed sw itch w ith coi ls wrapped round  it (see Fig. 5.1) . S he then ex-

plained

  the

 con f igura t ion

 o f a

  simple circuit connected

  to an LED

 (see Fig.

5.2a),

  and the

  conf igurat ion

  of a

  complicated circuit also connected

  to an

L E D   (see Fig. 5.2b).

Th e teacher asked the s tudents to condu c t tw o exper imen ts . In the

  f i rst

expe r imen t , s tudents w ere asked

  to

 connec t

  tw o

 c i rcui t s .

 O ne

 c i r cu i t

 w as

cont ro l led by a push bu t ton sw i tch  an d conn ec ted  to an L E D , and w as re-

F IG .

 5.2.  (a) A  s imple  ci rcui t ,  (b) A  compl ica ted c i rcu i t  w i t h  a reed relay.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 129/255

1 1 6

T S U I

  E T A L .

F I G .  5.3.  (a) A  s i m p l e c i rcu i t ,  (b) A c o m p l i c a te d

 c i r c u i t

  w i t h  reed

  relay .

ferred

  to as the

  s imple circuit  (see Fig. 5.2a).

 T he

  o ther c i rcui t

 w as

  a lso

control led  by a p u s h b u t t o n , and had a reed re lay con nec ted  to it as w e l l as

an   L E D .

 This

 c ircui t  w as referred  to as the

  com plicated circuit

  (see Fig.

5 .2b) . Th e teacher asked the s tudents to press the pu sh but ton sw i tch for

both c i rcui t s . In both c i rcui t s , the LED glowed when the c i rcui t s were

closed  by pressing  the push bu t ton swi tch , w h ich a l lowed an e lectric cu r-

rent to  pass  thro ug h. In the second ex per im ent , the teach er asked the s tu-

den t s to r ep lace the LE D w i th a mo to r, and the push bu t ton sw i tch w i th an

L D R

  ( l ight-d iode res is tor )

2

  fo r

 both

  th e

  s i m p l e

  and the

  c o m p l i c a t e d c i r-

cu i t s  (see Fi g. 5.3a and F ig. 5.3b) . An L DR i s a l ig ht sens i t ive device t ha t

stops  an

  electric

  cur rent f rom pass ing through. When l ight shines on an

L D R ,  its  resis tance drops  an d  e lec t r ic i ty  can  pa ss th rough .  T h e  t e a c h e r

asked the s tuden ts to sh ine a torch on the LD R and w atch wh at hap pen ed.

T he  students  found tha t  th e motor  in the  s imple c i rcui t (wi thout  the

 reed

r e l ay )

 d id no t

 move , whereas

  th e

 motor

  in the

 c o m p l i c a te d c i r cu i t ( w i t h

 th e

reed re lay) did. Th i s i s beca use the e lec tr ic cur rent tha t passed thro ug h the

s imple c i rcu i t w as not b ig eno ugh to caus e the mo tor to ro ta te . By cont ras t ,

in the

  c o m p l ic a t e d c i r c u i t ,

 th e

  cu r r en t became m uc h la rger

  a f te r

  p a s s i n g

throu gh the reed relay , and hence a l low ed the m otor to ro ta te .

After

  the s tudents had per formed the two ex per iments , the teacher w ent

over w hat they had experienced w ith the wh ole c lass . She asked them to

 tell

her w h a t happened  to the LED  when they

 pressed

  the  push button switch

and they repor ted that the L ED in both c ircu i ts glo w ed. On h ear ing the s tu-

dents ' reports, the teacher posed the   fo l lowing q u e s t i o n :

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 130/255

5.

  Q U E S T I O N S

  A N D T H E

  S P A C E

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  1 1 7

5.1   [Physics Lesson/CMI]

3

T :  It

 seems tha t

 if

 this

 is the

 case,

 w e do no t

 need

 to use the

 reed

 re-

lay.

 W hy use the com plicated ci rcui t wh en the s im ple ci rcui t can

do

 the

 job?

 Do you

  agree?

T he

  teacher problematized

  the

  students' observation

  by

 pu t ting

 a

 hypo-

thet ical s tatem ent to them : "If this is the case, w e do not need to use the reed

relay." Here , "this

 is the case"

 refers

 to the fact

  that

 the LE D

 glow ed

 in

 both

circui ts ,

 sug gest ing that there w as no  difference  between the two ci rcui ts .

However ,

 the

 teacher signaled that this hypothesis

  m ay or m ay not be

 con-

firmed,  by

  prefacing

  the

  statement with

  "it

  seems

 that." This

 prepared

  the

ground

 fo r the

 fo l low ing quest ion: "W hy

  use the

 com plicated ci rcui t w hen

the

 sim ple circuit can do the

 job?"

 Th e presuppo sition of this que stion is tha t

the

 sim ple circuit

 can do the job in the

 same

 w ay as the

 com plicated  circuit .

W hat is quest ioned is the reason for us ing the co m plicated ci rcui t . The q ues-

t ioned  element focused

 the

  students ' at tention

  on the

 com pl ica ted  circuit ,

and the

 reason

  fo r

 us ing

 it . Th e

 jux tapo s i tion

 of the fact

  that

 the

  s imple

 and

com plicated ci rcui ts could both l ight

 up the L ED

 created

 the

 need

 for an ex-

planat ion (see Ogborn, K ress , M art ins , & M cGil l icuddy, 1996) and opened

up the

 space

 for

 exp loring

 the

 function

  o f a

 reed relay.

 T he

 qu est ion created

th e

  context

  for

 making sense

  of the

  ou tcome

 of the

  second experiment .

 T o

put i t another way, the quest ion created the need to look for the answer in the

second experiment .

As already described, in the second experiment, the teacher asked the

s tudents to replace the push but ton swi tch w i th an LDR and the LE D w ith a

m otor in both ci rcui ts . She asked them to shine a torch on the LDR and see

whe the r

  the

 motor moved

  in

 each

 of the

 circuits (see Fig.

 3a and

 F ig. 3b).

After

 the

 experiment ,

 the

 teacher w ent over wh at

 the

 students

 h ad

 experi-

enced.

  S he

 asked

  the

 s tudents w hat

 had

 happened

 to the

 m otor

  in the

 c o m-

plicated  circuit . The students reported that the motor rotated. She  then

cont inued

  as

  follows:

5.2

  [Physics Lesson

  /

  CM I ]

T :

  Yes ,

 h o w

  about th i s s ide? [The teacher po in t s

  at the

  s i mp l e

c i rcu i t ] This s ide  is  even s impler . Now, here  w e g o .  S t r o n g

l i g h t sh i n e s

 on the LD R, e l ec t ri c r e s i s t anc e va lue dec rea ses .

W e expec t t ha t there's an  e lec t r i c cur ren t ,  a  s t ronger e lec t r i c

cu r ren t , pas s in g th ro ug h the c i r cu i t and t he m o to r shou ld ro -

ta te , r igh t?

  But i t i s

  ve ry unhappy .

  I t does  n o t  r e s p o n d . W e

n e e d

  to  exp l a in t h i s .

This tim e, the teach er drew the studen ts ' at tention to the sim ple c ircuit by

asking , "How about this

 side?"

 S he problem at ized  the experience by po in t -

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 131/255

1 1 8  TSUI  E T A L .

ing  ou t that the  failure o f the m otor to respond, even tho ug h a stronger elec-

tric current

 passed

 through

  the

 circuit,

 w as

 contrary

  to

 w hat they expected.

Again she created a need for an explanation.

Let us

 pause here

 and

 look

 at why the

 teacher

 did not

 focus

 on the

 reason

for

 the

 m otor rotat ing

 in the

 com plicated ci rcui t ,

 bu t

 ins tead focused

 the

 stu-

dents'

 attention

 on the reason fo r the m otor  in the sim ple circuit no t  rotating,

even tho ugh the electric resistance value decreased and the electric curre nt

became stronger.

  W hy did the

  teacher

  do

 that?

In

  the first experiment, the LED worked in both circuits . If the teacher

had drawn the students ' at tention to the complicated circuit , the students

w ould have not been able to discern the

 differenc e

  between the two ci rcu i ts

because

  in the

  co m plicated ci rcu i t both devices ( the

  LED and the

  m o to r)

worked. B y jux tapos ing a c i rcu i t in w h i ch the motor worked (a com pl ica ted

circui t)

 wi th another ci rcui t

 in

 w h ic h

 it did no t

 work

  (a

 s im ple ci rcui t ) ,

 the

teacher brought into the students '

  focal

 aw areness different respon ses of the

same device, and opened  up the space  for exp loring the answer in the  differ-

ent  configurations  of the two  circuits.

Sub sequently, the t eacher put an a mm e te r  in each c i rcui t  to m easu re  the

strength of its electric current. T he readings showed that the electric curre nt

in the

 s imple c i rcui t

 w as

 m uch sm al ler than tha t

 in the

 com pl ica ted c i rcu i t .

T he teacher then guided  the  students through each circuit and  helped them

to  formula te the reason  w hy the m otor  did not  rotate  in the  s imple c i rcui t ;

that is,

  that a l thoug h

  the

  elect r ic current became s t ronger when

  the

  resis-

tance value decreased,

 i t w as not

 s trong enoug h

 to

 cause

 the

 m o to r

 to

 rotate.

The teacher did not s top here, how ever. She then w en t back to the s imp le

circui t

 w i th

  the LE D in

 Fig .

 2a and

  posed

  the  fo l lowing

 ques t ion :

5.3 [Physics Lesson/CMI/T8]

T: W hy is i t

  that this circuit [ the simple

  c i rcui t

  connected

  to an

LED ] worked?

  Why was i t

 [LED]

 so

 w el l -behaved?

 W hy did i t

[LED] l igh t

  up?

B y

 asking these quest ions ,

 th e

 t eacher

 w as

 j ux t apos ing

 the

 s im ple c i rcui t

connected to an LED w ith the s imple ci rcu i t conne cted to a m otor. In other

words ,

  she now

 held

  the

 ci rcu i t constant,

  and

 varied

  the

 device,

 the

 motor ,

and the

  LED.

  T he

  questions

  she

  asked brought into

  the

  students '

  focal

awareness the

  different

  responses of the two devices, and opened up the

space for exploring the reasons. A correct unde rstanding  of the reasons w as

achieved w hen  o ne student explained that th e c i rcu i t w i th the LE D l i t up be-

cause the electr ic current needed by the L ED w as sm al ler than the curren t

needed

 by the

 m otor. This

 is the

 corollary

 of w hy the

 m o to r

 did not

 rotate .

Let us  recapi tulate  the wa y in w h ic h the  teacher structured  the l earn ing

experience.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 132/255

5.  Q U E S T I O N S  A N D T H E  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 1 9

First of all, in the

  first

  experiment ,  the teacher varied the con f igura tions

of the tw o circuits so that there wa s on e w ithout a reed relay (i .e . , a sim ple

circuit [Cl]),

 and one

  with

 a

  reed relay (i.e.,

 a

  complicated circuit [C2]),

w hereas the device that the s tudents operated was invariant ( the LE D [D l]) .

In both cases, the LED li t up.

In the

  second experiment,

  the

  teacher changed

 the

 device

 from the LED

( D 1 ) in the first exper im ent to the motor (D2) for both c i rcui ts . This t im e, it

w as also the device that w as invariant (the m otor [D2]), and the circuit that

varied (the

 one

 w ith

 a reed

 relay

 [C1 ] and the one

 without

 a reed

 relay [C2]).

W hat the

 teacher

 did is

  represented diagrammatical ly

 in

 F ig. 5.4a .

How ever, in the second experiment , the variation tha t th e teacher wanted

the

  students

 to experience was not the

  variation between

 the two

  circuits,

but

 the varia t ion between w hat they experienced  in the previous

 conf igura -

tion  (the first experiment) an d the present

 configurat ion

  ( the second experi-

ment) , which

 was due to the use of

  different  devices:

  Dl in the first

exper iment

 and D 2 in the

 second experiment .

 T he

 teac her ' s exho rta t ion

 for

an  ex planat ion in the  second ex cerpt (5.2) and the  question in the  third e x-

cerpt (5.3) foc use d precisely

  on the different responses of the LE D (D 1) and

the motor (D2) . In other words,  the  teacher was now  construct ing the s tu-

den ts ' total experience  (of the two experiments) as a wh ole. S he held the cir-

cui t conf igura t ions cons tant

  (in

  both exper iments ,

  a

  s imple

  and a

compl ica ted circuit w ere involved), and w hat varied w ere th e devices ( in the

first experim ent an LED was involved, w herea s in the second experim ent a

motor

 was

 involved).

 The

 structure

 of the

 potential learning experience

 can

be represented by Fig. 5.4b, where the bolded parts represent the aspects

that

 w ere held constant

 in the first and

  second experim ents ,

 and the

 unde r -

lined p arts represent

  the

  aspects that varied.

This structure of  variation brought the

 responses

 of the  devices into the

students '

 focal

 aw areness

 in

 relation

 to tw o

 contexts:

 why the

 m otor rota ted

in the co m plicated circuit but did not in the simp le circuit , and w hy the LE D

worked in the  simple circuit but the m otor did not. If, in the  second experi-

men t ,

  the

  teacher

 had

 replaced

  the LED

 w i th

 a

  motor only

 in the

 com pl i-

First  E xpe r ime n t  Cl -^ Dl C2 -»• Dl

Second  E xpe r ime n t  C l  -*->  D2 C2

  -*•

 D2

C l =  s im p le c i r cu i t  D l = LE D  ( l ight-emit t ing d iode)

C 2 =  com pl i ca t ed c i rcui t  D2 = m o t o r

- * •  caused

 device

  to opera te

-*r>

  fai led  to

 ca use device

 to

 opera te

F IG .

  5 .4a . S t ruc tur ing learning.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 133/255

120

  T S U I

  ET AL.

First  E x p e r i m e n t  Second  Expe r imen t

Cl •* D_L

  C l -x->

 D2

C2

 •* Dl C2 •* D2

FIG. 5.4b. Stru cturin g of  learning; first experim ent versus second experim ent.

cated circuit (as shown in Fig. 5.4c) both devices would have worked and

the

 s tudents ' a t tent ion w ould

 n o t

 have been

  so

 sharply focu sed

 o n

 w h a t

 it is

that

 a

 ci rcu i t w i th

 the

  reed relay

 can do

  tha t

 one

 w i thou t canno t .

C onversely, if the teache r ha d started  off by us ing the m otor as the device

for

  both ci rcui ts

  in the

  first

  experiment , then

  all the

  s tudents w ould have

seen w as that the

 m o to r

 w orked only in the c i rcu i t w i th the reed relay. They

wou ld n ot have been able to see tha t the electric curren t in the s im ple c i rcu i t

w as a weak current w hich could operate an L ED , but not s trong enoug h to

operate the motor. Consequently, the main characterist ic of the reed relay

( that

  it uses  a  smal l current  to  swi tch on a  s t rong current) would n ot have

been   so

  effectively  h igh l igh ted.

Wh a t

  is

  even more interest ing

  is the w ay the

  t eacher m ade

  the

 s tudents

see the interrelat ionsh ip betwee n the first exp erim ent and the second experi-

ment : that

  the LED

  needs only

 a

 smal l current

 to

 operate (w hich

  is w hy i t

worked

  in the

  s imple ci rcui t ) , whereas

  the

  motor needs

  a

  large current

(which  is why it did not w ork). In order to ma k e the mo tor wo rk , a reed relay

is

 needed becau se

  it can u se a

 smal l current

 to

 start

 a

 very large curre nt .

 The

teacher ' s e xplanat ion can be represented on  fo l low ing pa ge (see Fig. 5.4d).

T h e w a y   tha t  the  teacher s t ructured  th e  learning experience  by  posing

questions

  a t

 cri t ical points

 in the

 lesson brou ght in to s tuden ts '

  focal

  aware-

ness cri t ical aspects

 of the

 exper im ent

  and

 opened

  up the

 space

  fo r

 explor-

ing the

  answers

  to the

  quest ions .

 This  w as

  cruc ia l

  in

  b r ing ing about

  the

s imu l taneo us aw areness of the three phenom ena ( i .e ., the sm al l current re-

quired

 to operate the

 LED,

 the

 very large cu rrent needed

 to

 operate

 t he mo-

tor, as we l l as the w ay in w h ich t hey are re la ted) , w hich w as necessary  for

the

  students

  to

 unders tand

  the  funct ion  of the

 reed relay.

S o

  far,

 w e

  have i l lus t rated

 h ow

  quest ions

  can be a

 pow er fu l m eans

  for

bring ing cri t ical aspects

 o f the

 ob jec t

 o f

 l earn ing in to s tudents ' focal  aware-

ness,

  and

 opening

 up the

  space

  for  further

  enqui ry .

 W e

 have also seen

 how

Fi rs t  E x p e r i m e n t S e c on d E x p e r i m e n t

C 1 - D 1 C 1 - * D 1

C2-"D1  C 2 - D 1

F I G .

 5.4c.

  A l t e rn a t iv e s t ru c tu r i n g  o f

 l e a r n i n g .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 134/255

5.  Q U E S T I O N S  A N D T H E  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 21

Cl <C2

Dl

  <D2

C l

  - * D 1

C l  -x->D2

C2-*D2

F I G .  5 .4d . Teacher ' s exp lana t io n.

the teacher skillfully  structured the learning experience by posing que stions

at cri tica l junc tures

  in the

  lesson.

In  the

 follow ing section,

 w e see how

 different questions asked

 by the

 teacher

can bring different  aspects of a phenom enon into students'  focal  awareness.

E ng l i s h  Lessons:

  O ld M a c D o n a l d ' s F ar m

The

  data that

  is

  discussed

  in

  this section consist

  of two

  primary Grade

  1

En gl ish lessons (Class ID and Class IE) tau gh t by two teachers w ho used

very similar materials  and  whose lesson object ives were  the  same.  T he

topic o f the  lessons w as  "Old MacDonald 's Farm," a n Engl ish song about

farm  an imals that

 is

 fam iliar

 to

 ch ildren

 o f

 many

 different

 na t ional i t ies .

 O ne

of the ob jectives of the lesson w as to teach the determ iner "some" to  signify

inexact

  quantity.

 In the

  song,

  the

 word

 "some"

  appears many t imes

 in the

lyrics, and for this reason,  the teachers used the song as a vehicle for teach-

ing this determ iner

 to the ch ildren.

 B oth teachers used com puter-generated

im age s to revise

 farm

 anim al vocabulary and su bsequ ent ly to teach

 "some."

(For a deta i led description of the backgroun d of these two lessons, see M ok,

R unesso n, Tsui , W ong, Chik, & Pow, 2002.)

Teaching  "Some."

  B oth teachers began

  by

  revising

  th e

  vocabulary

  for

farm   anim als by asking the quest ion, "W hat can you

 see?"

 In each case, the

pic ture

 of a single anim al was presented on the com puter, and the respo nses

solicited

 w ere "I can see a [name of animal]." The

  focus

  at this stage o f the

lesson w as the nam es of the anim als , ra ther than the num ber. The imp ortant

difference  between the two lessons l ies in the subsequent episodes where

both teachers dealt with  the teaching of "some."

After

  the

  vocabulary revis ion,

  the

  teacher

  in ID

  asked

  the

  students

  to

guess the number of

 different

  kinds o f animals o n a farm.  She

 fol lowed

 this

by   showing  the  students  different  kinds  of  farm  animals  and  asking  how

ma n y

  animals there were in each pic ture . The fol low ing is an excerpt of

w h a t

 took place in the lesson im m ediately

 after

  the vocabu lary revision. To

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 135/255

122  T S U I  ET AL.

make

 it

 easier

 for the

 reader

 to

 fol low

 the subsequent

 discussion

 of the

 data,

the

 parts

 that will be

 discussed

 are underlined.

5.4 [English

 Lesson/Old MacDonald's

 Farm/Primary Grade

  I / I D ]

4

11 9 T: . .. Do you  know how many cows  o r hens? O r how m any ho r ses ,

120 how many hens , how m any p igs a re ( there) in a

  fa rm?

  Do you

1 21   know?  D o you k n o w ?

122  Ss: No.

123 T: OK,

  let's

  see

 wh a t 's

  in the

  farm.

  A ll

 r ight ,

 O K ,

 w h i ch

 one do you

1 24  w a n t to see  first?

125

  Ss:

  Cow.

126 T : OK. you w a n t to see cows. Righ t . Guess how many cows a re there

127 in the

 f a rm ?

128 S :

  [ shout ing

 from his

  seat] Four.

129 T: How  many cows?

130 S:

  [shout ing

 from his

 seat] Four.

131 T:  Yes, Leo.

132 S:

  Four .

133 T:

  W h a t

 do you

  think, Louise?

134 S:  Three.

135 T:  W h a t do you  think, H o  Seng?

136 S:

  Six.

137 T:

  W h a t

 do you

  think Elaine?

138 S:  F ive .

139 T: OK , Elaine. Do you wa nt to  find  out? OK, come here. Let 's  find

140 out how

 many.

 OK,

  cows. [Speaking

  to

 Elaine] Press

 the

 button

141 please. [Elaine com es up to the f ront and cl icks the m ouse. A n

142

  image

 of a cow appears on the  screen.] All right, OK. Le t 's see

143 how

 many cows

  in the

  fa rm.

  W h at

  can you see

 here? [Teacher

1 44

  points

 at the

  screen. ] What

  can you see

  here?

145 Ss: I can see a cow.

146 T :

  Good.

  N ow

 let's

 f ind out how

 m any cow s there are? [Teacher

1 47  clicks  the m ouse  and  four  more cows appear on the screen. ] H ow

1 48  many cows  are there?

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 136/255

5.  Q U ES TI O N S

  A N D TH E  SPACE  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 23

149 Ss:  Five.

150 T:  Four?

151

  Ss:

  Five.

152 T: 10?

153  Five.

154 T: . I can s e e . . .  ?

155 Ss: ... five  cows .

156 T:  Righ t . Or you may say ... ?

1 57 Ss: I can see f ive

  cows .

158 Ss: I can see a

 cow.

159 T:

  W h a t ?

16 0 Ss : I can see . ..

161

  T:  Some

162 Ss:

  Some cows .

163 T :

  Some  cows. Yes. There

  are

 some cow s here .

  Is it

  only one?

164

  [Point ing

 at the

  screen.]

165 Ss: No.

166 T:  There are four

  cows.

 You may

  say,

 I can see some

 cows.

 OK,

167

  wh ole c lass.

168 Ss: I can see some cows.

169 T : A l l r ight , le t ' s con t in ue . . . . [Teacher c l icks the m ouse several times

170 and six

 different

  types

  of

  animals appear

  on the

  screen]

  OK, do

17 1 you  w a n t  to f ind out how   many [point ing  to the pic ture  o f a

1 72  duck] ducks there [are]  in the  fa rm?  ...

In

 this excerpt , the teache r started w ith the qu estion , "Do y ou know how

m any cows

 or

 hens?

 O r how

 m any horses,

  how

 m any hens,

 how

 many p igs

are [ there] in a farm?" The quest ioned elem ent i s the numbe r o f anim als ,

and it w as  repeated  four  t im es . A s w e have pointed  out previously , it is the

ques t ioned

  e lement tha t

  focuses

  the

  students ' at tention.

  T he

  focus

  on the

n u m b e r o f

 an imals

 w as

  reinforced when

  the

  teacher asked

  the

  s tudents

 to

guess "how m any" cows there were on the farm (see l ines 126-127). A s w e

can

  see

 from

  the stude nts ' responses, they did  indeed  focus o n the num ber,

and

  proffered

  "four,"

 "three," "six,"

  and "five" as answers.  Af ter  th is ,  the

teacher asked

 the

 students

 to f ind out the

 answer

  for

 them selves

 b y

 invit ing

one of them to c o me up to the com pu te r at the front of the class . The s tudent

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 137/255

1 24

  TS U I

  E T A L .

cl icked  the  m o u s e  and one cow   appeared.  T he  teacher asked  the  s tudent

w h a t

  she

 cou ld see,

 and the

  student said that

 she

 cou ld

 see a

 cow. This

  w as

fol lowed

  by the

  teacher showing  four  more cows

  on the

  screen

  and

  once

again posing

  the

 ques t ion , "How m any cows

 are

 there?"

 B y

 doing th is ,

 the

teacher

  w as

 con trast ing "one" w ith "more  than one." M oreover,

  by

  repeat-

edly asking  the s tudents "how m any cows" there were , the teacher w as con-

trasting "one" with

 "other

 num bers," that is , w i th a va lue o f more than one ,

hence opening up a dime nsion of var ia tion in num ber . In other words, w ha t

w as brought in to the s tudents '  focal  awareness was exac t num bers , w hereas

the object ive of the lesson w as to teach

 "some,"

 w hich i s an inexac t num ber

rather than an exact number .

T he

  focus

  on exac t numb er w as re inforced by the teacher sub sequ ent ly

get t ing four s tudents to guess the exact nu m ber of cows ( l ines 1 3 1 , 1 3 3 ,1 3 5 ,

and 137) . W hen the cow s were shown on the screen, the teacher

 offered

 d i f -

ferent  n u m b e r s

  to

  tease

  th e

  s tudents

  an d

  de l ibera te ly suspended

  th e

evaluative   feedback

 on the

 exac t num ber tha t

 the

 s tude nts provided.

 I t was

no t unt i l  l ine 156 that the teacher t r ied to e l ic i t the determ iner "some" from

the students

 b y

 us ing

 th e b lank -f i l ling

 ques t ion :

 "Or you may say . .. ?" Th e

students '  responses  to this  b lank-f i l l ing  ques t ion were  te l l ing.  Some s tu-

dents sa id, "There are five cows," an d some said, "I can see a cow." Bo th a n-

swers provided exac t num bers , w hich sugges ts tha t exac t nu m ber

 w as

  stil l

very m uch in their focal aw areness. Th e determiner "some" was supp l ied by

the teacher and the s tudents were m ade  to repeat  it after her. Bu t i t is doub t -

fu l

  if the

  s tudents

  ful ly

  unders tood what

 "some"

 m ean t . W hen

  the

  teacher

m oved on to the next episode ( l ine 170), w here she showed the pic ture of a

duck, she aga in asked the s tudents whether they w anted to f ind how m any

ducks there were on the farm.

The focus on exact num ber w as a lso re inforced by the way in w hic h the

teacher introduced

  the

 determ iner

 "some."

 I n

 both cases,

  the

 word

 "some"

w as

 in t roduced

 a t the

 very

 end o f the

 episode,

  by

 presenting

 it as an

 a l te rna-

tive

 s ta tem ent

  to an

 exac t num ber,

  fo r

 examp le, "There

 are

  four  cows .

 Y ou

m ay say, I can see som e cow s," "There are seven d uc ks , or you m ay tell m e, I

can see

  some ducks."

  In

 both  cases

 the

  exact number preceded

  the

 inexac t

number s igni f ied b y

 "some."

Le t us com pare the ju st c i ted discourse w ith the fo l low ing excerpt that

shows w hat took place in the lesson of Class IE   after  the teacher had fin-

ished vocabulary revision.

5 .5 [Engl ish Lesson/Old M cDonald 's Farm/Pr imary Grade I / I E ]

70 T:  [Teacher cl ick s the m ouse ]  A  dog. [Teacher cl icks the  m ouse

71

  aga in ] ano ther

 dog

  [Teacher cl icks

 the

 m ouse ag a in ] ano ther dog .

72  W o w , w h a t can you  see? Al ice?

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 138/255

5.  Q U E S T I O N S  A N D T H E  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 25

73 S: I can see   three dogs.

74 T:  V ery goo d, three dogs. There  are  three dogs. Thank yo u. And you

75 can say that, in other w ords, w e can say that . ..

76 Ss: We can see

  dogs.

77 T:  Remember  the word "some"?

78 Ss:

  Yes .

79 T:

  Thank you , very w el l .

 I can see

  some dogs.

 Do you

 remember

  th e

80  word "some"?

81 Ss :

  Yes.

82 T: Yes, a l l r ight . Now , another one. [Teacher cl icks the m ouse] A no ther

83  p ig . OK. Now, so we have a pig, w e' l l  see OK. C o m e and press the

84

  but ton.

  [A

  student comes out.]

85 T: Is he

  finished?

  Is there another pig there? Press the but ton. W ow,

86

  ano ther p ig? W ha t

 can you see

  then?

87 S: I can see  some pigs.

88 T:

  V ery good

To

 begin w ith, the teacher in IE showed the picture of a single an im al, a

dog , ju s t

 as the

 teacher

 in 1D had

 done. However, before

 she

 showed

 the

 p ic-

ture,

 the

  teacher asked,

 "What can you see?"

 rather than "How m any dogs

can

  you

  see?"

  T he  quest ioned element  is

 "what"

  and not  "how many."

Asking "What

 can you

 see?"

 did not

  focus

  the

 stude nts ' at tention

 on the ex-

act

 num ber.  I t w as open  to the students  to give ex act or inexact num bers  as

answers .

 W h e n

 the

 teache r asked

 the

 students

 to

 offer

  an

 al ternative

 fo r

 see-

ing

 three dog s, the studen ts said, "W e can see dogs." In other w ords, conc ep-

tually,  the  students were aware that  one  alternative  was to  leave  out

speci fying

  the

 exact number ,

 and to

 s imply

 use the

 p lura l  form

  to

 indicate

that there

 w as

 more than

 o ne

 dog.

 A t

 this point ,

 the

 teache r provided

 the

 l in-

guistic

 support

  to the

 students

 by

 reminding them that

 the

 word

 "some" had

been in troduced before .

  In the

  subsequent episodes ,

  the

  teacher showed

three

 different

  kinds o f farm  anim als : p igs , duc ks and cats . In each episode,

she

 varied

 o ne

 w i th

 more

 than one,

 and

 posed

 th e

 same question, "What

  can

you  see?"

  T he

  students

 had no

 problems responding wi th ,

 "I can see

 some

p igs

  [some ducks , some cats]."  In other words , the quest ioned element ,

"what," opened  up a dimension  of variation  in  inexact number, that is, on e

versus

  some.

Af ter

  successfu l ly  soliciting the determiner

  "some"

  from  the students,

the teach er w ent on to pose the quest ion, "How m any [anim als] are there?"

in  the

  third episode.

 L e t us

  consider

  the  fo l lowing

  excerpt:

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 139/255

126  T S U I  ET AL.

5.6 [English Lesson/Old MacDonald 's  Farm/Primary Grade I / IE ]

103   S: I can see  some cats .

1 04

  T :

  V ery good.

  O K.

  W a it

 a

 m inu te .

  How

  many ca ts

 are

  there?

1 05  How m any ca ts a re there?

106 S: Four cats .

107  T: Is he  correct?

108 S:  Yes.

1 09

  T :

  V ery good.

 Thank

 you .

 OK, how

 about this picture? A lright.

1 1 0  [Teacher clicks  the m ouse.]  H o  Ming, would  you come  ou t

111

  and

 press

 i t for me to see

 what p ic ture comes o ut?

 [Ho

 M in g

1 1 2  c l icks the m ouse .]

11 3

  T :

  W h a t

  is it?

1 1 4  Ss : Ducks , ducks .

115  T: V ery good. Thanks. [Teacher asks Ho M ing to c l ick the

1 1 6

  m ouse . ] A ny more ducks? [S tudent c l icks the m ouse and

11 7  some duck s appear.]

118

  T : Any

 more?

  [Student clicks

  th e

  mouse again

  and

  some more

1 1 9  duck s appear .] A ny m ore? [Student c l ic ks the m ouse

1 20  aga in . ] No more . OK , w hat can you see now ? Hold the m ike

1 21

  and

  te l l yo ur c lassm ates,

  O K?

122

  S: I can see some duc k.

123

  T:

  D u c k s .

124 S:

  Ducks .

125  T :  Ducks .

126   S:  Ducks.

127   T: OK. V ery good. How m any ducks a re there? How m any

1 28

  duck s are there?

1 29   S: [same student] Five duck s.

1 30  T: Five duck s. Thank you very m uc h. Five duck s. Yes. you can

1 31

  see some duck s, you can see some pigs, you can see some

132   cats,

  and you can see

 some

  ...

133

  Ss:  Dogs.

1 34  T :  Dogs. O K . . . .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 140/255

5.

  Q U E S T I O N S

  A N D T H E

  S P A C E

  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 27

Let us consider  the  sequence  o f quest ioning

  from

  the p erspect ive o f the

logical relationship between  inexact

  and

  exact numbers.

 The

 determiner

"some" signif ies  a range of num bers or qua nt i t ies and is inclus ive of exact

num bers such as 3 ,4 ,5 ,1 0 , and so on . The teacher in Class IE

 first

 posed the

ques t ion ,

 "What

  can you see?"

 fo l low ed

  by

 "How m any?" This

  is a logical

sequence—when reques t ing in format ion ,

 one

  often  goes

  from

  general

  to

speci f ic—and the exact num ber is seen in relat ion to the inexact num ber. B y

contrast ,

  the teacher in Class ID s tar ted wi th , "How many cows can you

see?" which is already very sp ecific, and therefore renders the use of the de-

te rminer

 "some"

 to describe an inexact num ber su perf luou s . Thus the pro-

duct ion

  o f

 "some"

 becom es contrived.

W hat both teache rs tr ied to teach w as how to express inexact nu m bers

that

  are

  more than one. However,

  the

  different  quest ions that

  the

  teachers

asked,

  and the way

 they sequen ced

  the

  questions, focused

 the

  students '

  at-

tent ion  o n

 different w a y s

 of

 express ing nu m bers that

 ar e

 m ore than

 o ne

 ( that

is , exact num ber  and inexact num ber),  and opened  up different dim ensions

of variat ion. The enacted object of learning in one case w as an inexact nu m -

ber, wherea s

 in the

 other case,

 i t w as an

 exact number , which

 w as not an in-

tended  ob jec t of learning.

In post lesson interviews con ducted wi th hal f

 of the

  s tudents

  from

  each

class ,

 a task w as given to solici t the use of "some" to describe a selection of

pic tures . The results showed th at students in Class  IE perform ed bet ter than

students  in

 Class

  ID .

QUESTION

  TYPES,

  QUESTION  S EQU EN C ES ,

A N D T H E

 SPACE

  O F

  L EARNI NG

Quest ion   Types

Many studies have focused on the cognit ive aspect of questions, and vari-

ous

 t axonom ies

 o f

 question types ha ve been proposed,

  for

 example ,

 the

 sys-

tem

  of

  classifying questions according

  to

 whether  they require

 higher or

lower order thinking skil ls . Our concern in this chapter is not to examine

que stion types per se. W e are interested in those qu estions tha t have been re-

ferred

 to as

 "open"

 and

 "closed"

 que stions, and the oppo rtunities for learn-

ing that are   afforded  by these qu estions. In part icular, we are interested in

the w ays in wh ich teachers m odify and sequen ce their qu est ions in order to

el ici t

 responses  from s tudents ,

 and

 hence

 how

 teach ers shape their s tud en ts '

learning experiences.

In classroom research l i terature, dist inctions have been made between

closed

 a nd

 open qu estions. However, there

 a re different

  criteria

  for

 identify-

ing

  these q uestion s. In education research l i terature, a

  c losed ques t ion

  has

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 141/255

128  T S U I  ET AL.

been un ders tood as having only one r ight answer, whe reas an  open ques t ion

is one

  that

  has a

 n u mb e r

  of right

  answ ers.

  In

  th i s vo lum e,

  we use

  c losed

ques t ions

 to m ean qu estions w here there is only one acceptable answer, and

open

  ques t ions  to mean quest ions where there is a range of poss ib le an-

swers,

  or a

 range

  of

 poss ib le w ays

 of

 present ing

  the

 answer.

  For

 exam ple,

"Why does this motor work?"   is an  open ques t ion , w hereas "W hat  is the

nam e given to th is m agnet?" is a closed q uest ion . W hen the teache r asks an

open question,

  the

  space

  of

 learning

  is

 w idened because

 the

 question chal-

lenges the s tudents to consider a num ber of

 poss ib i l i ties ,

 and to formulate a n

answer that makes sense no t only to them selves but also to the res t of the

class.

 T he

  formulat ion

 of an

 answer

  is a

 process

 in

 w h ich

 the

 students clar-

ify

  thei r th inking

 a nd

 the i r unders tanding

 of the

  ob jec t

 o f

  learning.

 On the

other hand, closed que st ions narrow down the poss ib le an swers to only one

choice,

  or a

  l imi ted number

  of

  choices,

  and

  hence al low l i t t le room

  for

learners  to explore answ ers .

In classrooms,  it is  very common  for  teachers  to decompose  a complex

open question into

 a

 series

 o f

 simpler questions

 in

 order

 to

 help students

 to ar-

rive

  at

  an

  appropriate answer. This

  is

 referred

 to as

  "piloting"

  by

  Lundgren

(1977). Studies

 of

 classroom questions have identified different  sequences

 of

questions u sed by teache rs, and ana lyzed them as to whe ther they were suc-

cessful

  or not in

 term s

 of

 helping students

 to

 answer

 the

 que st ion.

 For

 exam-

ple, among   th e sequences of  questions that Brown  and  Edmondson (1984)

identified w ere these

 tw o

 sequences , w hich they referred

 to as

 " funne lm g"

 (p .

114): an open que stion follow ed by spec ific  quest ions; and an open quest ion

followed by a narrowing down of this que stion to recal l facts  and  s imple de -

ductions.

  "Funneling"  is mos t com m only  found  w h e n the  teacher has  failed

to elicit a response from students w ith the ini t ial open que stion.

In c la s s room s w here a l angu age o the r than th e m o t h e r t o n g u e is used a s

a

 m e d iu m

 o f

  teach ing

 and

  l earn ing (e .g . ,

 in

 E n g l i sh

 as a

 s ec o n d l a n g u a g e

classrooms),  we typical ly  find  modi f ica t ion of wh-ques t ions ( i . e . ,  from

w hy   ques t ions

  to

  what  ques t i ons ) ,

 and

  c h a n g e s

  in

 ques t i on

  fo rmat

  (e .g . ,

f rom   wh-ques t i ons  to yes-no  ques t i ons ) . Th i s  is  p r imar i l y because  re -

sponding  to  why-ques t ions  is  l inguis t i ca l ly more demanding than  re-

sponding

  to

  w h a t -qu e s t io n s ( w h i c h  of ten  r equ i re answers cons i s t i ng

 o f

only  on e or two w ords  o r a phrase) .  A nd responding  to yes-no ques t i ons

(or a l t e rna t ive que s t ions ) i s leas t dem and ing becau se i t genera l ly involves

the

 product ion

 o f

 ei ther

 a

 "yes"

  o r a

 "no" answ er

 (o r in

 some cases, m ak in g

a  choice  among  the  al ternatives provided). "Blank-fi l l ing" questions

(where

  th e

  t eache r p rov ides pa r t

  of the

  sen tence

  an d

  b l a n k s

  out the key

word( s )  for the  s tudents  to f il l in) are  a l so typ ica l ly found  becau se t hey

lessen

 the

  l ingu ist ic

 burden of the

  students (see Tsui,  1995).

Let us

 return

 to the

 physics lesson

 on the

 reed relay

 a nd

 consider

 the

 fo l -

lowing extract :

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 142/255

5.  Q U E S T I O N S A N D T H E S P A C E O F  L E A R N I N G  1 29

5.7  [Phys ics Lesson/Reed R elay /EM I/S2/T8]

1 T: . .. Oh

 then surely ,

 n ow

 girls,

 can you

 explain

 to m e, why

  doesn ' t

2

  th is m otor work?

3 Ss:  [silent]

4 T : W hy does n ' t th is m otor work? Jus t because m otor m ust use ?

5 Ss:

  [silent]

6 T: The current tha t is ? Large or small ?

7 S:  Large .

8 T:

  Large .

W e can see that the teacher asked an open quest ion and no response was

fo r thcoming .

  Then  the  teach er provided some l ingu is tic scaffolding  in the

form

  of filling in the

 b lanks . W hen th i s  failed

  to

 el ic i t

 a

 response,

  she

 pro-

vided

 m ore l inguis t ic help and a gain invi ted the s tudents to f i l l in the b lank .

W hen a response w as not imm ediately for thco m ing, she provided a choice

for  them . This kind

 of

 sequen ce res t r ic ts wha t counts

 as an

 acceptable

 an-

swer; the answer n ot only has to be  appropriate  in term s o f the content , but

also

 has to f i t into the l ingu ist ic structure provided. In order to an swe r the

teacher ' s

 ques t ion , th e s tudents had to shift  their focus o f at tent ion

 from

  the

substantive pa rt (i .e.,

 the

 reason

 why the

 motor

 did not

 work),

 to an

 answer

tha t w ou ld fi t the syn tax given by the teacher . W hen the teacher provided

tw o

 choices

 to the

 s tudents ,

 the

 cogni t ive deman d

 w as

 m in im al . This k ind

 of

funnel ing   effect  reduces th e space in w h ich the students can explore variou s

possible answers for them selves ,  and  formula te the  appropriate answer. It

a lso has the   de t r im enta l  effect  o f  encouraging s tudents  to gues s wha t  the

teacher has in

  m i n d ,

  and to try to

  produce

  an

  answer tha t wi l l meet

  the

teacher's approval. T he fol low ing is

 another

 excerpt

  from

 th e

 physics

 lesson

on

 the

 reed relay taug ht through E ngl ish (EM I).

5.8  [Physics Lesson/Reed Relay/EMI/S2/T8]

T he teache r asked  the students  to  compare  the  electric current  re-

quired

  to

  operate

  a

  motor

  and the

  electric current required

  for an

LED to

 light

 up .

1 7  T: . .. W hy

 does

  the LE D

  work here? [Nom inate]

1 8  S :

  [silent]

1 9

  T: The LE D w orks, but it [Teacher points at the m otor] does not

20 rotate . It 's [a] sim ilar construction. [N om inate] Tell m e, how

21

  m uch cu r ren t

 is

 needed

  to

 operate

 a

  motor?

22 S:  [silent]

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 143/255

1 30  T S U I  E T A L .

23 T: How  much cu r ren t is needed? It should be very  ?

24 S:  200mA.

25 T : A h ,

  nearly 200mA, very good, very large current .

 B ut can you

26 t e ll m e how m uch curren t i s needed to opera te a L ED ?

27 S:

  V ery smal l cur ren t .

28 T:

  Yes, very sma ll current

 is enough all

 r ight . Then

 can you

  explain

29 to me, why does  this LED  light?

30 S:  [si lent]

In

  th i s excerpt ,

  th e

  t e ach e r posed

  an

  o p e n q u e s t i o n

 in

  l ine

  1 7:

 " W h y

does the LED

 work?"

 W h en the s tudent had d i f f i cu l t i e s  responding to the

ques t ion ,

  th e

  t e a c h e r  m o d i f i e d

  th e

  q u e s t i o n i n t o

  a

  m o r e

  spec i f i c  o n e

a b o u t

  th e

  a m o u n t

 o f

  e l ect ri c cu r ren t r equ i red

 to

 o p e ra te

 a

  motor , "How

m uch cur ren t is needed to

 operate

 a

 motor?"

 ( l ines

 20-21).

 She then na r -

ro we d d o wn   th e  ques t ion  fu r the r  by  t u rn i n g it  in to a b l a nk- f i l l i ng  q u e s -

tion

  in

  line

  23, "It

  shou ld

  be

  very

  ?"

 w h i c h p ro v i de d

 th e

 s t u d e n t s

wi th  the  l inguist ic s t ructure of the first par t of the answer , and on ly re-

qui red them

 to f i l l in the ke y

 w o rd ( s ).

 A s

 p o i n t ed

 o u t

 be fo re , t h i s k ind

 o f

funne l ing o f ten  h a p p e n s

  in

  c l a s s ro o m s wh e re

  th e

  s t u d e n t s ' l a n g u a g e

ability

  is

 we a k because

 i t

 lessens

  the

 l ing uis t ic burden

 on the

 s tudents

 to

f o r m u l a t e

  the re sponse .

The use of this type of quest ioning sequence focuses the studen ts ' attention

on how they can finish the sentence in a w ay that w ill fit into the syn tax of the

partial

 sentence provided, and, very

 of ten,  in a way

 that

 wi l l

 also correspond

with

 th e

 answer

 the

 teacher

 has in

 m i n d.

 In the

 ju st c i ted excerpt ,

 the

 teach er 's

subsequent question "How m uch current is

 needed?"

 focused the students' at-

tention  on the

  size

 of the

 current .

 The

  l inguist ic

 scaffolding

  that

 the

 teacher

provided

 required

 the

 students

 to put in an

 adjective rather than

 an

 exact

 figure

(see line 23). Th e student, however, attended to the "how m uch " qu estion and

provided

 the

 ex act size

 of the

 current. Al thou gh this answer

 was

 accepted

 by

th e  teacher, she rephrased  it as "very large current." W e can assum e that the

students then realized th at the teacher was loo king for a general description of

th e

 size rather than

 the

 exa ct size

 of the

 current ,

 if w e

 look

 at the

 subsequent

exchange.

 W h en

 th e

 teacher asked, "How m uch current

 is

 needed

 to

 operate

an

 LED?"

 (line 26), a student this tim e replied,  "a very sm all current" instead

of

 giving

 the

 ex act size

 of the

 current (see l ine 27).

From this discussion,

 we can see

 tha t

 th e

 types

 o f

 qu est ions asked

 by the

teacher can

 either o pen

 up the space of

 learning

 by

 encou raging students

 to

explore possible answers

 an d to

 form ula te the i r

 ow n

 answers ,

 or

 reduce

 the

space  o f learning by conf in ing  students to only a rest r ic ted num ber of possi-

bilities

  and

 even

 by

 encouraging them

  to

 engage

  in

  guesswork.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 144/255

5.  Q U E S T I O N S  AND THE  S P A C E  OF  L E A R N I N G  131

Question Sequence

 and

  Focal Awareness

So

 far, w e have seen that questions play a very im portant role in constituting

th e

 space of learning in the classroom . They structure the learning experience

and  they  focus  th e  students' attention  on

  different

  aspects  of the  object of

learning, w hether wit tingly,

 or

 unw ittingly,

 on the

 part

 o f the

 teacher.

 In

 dis-

cussing th e data from the E nglish lesson on O ld M acDonald 's Farm, we have

seen that different  sequences of questions can fo cus the learners' attention on

quite  different

  things.

 W e

 also men tioned that teachers

  often

  decompose

  a

complex question into simpler questions,

  and we

  focused

  on the

  ways

  in

which such sequences could reduce the space o f learning. In this section, we

draw

 the

 reader's attention

 to the

 fact

 that such sequenc es som etim es lead

 to a

shift  in  focus,  with the result

 that

  the  intended object of  learning is lost.

Let

 u s

 take

 for

 example

 th e

 fol lowing

 tw o

 paral le l excerpts

 from the EM I

and the CM I

 phy sics lessons, w here

 the

 teachers exp lained exa ctly

 the

 same

phenom enon : why the m otor does not rotate in the  simple

 ci rcui t .

5.9   [Physics Lesson/Reed relay/EM I/S2/T8]

1

  T: . .. O h

 then surely,

 n ow

 girls,

 can you

 expla in

 to m e, why

  d oesn ' t

2 th i s m otor wo rk?

3 Ss:  [silent]

4 T: Why doesn't

 this

 motor

 work? Just because motor m u s t

 use ?

5  S s:  [silent]

6 T : The  cu r ren t tha t is ? Large o r  small?

7 S:

  Large.

8 T:

  La rge .

1 7  T: . .. W hy  does  the LE D  work here? [Nom inate]

18  S :  [silent]

1 9  T: The LED works, but it  [Teacher points at the

 motor]

 does not

20

  rotate . I t ' s

  [a]

 similar con struct ion. [No m inate] Tell

 m e, how

21 m uch cur rent i s needed to opera te a m otor?

22 Ss:

  [silent]

23 T: How m uch current is needed? I t should be very ?

24 S:

  200mA.

25 T: A h, nea r ly 200m A , very good, very large current . B ut can you

26

  tell

 me how

 m uch current

  is

 needed

  to

 operate

  a

  LED?

27 S:  V ery sma ll current .

28 T:

  Yes, very sm all current

  is

 enough.

  A ll

  right. Then

  can you

29 exp lain to m e, w hy does this LE D l ight [up]?

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 145/255

1 32  T S U I  E T A L .

30 Ss:  [si lent]

31 T : I t [ re fer r ing to the m otor] does not ro ta t e , j u s t beca use the re ' s

32 no

  large current .

 The LED

 wo rks jus t because

  ?

33 S:  Because the L E D —

34 T:

  U s e —

35 S: Use

  smal l current

 and the

 m o to r

 use

  very large current .

36 T: All  r ight , yes, your answer is so  impressive. Would you  please

37

  repea t

  it

 once . I t ' s cor rec t , repea t

  it

 o n c e .

38 S: The LED use[s]  smal l current w hi le the  motor use[s] very large

39

  current .

40 T :  Yes , very goo d , t he L E D u s e [ s ] [a ] s m a l l c u r r e n t w h i le the m o to r

41

  use[ s ] very l a rge curre n t .

 A ll

 r igh t ,

 O K .

Here the  teacher asked  two  open quest ions: "Why doesn' t this motor

work?"

 and

  "Why does

 the LED

 l ight

 [up]?" These

 qu est ions opened

 up a

num ber of possible answ ers , as pointed out before . However, because the

teacher used

 b lank-f i l ling

  quest ions to help the s tudents to provide an an-

swer

 to the

 open quest ions,

 th e

 s tudents '

 focus o f

 awareness shifted  from

  the

reasons w hy the m otor did not rota te , and why the LE D l i t up, to the am ou nt

of cu rrent needed

 by the

 m otor

 and the

 L E D .

 F or

 exam ple,

 in

 l ines

 20 and

23,

 the

 teacher repeated

 the

 ques t ion , "How m uch current

 is

 needed

  [ to op-

erate a

 motor]?"

 In line 26, she asked the same q uest ion abou t the L E D . A l-

though

 the

 students were able

 to

 tell

 the

 teacher tha t

 the LE D

 needed

 a

 very

sm all current , w hen the teacher posed the quest ion about w hy the L ED did

not l ight up in l ine 29, there w as no response from the c lass

 unt i l

  she gave

them some help.

  In

 other w ords,

 the

  students ' at tention

 w as

 focused

  on the

size of the current bu t they w ere u nab le to relate the size of the cu rren t to the

reason w hy the m otor did not rota te w hereas the LE D did l ight up .

Let us

 com pare

  the

 a foremen t ioned exam ple w i th

 the  fo l lowing

 excerpt

taken

  from

  the CM I c lass room .

5.10 [Physics Lesson/Reed re lay/CM I/S2/T8]

1 T: We find

  that even thou gh

 we

  shone

 a

  strong l ight

 on the

  L D R ,

2

  yes,

 its

 electric resistance value decreased.

  But in fact  th e

 electric

3 curren t tha t w i l l pass through i s and the e l ec t r ic cu rren t neede d

4 to by the motor is . Do you  kn o w w h a t I wo uld l ike you to

5

  answ er? [ N omi na te ]

  Y o u

 try,

 you t ry to

  exp l a i n

 w hy

 n o t h i n g

6 happ ens here . N ow I sh ine a s trong  l igh t on the L DR . r igh t? I ts

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 146/255

5.

  Q U E S T I O N S

  A N D T H E

  S P A C E

  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 33

7

  res is tance

  is ?

8 S:  Smal l .

9 T :  Yes, sm all.  So there w ill naturally be an electric current com ing

10 in . Can you guess w hat the

  difference

  is between this electric

11  current

  and the

  electric current

  it

 ac tua l ly needs?

1 2

  S : The

  resistor 's electric current

  is

 smal ler than

  the

  motor ' s .

1 3  T :  This means  the  motor needs  an  electric current that  is ?

14 S:

  L a rge .

1 5  T: N ot only large, but very large. Yes, and so . . . thank yo u, you

16 have answ ered correctly. An d so, even thoug h w e shine a strong

17 l ight on the LD R , i ts resistan ce value drops, and the so-ca lled

1 8 larger electric curren t

  f lows

  through the c i rcu i t . The m otor needs

19 a

 very large electric current

  to

 rotate,

 and so

 this

  is the  difference

20

  betw een th is c i rcui t

 and the

 other c i rcui t .

In this excerpt,

  the

  teac her also asked

  the

  s tudents

 to

 exp la in "why n o th-

ing

 happens

 here,"

 m eaning

 w hy the

 m otor

 did not

 rotate ( l ines

 5 and 6). She

also provided l ingu is t ic

  scaffolding

  to help the s tudents to provide the an-

swer ,

  by ask ing them what k ind of cur ren t would pass th rough and what

kind

  of

 cur ren t

 w as

  needed ( l ines

 3, 4, and 7).

 However ,

  after  the

 s tudents

had fi l led in the blanks, she posed an open que st ion once ag ain ( l ines 1 0 and

11 ) .

  All the  t ime,  the

 teacher

  focused  the

  s tudents '

  attention  on the

  differ-

ence be tween

  the

  current

  needed

  and the

  cur ren t tha t ac tua l ly passed

th r ough

 th e

 c i r cu i t .

 T he

 mo di f ica t ions

 of the

 open que st ion

 and the

 s e q u e n c -

ing of the

  closed quest ions

 did not  shift  the

 s tudents '  focal  awar enes s away

from   the

  or iginal open quest ion.

Final ly,  compare the fol lowing paral lel excerpts  from  the EMI and the

C M I

  physics lessons  where  again

  the

  s ame phenomenon

  w as

 be ing

  deal t

wi th ;

  that is , w hat happene d w hen the l ight shines on the L DR .

5 . 1 1

  [Phys ics Lesson/Reed Relay /CM I/S2/T8]

1

  T: OK. [Teacher points to the com plicated c i rcui t ] W e love

2

  shining

 a

  strong l ight

 on the

 LD R. Now , he re

  it

  conies, strong

3 l ight shines on the LD R . [Teacher shines the torch on the LD R ]

4 I' ll ask you questions step by step. For the circuit on the  left  side,

5

  th is c i rcui t ,

 the

  largest

 ef fect  is the

  e lec t r ic res is tance value ,

6   right? Strong light shines  on the  LDR, I 've told  you before , a nd

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 147/255

134

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36

37

S:

T:

S:

T:

S:

T:

Ss:

T:

Ss:

T:

S:

T:

Ss:

T:

S:

T:

S:

T:

T S U I E T A L .

the

  electric resistance value drops.

  O K,

  w h a t

  effect

  wi l l

 it

 have

on the  electric current  on  this side of the  c i rcu i t ? [nominate]

Electric resistance

  drops.

Electric resistance

  drops,  the

  electr ic curre nt

  ?

Increases.

Yes, th e

 electric

 current

 increases,

 th e  electric current

 becomes

larger,

  r ight?

  W h e n

  an

  electric current

  f lows

  through

  the

  c i rcui t ,

this coil

  is no

  longer s imply

  a

 coil ,

  it

 becomes

  an

 e l ec t romagne t .

E lec t romagnet s  can p rod uce  ?

Magnet i c f i e lds .

Yes, they produce magnet ic f ie lds. When

  it

 produces

  an

electromagnetic field, it has a series of effects  on this side [of the

circuit].  Then what?  [nominate], you try and con tinue. [Pointing

to the

 com pl ica ted c i rcu i t ]

[si lent]

N o w .

 the

 coi l

 has a

 m agne t ic

 f ield, and the

 reed  swi tch

  ? The

reed swi tch

  ?

[silent]

The  reed switch, the two  ends do not  touch each other originally,

but

 now  w hat happens  to  these  tw o  ends?

Touch each other .

Yes, the two ends touch each other . I f the two ends touch each

other ,

 how do w e

  describe this swi tch?

[silent]

The two

  ends  touch each other,

  how do I

 describe this sw itch?

Closed .

Close d, very good. Yes, thank y ou . No w, i f

  this switch

  closes,

then

  the

  elect r ic current

  can

  pass through th i s

 c i rcui t .

  Thi s motor

9

Rotates .

Rotates .

  Is

 tha t

  O K?

Here  the  teacher asked  four "open" quest ions (see l in es 7, 8, 1 9, 26, 28,

and 29) and  four "closed" ques tion s (l ines 10, 1 5, 22, 23, 34. and 35). If we

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 148/255

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 149/255

136

  T S U I

  ET AL.

1 3

  T :

  Should work ,

 but it

 does

 not

 rotate th is t ime .

 How

 a bout th is

1 4  ci rcu i t? [Teacher goes to the com plicated ci rcu i t ] Now once

15

  again l ight shines

 on the

 L D R ,

 its

 res is tance dec reas efs] .

 So,

1 6

  cur ren t f lows through th i s c i rcu i t . N ow  it's  no t  only a coi l

17  but a ?

18

  Ss:  [silent]

19

  T: Now

  this question again, [nominate]

  N ow

 th is t im e

  it is not

20  mere ly  a coil but a ?

21

  S s:  [silent]

22

  T :

  Artif icial

  magne t , all r igh t . Do you  still  rem em ber [ the]

23  name?

24 S:  E l ec t romagne t .

25

  T :  Once again .

26 S:  Elec t romagnet .

27   T :  Electromagnet , thank you,  it becam e an electrom agnet,  and

28 it produced  a mag net ic  field. So the  sw i tch here

  wi l l

 close

29 and a

  current [will]

 pass

  through this circuit ,

 so the

 m otor

30 rotate[s]. There is a sequence  of processes] [that] occur here an d

31 i t m ake[s] the m otor rotate .

Here  th e teach er asked an open question (lines 6 and 7):

 "What

 happens to

it  [resistance] when light shine[s] on the  LDR?" This q uestion, in a sim ilar

way to the qu estion posed  in excerpt 5.11 , asked  for a description o f the pro-

cess. W hen

 the

 teacher failed

  to elicit a

 response

 from the

 students,

 she

 posed

three blank-filling ques tions , one of which i s a repe t it ion (lines 10 ,1 1 ,1 6 ,1 7,

19, and 20), w hereas the others were c losed question s ( l ines 22 and 23) . How -

ever,  in the  course  of

  modify ing

  the  open question into

 blank-fi l ling

  ques-

tions,  the  blank-filling  quest ions became focused  on the  devices  and the

labels

 ( an

 artificial

  magnet

 and an

 electrom agnet) ,

 and the

 description

 of the

process

  w as

  lost.

  On the

 basis

  of our

  observation that questions

 focus

  stu-

den ts ' awareness on the aspect of the phenom enon that was q uestione d, w e

can reasonably conclude that

 the

 consequence

 of

 asking quest ions about

 the

devices,

 rather tha n the processes, wo uld

 result

 in the devices being brough t

to the  fore of the students ' aw areness and the processes rece ding to the back -

ground. A nd yet , it is the processes and not the devices

 that

 are cri t ical to the

understanding

 of the

  operation

 o f the

 reed relay.

C O N C L U S I O N

In chapter 1 , M arton, R une sson, and Tsui demon strated the central role

played by langua ge in con st i tu t ing the space of learning, and discussed at

length the im portance  o f l anguage as a m e a n s o f en capsu la t ing experience ,

m aking di s tinc t ions , and m odi fy in g one an other 's und ers tandin g of the

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 150/255

5.  Q U E S T I O N S  A N D T H E  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 37

world. They a lso

  observed

  the

  function

  of language in representing and

shaping

  the

  structure

  o f

  awareness

  o f

  participants

  in a

  d iscourse .

  In

  this

chapter , w e have foc use d

 specifical ly

 on the s tructure of que st ions and how

they  impinge on the s tructure of awareness of par t ic ipants in a discourse .

Throughou t  the  lesson  on the  reed relay,  w e  demonstra ted  how a  teacher

w as able to help her s tudents discern the cr i t ica l features of a reed re lay a t

cr i t ical

 points in the lesson, by asking qu est ions tha t focused  their a t tent io n

on aspects that var ied. Throughout  the  English lesson,  w e  demonst ra ted

how the different  quest ions asked by the teacher co uld resul t in s tudents fo-

cus ing on very

  different

  aspects of the object of learning. W e also showed

that

 the way in wh ich the teacher sequen ced the quest ions co uld resu l t in a

shif t

  of the

 learners '

 focal

  awareness

  from

  the

 crit ical aspects

 of the  object

o f

  learning

  to

  aspects that

  are not

  crit ical.

  In

  other words,

  the

  teacher ' s

awareness

  of how

  ques t ions

  can

  shape

  the

  space

  o f

  learning ( i .e . ,

  the en-

ac ted ob jec t of learning) is essential  if our effort  in br inging abou t  power fu l

l earn ing  in the c lassroom is to resul t in the convergence between the in-

t ended ,

  the enacted, an d the l ived object of learning.

E N D N O T E S

'The term   d issemina t ion  is  used  in a  neutral sense;  it  does  not  imply  a

t ransmiss ive view  o f  t each ing .

2

F or  an exp lana t ion  of the  L D R ,  see  chap te r  1 , endnote  4.

3

In

 this chap ter, data from the lesson taught in Can tonese w i l l be coded as CM I,

and data from  the  lesson taug ht in Engl ish wi l l be coded as E M I. T he data from the

CMI lesson   is  t ransla ted semant ica l ly , keeping  as m uch  as po ss ib le  to verba t im

transla t ion.

4

A1 1 s tuden ts ' nam es

  are

  f ic t i t ious .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 151/255

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 152/255

6

The  Semantic  Enrichment

of the

  Space

  of

  Learning

Amy B. M.  T s u i

TH E   SEMANTIC

  D I M E N S I O N

OF THE  SPACE  O F

  LEARNING

In   chapter  1 , M ar ton , Runesson ,  and  Tsui proposed  that learning  is the

proces s

 o f

 develop ing

 a

 cer tain

 w a y o f

 seeing,

  an d

 po in ted

 o u t

 tha t

  d i f fer-

en t

  w a y s

  o f

  seeing al low

  fo r  dif ferent

  w a y s

  o f

 ac t ing . They a l so po in ted

ou t

  t ha t in order  to he lp l earners  to develop  a ca pab i l i ty fo r  seeing things

in  a

  p o w e r fu l  w a y ,

  w e

  need

  to

  focus

  on

  w h a t

  is

  being learned

  and

  he lp

learners d i scern c r i t ica l fea ture s

 of the

 object

  o f

 l earn ing .

 In

 o ther words ,

inves t iga t ions   o f  l earn ing involve s tudying h o w  learners experience  th e

o b j ec t

  o f

 l earn ing ,

 and

 inves t iga t ions

 o f

 c lass room learn ing involve anal -

yses o f the opportunit ies  to experience the  object  o f

 learning

 that  are af-

forded  to learners . In th is sense, the space of learning is an  exper ient ia l

space.

  A n exp erient ial space is not an ins tan t iat ion, but rather a po tent ial

for  unders tanding, seeing, and act ing in the world (see Hal l iday &

M a t t h i e s s e n ,

  1999). It is in re la t ion to th is potent ial th at learners  can m a k e

sense of a pa r t icu lar objec t of learning . As su ch, the exp erient ial space is

e las t i c . The tea che r can e ither widen th i s space by  af fording  learners op-

por tun i t i e s

 to

 explore

 the

 o b j ec t

 o f

 learning

 in a

 variety

 o f

 w a y s ,

 o r

 nar row

this space

  by

  depriving them

  o f

 such opportunities.

In

  the

 preceding ch apters,

 w e

 have seen that

 the w ay in

 wh ich

 the

 object

of

 learn ing is experienced by learners depends on w hich aspects of the ob-

ject of learning  are

 being

 focused on and discerned  as critical.  In  this chap-

ter,  it is

  proposed that experiencing

  an

 object

  of

 learning—in

  the

  sense

  of

discerning i ts cri tical aspects and experiencing the patterns o f variation—

13 9

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 153/255

140

  T S U I

necessar ily involves ass igning a m ean ing (or me anin gs) to i t. For exa m ple,

for a youn g learner  w ho lives  in a country w here high-rise b uilding s do not

exist ,  a "house"  m e a ns  a  p lace where people  live,  as  opposed  to  p l aces

w here a nim als live, such as

 "stables."

 A ho use is therefo re a choice in the se-

mantic system  of accom m oda tion  for different  k inds of l iving th ings . How-

ever, to a you ng learner who l ives in a ci ty w here th ere are high -r ises as wel l

as houses, a house means something

  different.

  In this instance, "house" is a

choice in the seman t ic sys tem of accom m odat ion for people, wh ere houses

and

 apar tments

 are

 choices .

 A

 house m eans

 not an

 apar tmen t : tha t

 is. it is a

two-  or  three-story  building, rather than  a  single unit within  a m ul t i-s tory

building  com pr is ing m any un i t s .

T he

  meanings that learners assign

  to the

 object

  of

  learning depend

  on a

host of th ings . The teacher can  affect  these m ean ings t h rough examples and

analogies , throu gh

 the

 stories th at

 he

 tel ls ,

 and the

 contex t s tha t

 he

 br ings

 in .

The m ean ings wi l l also depend o n the persona l experiences that the learners

bring to bear o n the object of lear ning . Together , a l l of these m ea nin gs con-

stitute  the  semantic dimension  of the space  of  learning, o f which language

plays a central role.

In  this chap ter, exam ples from a variety of classroom s are ci ted to

  i l lus-

t rate wha t is mean t by the sem antic dimension  of the space  of learning, and

the way s in whic h the learners ' experience of the object of learn ing is en-

riched  semantically  are  discussed.  T he  central role played  by  language  in

the

 const i tu t ion

 of the

  seman t ic d im ens ion

 of the

 space

 of

 learn ing

 is i l lus-

t ra ted by com paring classrooms w here r ich

 l inguis t ic

 resources were avail-

able, with those w here  the  l inguist ic resources were l imited.

CONTEXTUAL

 VARIATION AND THE

 SEMAN TIC

E N R I C H M E N T  OF THE

 SPACE

  O F  L E A R N I N G

It is c o m m o n pract ice among teachers ,  no  matter what subject matter they

are dea l i ng wi th , to

 shif t

 f rom the c lass room c ontex t to co ntex t s ou t s ide the

classroom

  (ranging

  from

  contemporary contexts

  to

  historical contexts)

  in

the course of thei r explan at ions . How ever , few teachers ask why they are

doing this , and w h a t effect  th i s k in d of con tex tua l var ia t ion has on l earn ing .

In  this chapter,  it is  proposed that contextual var iat ion  is a very im por tan t

and

 co m m only used pract ice that help s learners to ma ke sense of and to re-

late

  to the

 object

  of

  learning.

M a t h e m a t i c s  Lesson:  Th e  P o s t m a n ' s  Ro ute

Let us t ake , fo r exam ple , the ma the m at ics l esson on the pos tm an ' s rou te

discussed

  in  chapter  3. To help  the  s tudents  to see the  relat ionships  be-

tween pa t te rns  ( as  seen th rough ro ta t ion  an d  r e f l ec t i on ) ,  th e  t eache r

started

  by

 asking

  th e

 s t uden t s

 to

 c o m e

 up

 w i th

 a s

 m a n y p a t t e rn s

 as

 poss i -

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 154/255

6 . S E M A N T I C   E N R I C H M E N T  O F TH E

  S P A C E

  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 41

ble , by c on nec t ing the dots that s tood for delivery po ints . In order to

  faci l i-

tate task com ple t ion , the teacher provided the context of a pos tm an h aving

to deliver let ters to eight d ifferent  places and then re turn to the post  off ice.

W h e n   the s tudents h ad com ple ted  the task, the  teach er asked the s tudents

to   f igure  ou t  wh a t  w as

 wrong

  with  one  particular design that  he  [the

teacher] ident i f ied , and then to dec ide wh at cons t i tu ted the best route . The

s tudents had no  diff icul ty

  po in t ing

 o u t

 tha t

 the

 design

 in

 ques t ion

 fai led  to

show the direct ion of the route , and fa i led to show h ow the po stm an c ou ld

get back  to the pos t  of f ice .  They a lso  had no  d i f f icul ty  dec id ing tha t the

best

 route

 w as the

  shortest route.

 T he tasks were n o t  diff icul t  for the

  stu-

dents becau se the con text of a pos tm an de l iver ing mai l w as very

  famil iar ,

and i t w as

  easy

  fo r

  them

  to

  ident i fy

  the

  routes

  in

  re la t ion

  to

 w h a t

  a

 pos t -

m a n ' s

 job

  involved. On ce

 the

 e ight

 bes t

 rou te s we re

 identif ied, th e

 t e ache r

swi tched from  the everyday context to a m athem at ica l context , so tha t the

designs took   on a different  meaning.

 They were

 no longer

 routes

 but pat-

terns .

 T h e

  c o n c e p t

 o f

 rou tes nece ssari ly involved having

  a

 d i rec t ion

 a nd

being able to re turn to the s tar t ing point , w hich m ade i t very easy fo r the

s tudents  to think of the cri ter ion for the best route . If the teacher had

started

 the lesson by present ing the pa tte rns in a mathem at ica l con text , the

students would have ha d to think  in sym bolic  and abstract  te rms and the

t a sk wo uld have been m ore  dif f icu l t .

W hat is

 interesting

 is

 tha t when

 the

 students failed

  to see the

 re la tionsh ip

between the e ight pat terns and hence were unable to categorize them , the

teacher switched back to the context of the po stm an's route , wh ich im m edi-

ately m ade

  the

 task easier.

 The

 students  were then able

 to see

 that

 th e

 eight

routes w ere re la ted  in two  different  ways: rota t ion and ref lect ion.  In other

words,

 the

 studen ts w ere able

 to

 draw

 on

 their everyday kno w ledge

 to

 m a k e

sense of what the pat terns m eant .

E ng l i s h  as a Second  L a n g u a g e ( E S L )

 Lessons:

W e a t h e r

  an d Seasons

In a

  s tudy

  o f  p r imary  E S L  teaching, Chan (2002) s tudied  the  p r imary

Grade 4 lessons taug ht by two teachers , in which the d i rec t ob jec t of lea rn-

ing

 w as  weather, c lothing,  and

 seasons.

  In the first

 part

 of an  80-minute

lesson, both teachers used p ic tures

 to

 e l ic i t

 th e

 vocabula ry

 fo r

 w ea the r

 a n d

c lo th ing .

 Hav ing establ ished that there

 w as

 shared kno w ledge

 o f

 di f ferent

w e a t h e r con di t ion s and the corresponding types of c lo th ing , both teachers

in t roduced "seasons"  and asked s tudents to deduce the season  from  the

clothing that they showed them .

 In so

 doing , both

 teachers

 w ere trying

  to

he lp the i r s tudents see tha t w ea ther and c lo th in g w ere re la ted in m eaning-

ful  wa y s . W e cou ld say tha t the teachers were t rying to establ ish a s e m a n -

tic

 f ie ld w i th two sem ant ic dom ains : wea ther and c lo th ing . Su bsequen t ly ,

both teachers in t roduc ed a  fur ther  semant ic domain of

 "seasons,"

 h e n c e

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 155/255

1H42

  T S U I

wea the r , an d c lo th ing ) w ere unders tood no t on ly o n the i r own , b u t also in

relation to

  each other.

In the

 latter part

 o f

 each lesson,

 th e

 teachers used

 a

 story

 a s the

 context

 fo r

recycling the vocabulary i tem s introduced, a nd for reinfo rcing the students '

unders tanding of

  wea the r

  and

  seasons.

  T he

  stories that

  the two

  teachers

told, however, were very   different.

Teacher A to ld a s tory abou t a poor  little gir l , M ary, w h o w a s driven o u t

of the house by her

 s tepmother

  in the

 w inte r wh en

 i t w as

 cold

 a nd

 snowing.

T he  s tepmother sa id tha t  she  w o u l d  not le t  Ma ry b a c k  in  un le s s  she

brought back  some  beau t i fu l  f lowers . Mary walked  up the  h i l l  and

  fell

as leep wi th ex hau s t ion . Four brothers ,

  th e

  four

  sea sons , c am e

 to her

  res-

cue. Brother Spring waved

  a

 mag ic w and

 and the

 w ea ther became w arm

an d

 ra iny . M ary

 w as

 then able

 to ge t

 f lowers

  for her

 s tepm othe r .

 B u t

 w h e n

Mary brought

  the

  f lowers home,

  h er

  s tepmother would

 no t le t her in and

told

 her to ge t

 some app le s . Th i s t ime Bro the r Su m m er cam e

 to her

 re scue ,

and the w ea ther becam e h ot and sunny so tha t M ary could p ick some ap-

ples . T h e

 s to ry con t inued

 and a l l four

 b ro the rs cam e

 to her

 re scue

 in

 tu rn ,

each t ime

  the

  greedy s tepmother m ade unreasonable dem ands

  fo r

  some-

th ing  tha t cou ld not be  f ound  in tha t pa r t i cu l a r sea son .

L e t us

  c o n s i d e r

  the

  s e m a n t i c d i m e n s i o n

 of the

  s p a c e

  of

  l e a r n i n g

t h a t w a s

  o p e n e d

  u p

  w i t h t h i s s t o ry . P r i o r

  to

  t e l l i n g

  th e

  s t o r y ,

  th e

t e a c h e r had a l ready e s t ab l i shed the r e l a t i o n b e t w e e n two s e m a n t ic do-

m a i n s ( s e a s o n s

  a n d

  w e a t h e r )

  b y

  s h o w i n g

  h o w o n e i s

  r e l a t e d

  t o t he

other . The story p ro v i d e d the c o n t e x t not o n l y for m a k in g

 sense

 of the

r e l a t i o n s h i p  b e t w e e n w e a th e r  a n d  s e a s o n s ,  b u t  a l so  fo r  i n t r o d u c i n g

a n o t h e r s e m a n t i c d o m a i n r e l a te d

  to

 w e a th e r

  a n d

 sea son , t ha t

 i s ,

 p l a n t s

a n d

  f r u i t s .

  W h i l e p l a n t s a n d  f r u i t s  w e r e p a r t  o f t h e  s t u d e n t s '

 e x i s t i n g

k n o w l e dg e , i t w a s th e t e a c h e r w h o b r o u g h t th e r e l a t i o n s h i p o f t h i s d o -

m a i n w i t h s e a s o n s

  a n d

  w e a t h e r i n t o

  th e

  s t u d e n t s '

  f o c a l

  a w a r e n e s s .

W h a t

 is

 i n t e r e s t i n g

 is the way the

 t e ach e r va r i ed

 the

 r e l a t io n s h i p s

 be-

t w e e n  s e a s o n s , w e a t h e r , an d p l a n t s an d  f r u i t s .  S h e s t a r t e d w i th a n e g a -

t ive re l a t ionsh ip b e twe en them

 by

  g e t t i n g

  the

  s t u d e n t s

  to see  \vhat

could  n o t b e found  i n a p a r t i c u l a r s e a s o n , a n d s h e h e l p e d t h e s t u d e n t s

to make

 sense

 of  t h i s n e g a t i v e r e l a t i o n s h i p t h ro u g h the  c h a r a c t e r  of

th e  s te p m o t h e r , w h o m a d e im p o s s i b le d e m a n d s o n l i t t l e M a r y . L a t e r , a

p o s i t i v e r e l a t i o n s h i p b e t we e n t h e s e d o m a i n s

 was

  e s t a b l is h e d w h e n

e a c h

 o f t h e  f o u r

  b r o t h e r s c a m e

  to

 M a r y ' s r e s c u e

 b y

  c h a n g i n g

 th e

  sea-

son and the w e a t h e r so t h a t the p l a n t or the f r u i t  d e m a n d e d by the  s t ep-

m o t h e r c o u l d b e

  f o u n d .

As the lesson progressed, there was a

 gradua l w idening

 of the

  semant ic

f ield  f rom w e a th e r an d c lo th ing , to inc lude sea sons , p l an t s , an d  fruits .  T he

fol lowing

  excerpt

  is an

 example

  of how the

  teacher related

  one

  semant ic

do ma i n

 w i th ano the r.

H

idening the semantic field so that the three semantic domains seasons,

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 156/255

6.

  S E M A N T I C E N R I C H M E N T  O F T H E  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 43

6.1 [English Lesson/P4/Weather  and  Season]

T :

  Mary's mother

  says,  "No.

  I don't

  want you.

  Go away." Her

m other says, "Okay,

 you ask me to let you

 come back,

 b ut

 unless

you get me some

 beau tiful flowers

 (I

 won't)."

 Can you get some

beaut i ful

  f lowers

 in

 w inter?

Ss:  N o.

T:  Outside  it is .

Ss  :

  Cold

  and

  snowy.

T :

  Can M ary get some f low ers?

Ss  :  [loudly] N o

T he readiness w ith

 w hich

 the students

 responded

 to the teacher

 when

 she

asked if M ary co uld get flow ers in the w inter when i t w as cold and snowy

show ed that they w ere able to make sense of the story an d see the relation

be tween  the  semant ic domains.

Teacher B also told a story

 after

  introducing all the vocabulary i tem s for

w eather, clothing,

  and

  seasons.

 T he

  story

  w as

  about

  a

 little boy, B illy,

 and

w h a t he did on each of the seven da ys of a w eek and wh at the we ather w as

like.  For

  example,

  on M onday,

  when

  it w as

  raining,

  Billy watched televi-

sion;

 on

 Tu esday, when

 i t was

  sunny, B illy play ed basketball ;

 and so

  forth.

The students were asked to learn the days of the w eek, and to say w hat B i l ly

did

 and wh at the weather w as l ike . The w ay in w hich the teacher tr ied to re-

late the three sem antic doma ins was largely arbitrary. A ltho ug h certain ac-

tivities w ere logically related to the w eather co nditions (e.g. , "swimm ing"

and

 "sunny w eather"), there were others that w ere not (e.g. , "w atching tele-

vision" and

 "rainy

 w eather").

 A ctivities

 and

 days

 of the

 week

  are of

 them -

selves

 no t

  log ically related either. When

  the

  three sem ant ic dom ains w ere

taken

  together, they

 did not

  form

 a

  sem ant ic

  field.  T he

  story therefore

 did

not  serve as a

 meaningfu l

  context to w hich the stude nts could easily relate,

nor did it

 help

 th e

 students

 to

 make sense

 o f

 each

 of the

 sem antic dom ains

 in

relation to the other two . In other w ords, the s tuden ts ' un derstanding of the

relat ionship

 betw een the three dom ains was not enriched sem antical ly by

the story.

  If

  anything,

  the

  story confused

  the  s tudents '

 u nderstanding

  be-

cause of the

  arbit rary re la t ionsh ip that

 the

 teacher

 had

 tried

  to

 establ ish.

S c i e n c e L e s s o n : N e u t r a l i z a t i o n

To   further i l lustrate what

  can be

  achieved

  by

  contextual variation,

  let us

take a  look  at an  example  of a  secondary Grade  2  level science lesson

(Hoare ,

  2003). The object of learning in the lesson was "neutralization."

The  teacher first introduced the  concepts

  o f  acidi ty , alkal ini ty ,

 an d the use

of the/?// value s cale  to measu re the strength of acids and alkalis. Sh e also

asked

 the

 students

 to find ou t the

 acidity

 and

 alkalinity

 o f

 som e acids

 an d al-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 157/255

144  T S U I

kalis

  by

  using

  pH

  paper .

  Af te r

  this,

  she

  asked

  the

  students

  to

  test

  the pH

value of several h ouseh old substances , su ch as lem on ju ice , vinegar , tooth-

paste , baking soda, b leach ing ag ent , and a canned soft  dr ink. The  fo l lowing

is

 an excerpt

  from

  the classroom.

6.2 [Science lesson/S/Neutral izat ion/EMI]

T: . . .

  Okay, now,

  in our

 dai ly l i fe

  w e

 have ,

 um, we

 com e in to con-

tact

  wi th many things , r ight? For example, toothpaste , lemon

juice, e tc . . Do you  k n o w w h a t  is the pH  va lue o f  th i s? I s th is a

strong ac id?

 Is it a

 weak ac id?

 Is it a

 strong

 a lka l i o r

 weak a lka l i?

W e

 don ' t know. S o in the  fol low ing  I w a n t you to do [the] experi-

m e n t

  on

 page

  .

S:

  167, 167.

T :

  167,

 is

 th at right?

 A nd

 then

 w e

 have

 to

 test

 the pH

 va lue

 of

 lem on

juice, bleaching agent, baking  soda,  detergent, glass cleaner,

toothpaste , 7-Up™,  but  don' t drink  it  [s tudents laugh ] , m i lk o f

magnes ia ,

  sea

 w a te r

  .. .

S : I[ ' l l] drink it .

T :  Do n' t dr ink  it.

S:

  Why?

T: I [wi l l] have to call the am bulan ce. So, you do the same. Use the

pH

 paper.

  H ow

 ma n y

  o f

 t hem?

S:  Those  7-Up, change [them]

  to

  Coca-Cola™

  ah

  [Meaning

  re-

place 7-Up with Coca-Cola]

T:

  Yes.

S :

  Ten...

S: Those 7-Up chang e [ them]

  to

 C oca-Cola .

T: Yes, you know that there is C oca-Co la there.

S: [inaudible]

T:  Yes, sorry, ah,  maybe 7-Up  w as

 diff icult

  to buy.

S: A h I don ' t . .. I can go to the 7-11 ™ to buy i t.

T :

  Okay ,  after  this lesson. Okay

  w e

  change [ them]

  to

  Coca-Cola.

B ut

 don't

 drink

  it.

  Remember.

[Students co nduc ted the experiment and recorded the pH values

of the

  substances.]

T: Now

 okay look here. Lemon juice

 is of pH ...

S:  Three.

T: So we

  cal l

  it .

Ss: Strongly  acidic, strongly, strongly acidic.

T: How do we  describe  it?

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 158/255

6.

  S E M A N T I C E N R I C H M E N T  OF THE  S P A C E  OF  L E A R N I N G  145

Ss:

  [silent]

T :  Yes, it is a  strong acid, so it is  s t rongly ac idic . . . .

Prior

  to

  this experiment, al though

  the

  s tudents were famil iar wi th

  th e

household substances listed

 by the

 teacher, these substances m eant n othing

more than things that they   found

  in

  supermarkets

  and

  k i t chens .

  In

  other

words ,  the  meanings  of  these substances were interpreted  in the  semant ic

domain

 of the

 supermarket

 or the

 ki tchen,

 an d the

 level

 at

 w h ich

 the

 students

were operat ing when they thought about these objects was the level of ev-

eryday knowledge. However, when the teacher asked the students to do an

exper iment

 on

 hou sehold substances ( i .e., w hen

  the

 s tudents w ere engaged

in

  f inding

 o ut their pH values and using tech nical langu age, su ch as "acid,"

"alkali ," and "strongly acidic" or "weakly acidic" to describe these sub-

stances),

  the

  household substances  were recon strued,

  not in the

  semantic

dom ain of everyday items, but in the dom ain o f chem icals . A t th is point , the

level at w hich  th e students were operating changed from the level  of  every-

day

 know ledge

 to the

  level

 o f

  scientific

  (or

  technical ) knowledge. What

  is

interesting

  in the

 jus t m entioned excerpt

  is

 that

 w e can see the

 students

  and

teachers operat ing at both know ledge levels and in both sem ant ic dom ains .

In

 other words ,

 we may say

 that

 the

  semant ic dimension

  is

 enriched

  in the

sense that there

  is an

 addi t ional layer

 of

 mean ing .

Having  established the con cep ts of alkalinity and acidity, the teach er in-

t roduced the

 concept

 o f neutralization  by

 asking

 the

 students

 to mix an

 acid

(hydrochlor ic

 acid) with a n alkal i (sodium hydrox ide) unt i l they obtained a

pH  value o f seven, a point a t wh ich t he so lut ion becom es nei ther acidic nor

alka l ine .

 T he teacher explained that neutral izat ion is a process by w hich an

acid  is added to an alkali unti l the result ing solution  is salt a nd water . A f te r

the s tudents had done the experim ent , the teacher t r ied to help them to m ake

sense

  of

 neu tralization

  by

 shifting

 from

  the

  science laboratory context

  to an

everyday context . She used a num ber o f exam ples to explain the app l ica-

tions

  of

 neutralization.

  O ne

 example

  w as

  that

  of

 using toothpaste

  to

  brush

our

 t ee th .

 S he

 explained that

 the

 ac id produced

 after

  eat ing  food  causes

 o ur

teeth   to decay,  and  that toothpaste neutralizes  the  acid. Another example

was the use of

 m i lk

 o f

 magn esia

  to

  neutral ize excessive acid produced

 by

food

  in the stomach, and thus al leviate stomachache. Yet another example

w as the use of ointm ent con taining alkal i to neutral ize the acid produc ed by

m osqu i to b i tes .

  In

  this section

  of the

  lesson,

  w e see the

  reverse process

  of

contextualizat ion happening. Previously, neutral izat ion

 w as

 m ere ly

 a

 labo-

ratory process in w hich ch em icals were m ixed together . Here, neu tral iza-

t ion

 w as presented as a process that takes place frequen tly in our everyday

lives. Ac ids

 and

 alkalis were

 no

 longer un derstood on ly

 a s

 chem icals

 in

 sci-

ence laboratories

  but as

  chemicals that could

  be

  found

  in

  ma n y

  different

substances outside the laboratories. Once again, there were two levels of

knowledge

 operating, and also tw o

  semantic domains

  in

 wh ich neutral iza-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 159/255

146  TSUI

t ion w as unders tood: a scien t if ic process dom ain, and a dai ly   life  process

domain. The  fol lowing excerpt shows that as soon as the teacher explained

the toothpaste as an example of neutral izat ion, the s tudents were able to

make sense of neutral izat ion in another daily

 life

  contex t— the use of m ag-

nesia to neutral ize ac ids in the s tomach.

6.3  [Science lesson/N eutralization/S2/EM I]

T:

  A h,  toothpaste  is  alkaline,  and it  neutralizes  the  acid  in our

mo u t h   af ter  w e  have taken some

  food

  . . . .  A nd n eu t ra l iza tion

takes place there so that we can ha ve hea l thy teeth . Okay. Sec-

ond,

 I

 know some, m any, people w ould like

 to

 have m any— a  full

lunch . . . . W hen

 you

 have taken

 a lot of

 food,

 and

 som etimes

  you

find

  that this part

  is not

 okay . S tom ach

  ?

S :  S tomachache .

T: You  have s tom achache . Oka y . . . . W hen  you have taken a lot of

food, yo ur s tomach w i l l g ive

 out

 acid

 to

 m a k e

 the food

  in to sm al l

pieces   so that you can take  in the  food, okay?  . . . So at that t im e

w hen your stom ach gives out a lot of acid, you  find that it is pa in-

f u l . . . .  S o what should you do a t

 that

  t im e? Some peop l e w ou ld

like to

 take some tablets. O kay this

 is one of the

 tablets .

 W e call it

m ilk of m agnes ia . . . . H as anybody taken th is before? W hen you

have s tomachache,  you can take  one . . .

. .. W hy do w e  have to take one?

S: Neut ra l ize .

T :  Y e s , . . . the reason  is tha t it ne u t ra l izes the acid f rom o ur s tom ach

so

 tha t

 y o u

 becom e  peace fu l  . . . .

A s in the  previou s excerpt (6.2),  we may say that by varying the con text,

the sem ant ic dim ension w as enriched and the teacher w as able to help s tu-

dents ass ign an addi t iona l layer of m ean ing to the ob ject of learning .

COLLABORATIVE   E N RI CH M E N T  O F TH E SE MANTIC

D I M E N S I O N

  BY

 LEARN ERS

So

 far,

 the

 exam ples examined w ere classroom exam ples w here

 the

  teacher

enriched

  the

 sem ant ic d imen s ion

 by

 br inging

  in

 stories

  o r

 con texts outs ide

the classroom  and the laboratory  to help learners to m ake sense o f the object

of  learning.  In  this section,  w e  look  at  examples  in  w h i c h  the  learners

brought

 in

 their

 own

 experience

 to

 assign m eanings

 to the

 object

 of

 learning

and

 see how the space of learning w as enriched se m ant ical ly by the collab-

orat ive construction   o f m ean ing among  the  learners.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 160/255

6 .  S E M A N T IC E N R I C H M E N T

  O F T H E

  S P A C E

  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 47

C h i n e s e

  L e s s o n :

  The S lo th

The data presented here com e from a prim ary Grade 3 Chinese langua ge les-

son, "The

  Sloth,"

 th e

 background

  of

 which

  has

  already been introduced

 in

chapter

 2. W e

 shall

 now see how the

  learners assigned meanings

  to a

 video

that  they watched together,

  how

  they collaboratively constructed

  a

 written

representation

  of the

 video,

  and how the

  space

  of

 learning

 w as

  enriched.

In

 th e

 lesson

 on the

 sloth,

 the

 object

 of

 learning

 w as to

 w rite

 a

 descript ion

of the  appearance  of a youn g s loth ,  and a  story about  its  search  for a new

h o me

 af ter

  it lost its m other and had ea ten all the leaves on the tree where i t

lived.

 T he

 teacher provided

 a f ram ework for

 describing

  an

 animal that

 w as

based

 o n the

 de script ion

 of a

 panda that they

 had

 studied

 in the

 previous les-

son.

  The

  teacher used

  the

  word  "appearance"

  as a

  hyponym

  for

  body

  or

t runk, head (brain),

 four

 l im bs and color (of the

 fur

 or skin). Then he po inted

out  to the class that it w as also necessary to describe w hat the anim al was

like wh en i t was in m ot ion. For example, the movem ent of a panda is very

c lumsy. He also pointed o ut that i t w as necessary to describe the slo th 's eat-

ing h abi ts and character. After p roviding the f rame wo rk, he showed a video

of the

 story

 of a

 sloth.

 O ne

 striking featu re

 o f

 this lesson

 is the w ay in

 w h i c h

the

  teacher repeatedly showed

  the

  video,

  at

  first  with narrat ion

  and

  later

wi thou t

 narration, each t im e showing a

 different

  segment of the tape w ith a

different

  focus.

  This  repeti t ion not only rei terated the content , but also

opened up the space for students to explore the ways in which they could

m ake sense

 of the

 features

 of the

 sloth. Students w ere then asked

  to

 work

 in

groups

  and

 produce

  a

 writ ten text

  to

 describe what they

 had

  seen.

In the

 fo l low ing di scuss ion ,

 w e

 exam ine some

  of the

 t ex t s p rodu ced

 b y

the s tudents ,  and the  discuss ion data that  led to the  product ion  o f  these

texts .

  W e see how the

  s tude nts ass igned m eanings

  to the

  video,

  and how

their

 cons t rua l

  of

 wha t they

 had

  seen enriched

  the

  semant ic aspect

 o f the

space of l earn ing .

Sloth s  Head  a n d

 Body.  The fol lowing is a w ritten descript ion produ ced

by Group  2: '

It  is a

 s lo th ,

 it s

 head

  is

 l ike

 a

 b ig

  chicken egg,  it s

 body

  is  l ike  a

 b ig

  os tr ich

  e gg ,

 very

long ,  and the

  th ree c laws

  on its

  fo r e l imb s

  are

  pa r t icu la r ly long .

  Its

  co lor

  is

 b l a c k ,

wh i t e ,  gray, brown. Very

  short

  tai l .  T o

  look

  for  food  and a

  home ,

  the  s loth

  s low ly

c l i m b e d

  d o w n  the  tree,  and  swam frees ty le .  It  crawled  to the  shore .  A s  soon  as it

r eached

  th e

 shore ,

  it

 g lanced

 to the

  east

  and the

 west . V ery

 soon,

  it found  a

 tree.

 It fi-

nal ly  found

  a new  home .

Let u s

 focus

  for the

 m o me nt

  on the

 s tudents ' desc ription

  of the

 head

 and

body

  of the

  sloth. Although most  groups s im ply m ade

 u se of the

 teache r's

suggestion that  the  sloth 's head  w as  like  a m onkey 's head, this group  felt

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 161/255

148  T S U I

tha t  us ing another an im al

  to

  describe

  the

  sloth

 w as no t

  adequate ,

 and

 tha t

they ought  to use  something  else  for  comparison.  T he  fol lowing excerpt

shows

  how the

  students explored

  the

 descript ion

  of the

 s loth ' s hea d.

6.4  [Chinese  lesson/P3/The  Sloth/Group Discuss ion/Group 2]

296 S: W ha t 's i t [ the hea d] l ike?

297 S:

  Like

  a

 monkey ' s head .

298 S*  This  is not  s ay ing any th ing .

299 S: They are both an im als .

300 S : I ts h e a d . . .

301 S: Is l ike a chicke n egg.

302 S: Is l ike an oval chick en egg .

303 S:

  Ju st l ike

 a

 ch icken

 egg is

 okay.

304 S: No   need  to say  tha t it is oval as w e l l.

305 S:

  Just [put t ing dow n] chicken

 egg is

 o k a y ?

306 S:

  Like

 a

 ch icken

 egg is

 okay.

307 S:

  Ch icken eggs

 are

 a lway s oval .

308 S:

  It 's

  got to be

 b igger than

 an

 egg. It ' s

 no t

  t ha t sm a l l .

309 S: Th at 's r ight , chick en eggs are very sm al l .

310 S:  That's right .

3 11 S :

  Wha t ' s

  it

 l ike again?

312 S:  Like a big  chicken egg.

313 S: A big

  ch icken

 egg is not

  tha t b ig .

314 S:

  That's

 okay.

  Y ou

 describe

  the

 head

  ...

315 S: A big  goose egg .

316 S: A big goose egg is fine.

31 7  S: A big

  ch icken

 egg is f ine.

318 S: How do I

 w rite

 the

 word "egg"?

319 [The studen ts helpe d each other w rite  the word

 "egg"]

320 S:  Le t 's see, w ha t 's a big ostrich egg like. L et 's say a big ostrich

321

  egg,

  is

 that okay?

322 S:

  Okay, ostrich egg .

323  S: Ostr ich eggs are very big?

324 S:  Okay, let's describe the body.

325 S:  [ reminding the s tudent who i s w r it ing] B ig  ostrich egg.

326 S:  Ostr ich egg.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 162/255

6.  S E M A N T I C

  E N R I C H M E N T  O F TH E  S P A C E  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  1 49

From this discuss ion (6.4),

 we can see

 that

 tw o

 cri tical features w ere

 be-

ing

 explored by the students: name ly, shape an d size. Of part icular interest

is the

 ini t ial com parison

  of the

 sloth's head w ith

 the monkey's

 head

 by one

student

 (line 297), the reject ion of this com parison  by another student ( l ine

298),

  and the just if icat ion of the reject ion by a third ( l ine 299). What was

happening in that section of discourse? Why did the second student say,

"This is not saying anything," m eaning tha t com paring the s loth 's h ead wi th

a m onke y 's head w as not informa t ive at a ll? For the f i rst s tudent , w hat cam e

to

 the fore of his awareness w as the comparison of the sloth with other an i-

mals .

 T he

 dimension

  of

 variation

 w as

 "heads

 o f

 anima ls" (wh ich

 w as

 also

w h a t  the  teacher  had  previously i l lustrated). However,  for the  second  and

th i rd s tudents , w hat cam e to the

 fore

 o f their aw areness w as not to compare

the

  sloth w ith other anim als, as can be seen from line 299. Their subsequent

discourse shows that it w as the shape  that was in their

  focal

  awareness , and

t hus

 they acce pted the analogy of a chicken egg that m ade the shape im m e-

diately

 apparent .

 "Oval" was a

 shape tha t

 w as

 considered

 by the

 group

 to be

a critical feature o f the shape of the sloth 's hea d. The s tuden ts w ere trying to

decide

 w h ich

 w as the

 best a nalogy

 to use to

 describe

 the sloth 's

 head

 a nd i ts

body. The de bate about whe ther i t w as necessary to specify oval when u sing

a chicken

 egg as an

 analogy

  is

 interesting both

 at the

  conceptual level

 and

the  l inguist ic level . Conceptually, oval

  can be a

 feature that

  is

 qu i te inde-

pende nt of an object . For exam ple, a table can be oval , roun d, or square.

Specify ing

  the shape of an object presupposes that there are shapes other

than

  that which

  is

  specified.

  From   the

  discuss ion,

  it is

 clear that

  for

  some

students ,  oval was considered to be an inherent  feature  of a chicken egg,

w h i c h

  rendered

  the use of the

  adjective

 "oval" to

  describe

  the egg

 redun-

dant . This  is a  fairly  sophist icated level of unders tanding that  the  s tudents

w ere able to reach throug h col laborat ive ta lk . Lingu is t ical ly , "chicken egg"

w as

  used to describe  the  shape,  and  "chicken egg" took  on a m ean ing

  that

w as

 synonym ous w i th oval.

T he

  other feature

  is

  size,

  bu t

  here

  we see the

  opposi te happening.

  A l-

though

 there

 are

 chicken egg s that

 are b ig and

 those that

 are

 sm al l,

 th e

 varia-

t ion

  implied by describing the sloth 's body as a "big chicken egg" (as

opposed to "chicken egg"  for its

 head)

 w as considered inadequate because

even  b ig

  chicken eggs were thought

  to be too

  small . Instead,

  the

 s tudents

proposed the

  k ind

  of egg as a dimension of variat ion. Thus they proposed

goose egg an d ostrich egg as a variat ion in size, and finally dec ided on os-

tr ich

 eg g. Here

 w e can see

  three term s

  in the

  students ' semantic system

 of

size:

  chicken eggs , which are smal l ; goose eggs , which are b igger than

ch icken

  eg gs; and o strich eggs, w hich are biggest of al l three. In the final

draf t  tha t the  students produced, they added  the adjective "big"  in front of

ostrich

 eg g to describe the

 size

 of the

 body.

 B y

 doing this ,

 the

 students have

added o ne m ore term to thei r sem ant ic sys tem of s ize to m ake f iner dis tinc-

t ions in

 size.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 163/255

150

  T S U I

Th e collaborative co nstrual of the appe arance of the sloth is evide nt in the

way the

  students questioned each other 's contribution

  and

  then cam e

  to a

consensus   on the  most appropriate descript ion.

Sloth s Eyes.  Le t us now  cons ide r  th e  desc r ip t i on p roduced  by an-

other g roup , Group   4.  When desc r ib ing  th e a p p e a r a n c e o f the s lo th , o n e

s tudent in Group 4  said that the

 sloth's

 eyes were  b lack . However , the

o ther g roup me m bers im m edia t e ly po in t ed ou t t ha t eve rybody knew eyes

were black,  imp ly ing t ha t  this

 piece

 of  i n fo rmat ion  was  supe r f l uous .

How ever , ano the r s tuden t po in ted ou t tha t the eyes o f a rabb i t were spe-

c ia l , and ano the r s t ud en t ch ipped i n and sa id t ha t r ab b i t s ' eyes w ere r ed .

In

 t h i s d i s cus s ion ,

 w e can see

 t ha t i n i t i a l l y

 for th e

  s t u de n t s ,

 a ll

 eyes were

b l a c k  and t he re fo re t he re was on ly one t erm in t he sys t em o f m ea n in g o f

color

 of eyes.

 How ever ,

 as

 soon

 as the

 color

 of

 rabbits' eyes

 was

 b rough t

in , t he mean ing o f t he co lo r o f eyes changed . B l ack was no l onger  a

given, but a

 choice

 in the system of c o l o r— w h i c h at tha t par t icu lar po in t

in

  th e

  d i scou r se cons i s t ed

  o f

 b l a c k

  and no t

  b l a c k ,

  t h a t  is ,

  red .

  In

 o t h e r

w ords , we co uld say tha t the co lor o f the eyes was an aspe ct  t h a t w a s n o t

d i s ce rned

  in i t i a l ly

  (a t  l eas t  b y  s o me  of the  s t u de n t s ) .  I t was  t a k e n  fo r

gran t ed . How ever , w hen

  a n

 a l t e rna t ive

 ey e

 co lo r

 w a s

  e xp l i c i t l y b r o u g h t

u p , a d imens ion  o f var ia t ion in co lor  w as o p e n e d  u p . Di sce rn ing t ha t th e

co lo r o f the s l o th ' s ey es i s b l ac k imp l i e s a d im ens io n o f v a r i a t ion w i t h a t

least

 tw o

 poss ib le value s : b lack

 an d no t

 b lack . De scr ib ing

  th e

 eyes

 of the

s lo th a s b l ack m ean t t ha t co lo r was i n t he  f oc a l  aw aren es s o f one o f t he

s tuden ts . I t w as by

 va r y i n g

 th e

 co lo r

 o f a

 s l o th ' s eyes ( and

 a

 r a b b i t ' s e y e s )

t ha t s t uden t s w ere ab l e

  to

  d i s ce rn t ha t ha v in g b l a ck eyes

  w a s a  c r i t i c a l

fea tu re   o f t he s l o th ' s appea rance .

Sloth s

  Body.

  A l thou gh m ost groups described

  the

 body

 of the

  sloth

 a s

"roundish,"

 p lu m p , o r like  a big ostrich  egg (as Group  2 did), Group  5 de-

scribed the slo th 's body as very thin . Th e fo l lowing excerp t shows the m ean-

ing  that Group  5 a t t ached to th is descr ipt ion.

6 .5 [Chinese Lesson/P3/The Sloth/Group Discuss ion/G roup 5]

S :  Now , i t ' s bod y [shape]

  ...

S: I t 's body [shape] , i t 's body is qui te b ig , huh ?

S:

  Thin

S :

  Thinnish

...

  Looks l ike

 he is

 un dernour i shed.

S:  N o

S:

  Thin

He is

  very thin

S :  H is body shape  ...

S: In   fact,  it was his fur that covered  it up.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 164/255

6.  S E M A N T I C E N R I C H M E N T  O F T H E  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 51

S:  Oh no

S:

  The fur

 covered

  it up

S :  His body shape . . .

S:

  Yeah

S:

  In  fact  he is

  very plump

S:

  He is

 very thin.

S :  Yeah, th ink about th is . Plum p because o f the  fur, his fur covered

up his body.

S:

  Because

 his fur is

  long

This debate wen t on for quite a w hi le and finally the students agreed to put

down  "thin."  What happened here was that this

 group's

  construal of the

sloth's

 appearan ce w as m ade in the con text of the narration that said that the

sloth's mo ther ha d died, that it w as alone, and tha t the leaves on the tree w here

it lived had all been eaten up. For this reason  one of the studen ts said that the

sloth

  was "undernourished." In order to reconcile the discrepancy between

what they   saw on the video  and

 their

 understanding of what his

 appearance

should

 be,

 they cam e

 u p

 w i th

 an

 interesting explana tion: that

 th e

 sloth looke d

plum p only because

  of its

  fur. This suggests that

  the

  meanings that

  are as-

signed by the students to the phenom enon under discussion m ay lead to a di f-

ferent  construal of the object of learning.

Sloth's

  Diet .

  Let us

  take

  a

  look

  at an

  excerpt  from

  the

  discuss ion

  in

Group 4 on the s loth ' s diet . The video narration only provided the inform a-

t ion

  that

 the

  sloth

 ate

 leaves

  from  one

 par t icular t ree:

I t  [ the sloth]  looks around and  tries t o  find  a t ree wh ere i t wo uld  l ike  to  live.  The  tree

should

 be o f the same spec ies as the one that i t

 lived

 on before because wh en a

 sloth

 gets

used

  to the  taste of the leaves of a certain

 k ind

  of tree,  it w i l l  not  change  what i t eats .

6.6

 [C hinese Lesson/The

  S

 loth/PS /Group Discuss ion/Grou p

 4]

S :

  It

 eats

  ... it

 only eats leaves.

S:

  It

 does

  not eat

 anything else,

  it

  only eats leaves.

S:

  Ea ts leaves.

S :

  E a t s le aves , i f . . . i f . . .

S:  Like a big bear,  it's pin sihk

2

 (C antonese) [ $| ^ ] ("a

  fussy

eater").

 Like

 the

 panda ,

  it's

 p in

 sihk.

S :  He y . .. there's m ore

S:

  It ' s

  a.pin sihk

  an imal .

S:  It

 does

 not eat

 me a t

  and . . .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 165/255

1 52  T S U I

S: I t

 does

 not eat

 meat

  and

 does

 not eat fish.

 Does

 it eat fish?

Ss:

  N o

S: N o, i t also says . ..

S :

  When

  it

 gets used

 to the

 taste

 of one

 k ind

 of

 leaf ,

 it

 wi l l

 not

 e a t . . .

S:  That 's  why [ I said]

 ii'spin

  s ihk .

In the or iginal n arrat ion , the narrator ex plain ed that the s loth had to f ind

a

  tree

  of the

  same spec ies because

  of its

  p re fe rence

  for a

  cer tain type

  of

leaves.

  T he

 students

  in

 Group

 4,

 however, ass igned fur ther m ean ing

  to the

s lo th ' s p reference  fo r  l eaves . They used  a  s i n g l e e x p r e s si o n ,

 p in

  sihk

( Ca n t o n e s e )

 [ ̂ ^ ], an

 ad jec t iva l

 ph rase

 for

 de s c r ib i n g s o m e o n e

 who

 is

a   fussy eater, to describe the  eat ing habits  of the sloth.  T he  s tudents were

br ing ing in the i r own exp er ience of be ing to ld by th e i r pare n t s  tha t  they

should eat everything and not be

  fussy

  abou t the i r food  ( a com m on exhor -

tat ion made by parents  to their children ). T he phrase p in  sihk  is at a h ighe r

level of abst ract ion and general izat ion than the descr ipt ion given in the

video. Moreover ,

  the

  s tudents were also able

  to

  re la te

  th e

  s lo th ' s ea t ing

habits

  to the

  eat ing habits

 o f the

  panda, something that they

  had

  learned

about

  in the

 previou s lesson.

 T he phrase

 p in

  sihk

  proposed

  by the

 s t uden t s

has the fol lowing semant ic features : does not eat meat or f i sh , and eats

only one kind  of leaves. There are at least  tw o an im al m embers be longing

to

  th i s ca tegory : panda s

  an d

  s loths .

T he

  question

 is, how

 were

  the

  s tudents able

 to

 co l labora t ive ly cons t ruc t

such  a rich  text, both spoken  and writ ten?  How is i t  that  the  same video

could be const rued in such a sem ant ical ly rich m anne r? How did the teacher

bring this about?

It seems

  that there were  tw o  th ings that  the  teacher  did  that were qu i te

crucial .

 The first has to do

 w i th

 the w ay he

 varied

 the

 showing

 of the

 video

with and

 w i th o u t

 the

 narrat ion.

Variation:  Video With  and   Without  Narration.

  Wh e n

 th e

 teach er showed

the video for the f irs t t ime, the narrat ion w as given. B ut whe n he showed

segments of the video,  he took away  the n arration.  The purpose  of m ak ing

the video soundless w as, as the teacher told the studen ts, tha t if he pla ye d the

narrat ion as we l l , then there w ould be no thing for the s tu dents to talk about .

In

 other

 wo rds,

  th e

 narration

  in the first

 viewing gave

 the

 students

 an

 idea

 o f

w hat the s tory w as abo ut . In a way, one c ould say tha t the narrat ion provided

the

 context

 in

 wh ich

 the

 appearance

  and the

 m o veme n t

 of the

 s lo th cou ld

 be

interpreted. This  is  most evident  in the  description made  by one  group  of

students   w ho described  the sloth 's eyes as look ing l ike tw o  teardrops. Th is

construal  w as  obvious ly made  in  l ight of the  fact  that the  sloth h ad  lost its

mother  and had to  struggle  for survival.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 166/255

6 . S E M A N T I C E N R I C H M E N T

  O F T H E

  S P A CE

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  1 53

For the  second viewing,  the narrat ion  w as removed  in order  to allow the

students  to interpret what they saw in their own ways. By doing this , the

teacher opened

  up the

 space

  of

 learning.

  T he

 students

 were free  to

 bring

  in

their

 o w n

 experience

  and

 previous learning

  to

 make

 sense

  of

 what they

 s aw

on  the  video without being constrained  by wha t  w as said  in the  narration.

In

  other words, what

  the

  teacher varied

  was the

 n arration

  and

 wha t

  he

kept

 co nstant w as the visual im ages . B y so doing, the s tudents becam e very

m u c h  aw are of the lack of narrat ion, an d the  fact  th at they ha d to write the

narra t ion

 them selves .

 In

 order

  to  fulfill  the

 task, they nee ded

  to  focus

  thei r

at tent ion on the

 visua l images , especial ly

 the

 appearance

  and the

 mo ve me n t

of the

  sloth.

Let

 u s im agine that the teacher asked the students to do the same task, bu t

re ta ined the

 narrat ion w hen showing

 the

 video

 th e

 second time. W hat w ould

have happened?  A s the students  had been given  the task  of w riting  a narra-

tion  of the story,  it is likely tha t the structure  of their

 awareness

 would have

changed. What would have come   to the  fore  of  their awareness,  or  wha t

w o u l d

 have been  f igural , w ould be the narrat ion; wha t the narrator actu al ly

said. T he

  actual

 appearance and the m ovem ent of the s loth w ould have re-

ceded

  to the

  ground. What

  the

  teacher w ould very l ikely have achieved

  is

tha t  the  s tudents wou ld have t ried  to  regurgitate  the  narrat ion rather than

c o m i n g up

 w i th thei r

 ow n

  descript ions.

Variat ion:

  Teacher-Led   D iscuss ion  and S tudent -Led   D i scus s ion .

  R e-

searchers have pointed out that wo rking in groups is natu ral ly con ducive to

making

 use of

 variation (see W istedt

 & Martinsson's

 w ork, 1994,

 on

 inexac t

decimal representation

  of the

 fraction  "one third," cited

 in

 M arton

  &

 Booth,

1 997). How ever, this does no t m ean that group work w il l necessarily lead to

successful

  learning. W hether succ essful learning

 is

 achieved

  or not

  depends

on

 whe the r

 the

 teacher

  has

  exploited

  the

 conduciveness

  to

 variation

  in the

group wo rk. Ci t ing the wo rk of Lybeck (1 981) on group w ork ( in wh ich the

t eacher

  conf ron ted

  the

  students with variat ions

  in the

  concept

  o f  densi ty

proposed by other students, and put the variat ions und er scru tiny), M arton

(1986) observed that the teacher "funct ioned as the arch i tect of the peda-

gogical mi l ieu,

  the

  midwi fe

  of

  experience

  and the

  sculptor

  of

  thought"

(cited  in M arton  & B ooth, 1997, p .  69).

In the lesson  on the sloth,  the teacher focused  the studen ts ' at tention on

th e

 cr i t ical features

 o f the

  sloth, such

  as its

 appearance

  (which

  is

  stat ic),

and

 its

 m ovem ents (w hich

  a re

 dynam ic). This teacher-led discus sion pro-

vided the

 sca f fo ld ing

  for the

 subsequen t s tudent - led di scuss ion

 i n

 groups .

The teach er a l so emph as ized the co l labora t ive na ture o f g roup w ork , and

to ld th e

 s tudents tha t

 i t w as

 im por tan t

  fo r

 them

 to pay

 at tent ion

 to the

 con-

t r ibut ions of other group m em bers , to th ink about these con tr ibut ions , and

to

  t ry to im prove on them . These are the ins t ruct ions he gave to the s tu-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 167/255

154

  T S U I

dents  after

 h e had put the

  s truc tura l com pon ents

 o f the

 descr ipt ion

  of the

sloth on the board.

6.7

  [Chinese Lesson/PS/The Sloth]

T:  A fter w atching  th e video,

 each

  group wil l discuss among them-

selves...

 . You

 have

 to

 collaborate. D on ' t jus t write

 it all by

 your-

self.  Y ou mu st work together  and  contr ibute your opinion. You

have to see whether w hat

 another

 member  has said is right or not,

and

 whether

  it is

 good

  or

 not . Af ter

  a

 mem ber

  has  offered  a

  [de-

scriptive] sentence, you can add you r opinion. L ike, "It w ou ld be

better

 to

 w rite

 it

 this

 way

  ...,"

  "It

 w ould

 be better to say it

 that

 way

. . . ," or "No, I disagree, I saw it [the sloth] making this kind of

movement."  and you can give your own account [of what you

saw].

 If  everybody agrees, then y ou  write  it down.

B y  providing  the  students with  a  detailed explanation  of w h a t w as ex-

pected of them w hen they engaged in group w ork, the teacher was opening

up the

 space

  fo r

 variat ion

 in

 discerning

 the

 c r i t ical features

 of the

 s lo th .

 T he

data from the group discussions show tha t each group was able to bring their

ow n experience  to bear on the descript ion, a nd

 different

  d imens ions of vari-

at ion w ere opened  up .

A lthou gh the in i t i a l d i scuss ion w as t eacher- led , the l earn in g space w as

jo in t ly cons t i tu ted by the t eache r and the  s tude nts in the sense tha t th e s tu-

dents offered  descriptions of the  cri t ical features  o f appearance, such  as

the hea d, the body, the color, and the  four  l im bs of the s loth , as w el l as it s

mo ve me n t s .

T he teacher also  provided scaffolding  for the  group discussions  by re-

m inding s tudents w hich aspects they should

 be

 focus ing

  on in the

 course

 o f

the

 discuss ion.

 F o r

 exam ple,

 he

 reminded

 the

 s tudents tha t they shou ld have

finished discussing  the appearance  and should be m oving on to the descrip-

t ion o f m ovem ent . A s the s tudents w ere discu ss ing the movem ents , he re-

m inded them of the various m ovem ents they should be descr ib ing, such as

sw im m ing, crawling up the r iver bank, c l imb ing up the t ree, and so on.

Table 6 .1 summarizes the contr ibut ions made by the s tudents in the

teacher-led discussion

  and the

  student-led discussion

  in

 four  groups

  out of

six.

  T he

  features

  in the

  s tudent- led discuss ions that were

  no t

  found

  in

teacher-led discussion s are in i tal ics.

When  the  groups had finished  drafting their

 descriptions,

 the  teacher

asked each group

  to

 present their descript ions

  as the

 na rrat ion

 as he

 p layed

the video again. Here the teacher was   further  w idening the space of varia-

tion

 b y

 gett ing students

 to

 listen

 to the

 descriptions w ritten

 by

 other grou ps.

T he different

  descript ions showed that

 different

  aspects

 of the

 sloth were

 in

th e

  focal awareness

 of

  each group.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 168/255

TABLE   6. 1

D escr i p t i on o f t he S lo t h i n Teacher-Led an d S t u d en t -Led D i scu ss i on s

Teacher-Led   Discussion

T

M o v e m e n t

Appearance

Colo r

Head:

  l ike

m o n k e y ' s

head

Ss

Slow,  s lowly

 c l i m b i n g

up  a tree.

Furry

B r o w n ,

 w h i t e

Head very ro und, very

s mal l ,

  neck

  is

 very

long,  l i m b s

 ar e

 very

long

Student-Led Discussion

Gro up

  1

Mouth  (beak)

—  very

  f lat,

like  a  duck,

very   shor t

Gro up

  2

Very  short tai l

Grayish, whi t ish,

brownish and whit ish,

black, brown

Looks  l ike  a m o n k e y ' s

head ,  ch icken egg ,

  b ig

goose

  egg , b ig duck egg ,

big ostrich egg

Gro up

  4

Nose: b lack ,  body:  black,

eyes:  black

  (a s

 op p osed

to red) , mouth: f lat ,

protruding,

  b lun t

Head

  is l ike a

 m o n k e y 's ,

head can  turn  36 0 degrees

Gro up 5

Face: whi te, eyebrows:

brown, eyes: triangular,

l i ke m onkey

  's

  eyes , body:

l ike a

  hum an be ing , very

small , only   half  a year

old,  like

  th e

  thigh

  of  a

baby, l ike  a b ig  winter

m elon

R o u n d ,

  ova l

tn

in

(cont inued on next page)

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 169/255

l/l

Os

TABLE   6. 1  (cont inued)

Teacher-Led Discussion

T

Four

 l i m b s ,

f inge r s ,

  p a w s

M o v e m e n t :

c r a w l i n g

S w i m m i n g

Diet

&

Paws, c laws, three

s harp

 c l a w s

V e r y

 s l o w , s lowly

c l i m b s

  u p a tree

l o o k i n g

 a r o u n d

Student-Led Discussion

Gro up  I Gro up  2

Very  l o n g ,

  especially

  th e

claws  of

  the for

 climbs,

th ree c laws

V e r y  s l o w , s l o w l y c l i m b s

d o w n

  the tree ,

  exhausted,

l o o k i n g

  a r o u n d

 a s

  soon

  as

i t  r e a c h e d

 t he

  b a n k ,

c l i m b e d

  up the

  t r ee ,

 it

f i na l l y

  fo u n d  a new  h o m e

Free s ty le

Gro up

  4

H a n d s

 an d

  legs

 ar e

  very

l ong ,  nai l s are very

 pointed

(sharp) ,   four  l imbs are very

l o n g  an d  sma l l ,  claws  ar e

very  long and razor sharp

Puppy

  style, frog style

(breast stroke), free style

O nly

  eats leaves, l ike

  th e

panda,  it is pin

  sihk  (fussy

eater) , does no t ea t meat or

fish

Gro up 5

Eats on ly leaves, any k ind

of

  leaves that  it  likes.

Note .  T he  fea tures  in the s tuden t - led d i s c u s s i o n s  tha t

  w ere

 n ot  f o u n d  in  teache r - led

  d i s c u s s i o n s

  are given in  i t a l i c s .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 170/255

6.

  S E M A N T I C E N R I C H M E N T

  OF T H E  SPACE  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 57

L I N G U I S T I C

  RESOURCES AN D

  SEMANTIC

  E N R I C H M E N T

In th is sect ion, we look at the w ay in w h i ch the same object of learning w as

construed semantically in very

 different

 ways in tw o classroom s because of

the

  different  l ingu ist ic resources avai lable

 to the

 teacher

  and the

 learners,

that is , one of the classes w as taught through the medium o f Chinese, w hich

w as the  mother tongue for al l students (CM I), and the other through the me-

dium

  of Eng lish, wh ich wa s a second language for all students (EM I).

The  history lessons used  as  examples  in  this section  further  i l lustrate

w h a t  w e  mean  by the

  seman t ic d imens ion

  of the  learning space. (For  the

background   of

 these lessons,

  see

 Tsui , Aldred, M arton, Kan ,

 &

 Runesson ,

2001.)

  The

  extracts

  are f rom two

  secondary Grade

  2

  (S2) (i.e., Grade

  8)

c lass room s in w h i c h the sam e topic w as taught , and il lustra te the

  difference

in the sem antic space opened up because of the different  linguistic resources

avai lable  to the s tudents .

H i s to r y L e s so n s :  T h e  M a r k e t  in  Early

  Rural

  C o m m u n i t i e s

In these tw o history lessons, the objec t of learning was the concept o f

  m ar -

ket

 in early rural com m unities in Hong Kong. The critical features that dis-

t ingu ish

  this market (which perhaps t ranslates more accurately as  "fair")

from

  the market in modern societies, are the differen t func tions. In i ts his-

tor ical

  contex t ,

 th e

 m arke t

 was a

 p lace w here people would come both

  to

buy produce  and to sell their ow n produce,  or  sometimes even to  exchange

goods. The m arket w as also a p lace for social in teract ion, and w as loca ted

in  a

 particular place that

 w as

 accessible

  to

 man y villagers

 and

 clans. How -

ever, due to poor t ransportat ion markets w ere no t h eld every day but at r egu-

lar intervals.

The

  fo l lowing

  is an

  excerpt

  from

  the EMI

  lesson

  in

 w h ic h

  th e

  teacher

tried to elicit  from  the students the critical fe atures of the m arket in its his-

tor ical

 con tex t .

6.8

  [History Lesson/S2/EM I]

1

  T : Do you think th e m arket  at that time is the same as you[ V e ] go t n o w?

2

  Ss: No.

3 T: How

  [was

  it]

 d i f fe ren t?

4 Ss:  [silent]

5 T:

  H u h ?

6 Ss:  [silent]

7 T: How [was it] different? Can you imagine? In the past,

 market

 [writing

8 the  word "market" on the board]. Y ou[ Ve] got a m arket, there,

 isn't

 it?

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 171/255

158

  T S U I

9 Is it a market there? [Points to the market next to the school. ] Have you

10

  been there?

11  S:

  Yes.

12  T:

  Yes.

 But in the

 past

 we

 call[ed]

 it

 heui. (Cantonese)

  [ ̂ ]

 [Writes

 the

13 Chinese

 character heui (Cantonese) [J^]

  on the

 blackboard.] Chan

14  heui.

 (Cantonese) [ ̂    ̂ ] And then at hat time people going to

15  market, we call[ed] it chan heui.

16 Ss:  [laughter]

17

  T:  [writes the words chan heui (Cantonese) [ ̂   4sj|

 ]on

 the blackboard]

18 And then what does it mean? At that time the people—you  find out that

19 for the

 market ...?

20 S:  [silent]

21 T: The market opens every day? Is it? There? [Pointing at the market outside

22 the

 window]

23 Ss:  Yes.

24 T: But at that time the market [did] not open every day. They

 w i ll

 fix some

2 5 day, for example,

 for

 each month, [on the] fifteen[th] and thirtieth], they

2 6 will have [the] market. And then people go there, and then to sell...

 w h a t

2 7

  they produce.

 For

 example,

 you

 look

 at

 him,

 he

 go[es] there

 to

 sell

 his

 pigs

2 8 and cocks, and at the same time by making money he can buy something

29

  that

 he d id not

 produce

 by

 himself. Understand?

 So i t is

 dif ferent

  from

3 0 your market, isn't it? And at the same time, at

 that

 time the market has a

31

  function. First

 of

 all,

 it is

 selling

 and at

 same

 t ime you buy

 something.

 A nd

32

  also

 you  f ind out

 that,

 in the

 second paragraph, what

 did he

 say?

33 [referring to the diary of Tang Tai Man.]

34 Ss:  [silent]

35 T:

  What

 did he

 say?

36 Ss:

  [silent]

37 T:  What did he say?

38 Ss:

  [silent]

39 T: Huh? [Nominate], what did he say in the second paragraph?

45 S:  [reading from the second paragraph of the diary of Tang Tai Man]

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 172/255

6.  S E M A N T I C  E N R I C H M E N T  O F T H E S P A C E O F  L E A R N I N G  1 59

46

  "Today

 I

 also

 met my  f r iends

  coming

  f rom another village, we

47

  chat[ted]."

48 T:  Okay,

 okay,

 so what

 did,

 what

 did he do in the

 second paragraph?

49 S:  [silent]

50 T:

  What

 is happening to

 him?

51 S: He met his  f r iends.

52 T: He met his

  f r iends.

 Okay, sit

 down.

 You

 f ound

 out

 tha t then,

 for the

5 3  market at that time, it also provides a chance for them to meet their

5 4

  f r i ends . . . .

 You

 found

 out

 that

 at

 that

 time [there

 was]

 no

 transport,

 you

5 5 can

 only

  ?

56 S:

  Walk.

57 T:

  Walk.

 Then sometimes it is

 difficult

 for you to find

 your

 friends, is it? So

58 the market provide[d] a place for them to

 meet

 their friends.... You think

59

  about now,

 is it

 very easy

 for you to see

 your friends?

60 T: ... So you find out that here this is the function of the m arket [referring to

61 the

 handou t ]

  ...

Here the t eacher w as t rying to br ing ou t the difference s be tween the mar -

ket in the  past an d the m arket  in the present,  and its funct ions in early ru ral

c o m m u n i t i e s

 in Hong Kong . S he started w ith a n open quest ion of how the

m arket in the old days w as different from  the m arket nowadays (see l ine 3,

"How [was

 it] different?").

  W h e n

  she failed  to get an

 answer ,

 s he

 na r rowed

down the question to specific features. First , she tried to draw on the stu-

dents ' m other tongue resources by invoking the exp ress ion chan heui ( Ca n -

tonese) [^^],

  an

  expression that

 all

  students were familiar with

 as

 evi-

denced by the l aughter . S he asked them to extrapolate one of the fea tures of

the m arket  from

  the

 m eaning

  of

  this expression. (The word  chan  (Canton-

ese) [^ ] m e a ns to take the opportunity, and he word

 heui

 (Cantonese) [ ̂ ]

w as

 used

 by the

 teacher

 as an

 equivalent

 of the

 market

 in the

 past . T herefore,

chan  heui imp l ies tha t the market was not open every day, and t ha t w hen it

w as, people took the oppo rtunity  to get what they wanted from  the m arket .)

This

  t e c h n i qu e

 did n ot

 m ee t w i th m uch succes s because unpack ing

 a

 Ch i -

nese expression   in E ngl ish was far too  difficult  for the  students. Failing  to

get a response , the teacher s im ply pointed o u t tha t un l ike the m arke t nex t to

their  school, m arkets in the old days were not open every day.

Af ter  dea l ing wi th  the first  cr i t ical feature, the  t eache r w en t  on in the

same turn

 to

 provide

  the

 second critical feature

 of the

 m arket: U nl ike today,

peop le w e n t

 to the

 m arket

 n o t

 only

 to

 sel l their pro duc e

 or to buy

 som eth ing

they  needed, but to do both at once. From lines 32 to 58, the teach er tried to

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 173/255

160

  T S U I

elicit

 a

 third critical fea ture

 of the

 m arket

 f rom the

 s tudents ,

 by

 ask ing them

to

 look

 at the  second paragraph  of the diary that she had been referr ing to

earlier. One of the s tudents , after  s t ruggl ing w i th th e t each er ' s ins t ruc t ions ,

sim ply read

 out the

  second paragraph instead

 of

 inferring

 from

 th e

 t ex t w hat

the th i rd cr i t ical feature wa s, as the  t eacher h ad w a n t e d. A l th o u g h the stu-

dent  w as able  to provide  an  appropriate answer

  after

  several prompts  from

the teache r (see l ine 51 ) , the conn ect ion be tw een w hat appeared  in the t ex t

and the  third cri t ical feature of the m arket ( i .e ., that the m arke t p rov ided a

place for people to meet thei r f r iends) had to be m ade by the teache r . The

connect ion between   the  funct ion  of the  ma r k e t  fo r  soc ia l in terac t ion and

poor t ransp ortat ion w as also made by the teache r .

W e

 can see in the

 ju s t c i ted excerpt that

 th e

 teach er tried

 to

 focus

 the

 s tu-

dents ' at tention  on the cr i t ica l fea tures of the m a r ke t in old r u ra l c o m m u n i -

ties

  by

  varying

  the

  contex t s

 of

 pas t

  and

 p resen t . However , because

  of the

s tudents ' l im i ted l in gu is t ic resources   in En gl ish , the  teache r h ad p rob lem s

engaging   the  s tudents in m ak ing s ense  of the  contex t s in w h ich t hese f ea -

tures were to be  understood.

Let us  compare this lesson wi th  an  excerpt below  f rom a CMI  lesson,

where the  same concepts were being explicated.

6.9

  [History Lesson/S2/CM I]

[The teacher asks the students to   identify  on the m ap place s that con-

tain the word

  heui

 (C antonese)  [

 ̂  ]

  [standing for "market."] The n he

con t inues to ask the s tudents the

  funct ions

  of a "market."]

1 T :  Nex t  I wan t to ask you , what are the funct ions o f a heui?

2  S :

  [cheeki ly]

 N o

  use .

3 T :

  W h a t? W h a t

 are the funct ions

 of

  heui . H as

 anybody heard

 the

4  saying "Three, w ha t , make  a

 heui?"

5  Ss:  Three w om en

6 T :  Wha t does that m ean? N ow w e a re not discriminating against fe-

7  ma l e

 students.

 W e are not saying that female

 students

 are talkative. So

8  what is the function  [of a

 heui]?

 In fact, how can three people m ake

9 a

 heui? W h at

  is the

  characterist ic

 of a

 heui?

10

  S:

  V ery noisv.

11   T :

  V ery noisy. M aybe

 w e

  should

 not use

  [ the w ord] noisy.

12

  S:

  Busy .

13  T :  Busy A lot of  fun . Other than this? W hat else?

14

  S :  Lots of  th ings for  sale.

15

  T :

  W h a t  funct ion  does

  th e

 m arket have?

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 174/255

6 . S E M A N T I C E N R I C H M E N T O F T HE

  S P A C E

  O F

  L E A R N I N G  _

16  S:  Transact ions .

17

  Co nducting transactions, that's right. W hat kinds of things

 for

 us to

18  T:  buy?  In earlier

 societies?

19  S:  Jewelry.

20  T: Jewelry . A nything else?

21

22  T :  That is, the market has the

 function

 of [conducting] transactions.

23  Where  is it?

24 S:  "Open  air

  n ightclub"

3

25

  T :

  "Open

 air

 nightclub

 ."That's

 right . U sua lly

 it is on a

 piece

 of

 flat

2 6  land. W hereabouts? Is it near the village? Inside th e village ? N o.

27 Usual ly   . . . c an you

 imagine?

28 S:  N e a r  the vi l lage s .

29

  T :

  H m m ,

 any

  other suggestions?

30

  S:  Where there are  lots of people.

31   T: W here there are lo ts of peo ple. An y other suggest ions?

32   S :

  W here lo ts

 of

 people

  can get to it.

33   T:  W here lots of people can get to it. Is it a

 place

 only where people

34

  w i th the surname "Tang" can go?

35

  S: No.

36 T: The place is a piece of flat land where u sually other villagers can get

37 to as w ell . That is where th e m arket was loca ted. A lso, in the old

3

 8 days, there w ere no buses, no subw ay, so it w asn 't so conven ient.

39  So w ou ld there be a m arket every day? W ould people go to

40 the

 m arket every day?

41 S: No.

42 T:  That seldom happen ed. U sua lly there was a

 fixed

 t im e, usual ly

43

  several t im es a m on th, or once a w eek. B esides being a place

44 for  co nd uc ting transactions, w hat other function  did it have?

45

  Jus t

 n o w w e

 said that three w om en m ake

 a

 m arket,

 very

 busy,

46

  [people] cha t t ing

 to

 each other . W hat funct ion

  [of the

 m arket]

47 can you

 deduce

  from this? A h,

  "You come  from

  the

 nearby

48 vil lage , how is so and so?"

49 S: So cial interaction activit ies.

50   T :  W hat kind  of funct ion did it have?

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 175/255

1 6 2  TSUI

51   S :  Social

  interaction activities.

52

  T:  That's right. Yo u are

 correct.

 It

 actually

 had a

 kind

 of

 interactive

5

 3

  function,

 the

 interaction between villages, am ong people

 from

54 differen t clans.

W e

  can see from  this excerpt that  in the CMI  c lassroom,  the lea rn ing

space in wh ich the features of the market w as understood w as jo in t ly con s t i-

tuted by the

 teacher

 and the  students.  I t was

 also

 semant ical ly m uch richer

than that const i tuted in the E M I c lassroom.

Firstly,

 by a l luding to the C hinese co l loquia l expression, "Three w o m e n

make

 a

 m arket,"

  the

 teacher

 w as

 im m ediate ly able

 to g et the

 students

 to as-

sociate

  th e expression

  wi th

 "very noisy"  and "busy"

  (l ines

  10 and

  12).

W hen he asked the s tudents w here the market w ou ld no rm ally be located,

they used the ana logy "open-air n igh tclu b" (l ine 24). (The

 "open

 a ir n igh t -

club"  was a  large , w el l-kno w n, open-air nig ht m arket  in Hong Kong , that

besides

 having

  a

 variety

  of

  things

  for

  sale, also

  had

  entertainment shows,

fortune-telling,

  and

 haw ker stalls. People w ent there

 no t

 j u s t

 to buy and

 sell

things, but

 also

 to

 have

 a

 good t im e.)

 The

 teacher p icked

 up on the

 s tuden t 's

analogy (l ine 25) and  further  developed  the  topic by  involv ing s tudents in

exploring the locat ions of markets . Again draw ing on s tudents ' kn ow ledge

of the expression, "three

 women m ake

 a

 m arket,"

 th e

 teach er solicited

 from

the  students that another cri t ical feature of the  m arket  was its  funct ion  for

social

  interaction (l ine 49).

Here  we can see that the rich l ing uistic resources of the m other tongu e en-

abled

 th e

 teacher

 and the

 students

  to

 collaboratively construe

 a rich

 picture

 of

the

 m arket w ith

 a

 n u m b e r

 of

 seman t ic features:

 for

 exam ple , noisy, busy, joy -

ous; w ith lots of activities going on; buying and sell ing; people from

 different

villages and

  clans meeting

  up and

  making

  friends;

  people performing

 and

watching entertainment shows. Going

 to the

 m arket

 (chan heui) was a

 social

event that

 took

 place at regu lar intervals rather than every day because of the

lack  o f transp ortation. W e can also see that the s tudents ' understanding of the

market

 w as

 m uch m ore sophist ica ted

 due to the use of

 their m other tongu e.

For example, the concept of

  social interact ion

 is m uch m ore sophistica ted

than jus t

 "meeting

  friends" and

 "entertainment,"

 and the concept  of  t ransac-

tion  is mu ch m ore sophisticated than "buying and sell ing."

The

  fo l lowing tab le sum m arizes

 the

  semant ic features

 of the

 m a rk et

 in

the two   lessons (see Table 6.2).

C O N C L U S I O N

In

  this chapter,  w e  have looked  at  another very important aspect  of the

learning  space, that

 is , the wa y in

 w hich

 the

 object

 of

 learning

  (in the sense

of the l ived object of learning) is construed by the learners when they are

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 176/255

6 .  S E M A N T I C E N R I C H M E N T O F T H E  S P A C E  O F

  L E A R N I N G

1 6 3

TABLE   6.2

Features

  of the

 M a r ket

  in C M I a n d E M I

 Lessons

EM I

  Lesson

Funct ions  o f  marke t

  S e l l i ng  as

 w e l l

  a s

  b u y i n g

•  M e e t in g  f r iends  ( b e c a u s e o f poor

t r anspor t a t ion)

Opening

N o t  open

 da i ly

  ( n o r ea sons

 g iven)

Funct ions  o f  marke t

  Tran sac t ions ( f ood  and  o t h e r

 t h i ngs )

•  Soc ia l  in teract ion  be tween v i l lage s and  c l ans .

Imagery

" T h re e w o m e n m a k e

 a

 m a r k e t"

 ( to

 d ed uce

func t ions )—noisy ,

  busy ,

 a lot of

  activit ies,

 j o y o u s

socia l  in teract ion .

Analogy

"Open   a ir

  n ig h t c l u b"— bu y in g

  and

  se l l i ng , f ood ,

en t e r t a i n m en t  show s , hav ing a good tim e .

Locat ion

Access ib le to peop le  from

  d i f fe rent

  clans and

vi l l ages .

Opening

N ot  open

 da i ly ;

 poor t r anspor t a t ion , no t

 e n o u g h

goods  and  p rod uce for sa le .

able to bring in their ow n experiences, their ow n culture, their fo rm er learn-

ing,

 fo lk lore ,

 and so

 for th .

 W e

 referred

 to

 this aspect

 o f the

 learning sp ace

 as

th e

 sem antic

 dim ension

  of the

 space

 o f learning, a nd

 proposed that this

 s e-

mant i c

 dimens ion

 is

 elas t ic, that

 is ,

 that

 it can be

 enriched

 or

 impoverished.

W e

  i l lus t rated

  how the

  teacher enriched

  th e

  semant ic dimension

  of the

space of learning by help ing learners to perceive the interre lat ionsh ip of as-

pec ts of the pheno m enon that were hi therto perceived as unrelated. B y in-

t roducing

 n ew

 semant ic dom ains

 or

 d ifferen t levels

 of

 knowledge

  in

 w h ich

the

 phenomenon

  can be

 con st rued,

  the

 teacher enriched

  the

 learning space

semantical ly.  W e  have  also  illustrated  how the  learners themselves  en-

r iched the learning space sem ant ical ly by br inging their ow n experiences to

bear o n the object of learning, and how they can thus col laborat ively con-

struct and ass ign new m eanings to the object of learning. Th e w ay in w hich

the

  semant ic dimension of the learning space is enriched and thus helps

learners exp erience

  the

 object

 of

 learning

 in a

 certain

 w ay in

 each lesson

  is

an instan tiat ion that wil l eventually lead to the en larging o f the sem antic po-

tential  in w hich the object  of learning is being construed. Finally,  by  com-

paring

  the

  di f ferences

 in

  semant ic

  richness of

  const rual

  of the

  object

  of

l ea r n ing—in

 c lasses w here learners have the necessary l ingu is t ic resources

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 177/255

164  T S U I

(mother tongue

 medium), and

 classes

 in which they do not (second  lan-

guage

 medium)—we have illustrated the way in which language plays a

central

 role

 in the semantic enrichment of the space of learning.

E N D N O T E S

'The writ ten texts produced by the s tuden ts were in M odern S tandard Chinese .

The translations of the texts presented here are semantic translations  but the  syn-

tact ic s t ructures of the texts have been re ta in ed as far as poss ib le .

2

In

  th is volume, characters  and  words spoken  in C a n t o n e s e a re  t ranscr ibed  in

the  Yale system, w h ich is

 used

 by some Can tonese-learning m ater ia ls produced  by

the Ya le U nivers i ty , and by The U nivers i ty o f Hong K ong in it s C an ton ese-En g-

lish,

 English-Cantonese

 dic t iona r ies . (N ote that diacr i t ics that are used to indic ate

Cantonese

 tones have been

 left

  out . These diacri t ics are very

 di ff icul t ,

  if at all pos-

s ible , to produce us ing Word, and some research repor ts us ing the   Yale  sy s t em

simply

  do not

  include

  the

 diacr i t ics .)

3

"Open

 a ir nightclub" is the name given  to a  large , wel l-known, open-air n ight

m a rke t  in  Hong Kong .  It does  no t  ex i s t anym ore  in its o r i g ina l f o rm .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 178/255

 

T h e   S h a r e d S p a c e   o f   e a r n i n g

A m y B . M .

  T s u i

M ar to n an d B o o th ( 1997) ex p o u n ded th e qu a l i t a t ive ly

  d i f ferent

  w a y s i n

w h i c h

  lea rn ing  c a n b e  exper ienced :  T h e  learner  m a y  f o cu s l a rg e ly  on the

situation in

 w h i ch

 the

 p h e n o m e n o n

  is

 e m b e d de d ,

 or on the

 phenomenon

 as

it

  is revea led in the s i tuat io n ( see p . 83) . The y o bserved tha t the asp ec ts of

the

 p h e n o m e n o n

  and the

 re l a t i o n sh ip s be tw een th em th a t

 a re

 d iscerned

 a n d

he ld  in aw ar en es s s imu l t an eo u s ly de t e r min e th e w ay th e p h en o men o n i s

ex p e r i en ced

 by the

 in d iv idua l . Therefo re

  th e

 s am e p h e n o m e n o n

  m a y b e e x -

p e r i en ced in qu a l i t a t ive ly  d i f ferent  w ay s by in d iv idu a l s becau se th e a sp ec t s

and the r e la t ions h ips tha t they d iscern m ay be   di f fe rent ,  and w hat i s he ld in

a w a r e n e s s s i m u l t a n e o u s l y

 m a y

  a lso

  b e

  di f fe rent .  Wh en th i s h ap p en s ,

  th e

l ived o b jec t o f lea rn ing w i l l be

 d i f ferent

  from the enac ted ob je c t o f

 l e a r n i n g .

To b r in g abo u t  s uc c e s s f u l  lea rn ing , it i s necessary tha t the teac her and the

learner share

  a

  l a rg e co mm o n g r o u n d

  in

  r e la t ion

  to the

  o b j ec t

  o f

 l e a r n i n g .

T h e

 task befo re

 th e

 teache r , therefo re ,

  is

 th reefo ld . F i r s t ,

 th e

 t each e r

  s h o u l d

en su r e th a t

 the

 co n d i t i o n s

 a re

 there

  for the

 lea rner

  to be

  ab le

 to

 d iscern

 an d

s i m u l t a n e o u s l y ho ld in awa reness the c r i t i ca l aspec ts o f the ob jec t o f lea rn -

i n g ,  and the  r e la t ionsh ips be tween these aspec ts . Secondly ,  th e  t e a c h e r

sh o u l d

  b e

  aware

  of the

  lea rner ' s exper ience

  of the

 o b j e c t

  o f

  l ea r n in g ( s ee

a lso Ale xan dersso n , 1994), and be v ig i lan t o f s igna ls from learner s

  i nd i c a t -

ing  a

  lack

 o f

 co m m o n g r o u n d . Th i r dly ,

  th e

  t each e r sh o u ld

 t ry to

 w i de n

  th e

s h a r e d c o m m o n g r o u n d .

 These

  th r ee ta sk s can n o t

  b e

  ach ieved in dep en d-

e n t l y

  o f each o ther ; they a re in te r twined .

W hat does a l l th i s m ean to the p rac t i tioner , and how

r

 i s th i s th reefo ld t a sk

r ea l ized

  in

  c lass room discourse? Befo re

  w e

  p roceed

  to

  address th i s que s-

t ion , w e

 n eed

  to

 h ave so me u n de r s t an d in g

 o f

 w h a t

  is

 in vo lved w h en

  t wo o r

m o r e p eo p le

  a re

 en g ag ed

  in a

 d iscourse

  o f

  some sor t .

1 6 5

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 179/255

166

  T S U I

D I S C O U R S E   A N D  C O M M O N  G R O U N D

When

  two or

  more people engage

  in a

  discourse

  o f

  some sort , they bring

w ith them   a set of assum ptions that they bel ieve are  shared between them .

There

  are cer tain assumptions that per tain to communicat ion in general .

For exam ple, tha t these people speak  the same langua ge, that they a re aware

of and  observe s im i lar convent ions regarding discourse rules ,  tha t ne i ther

of them have speech and/or hearing im pairm ents, and so on. These assum p-

tions pertain  to the channel  o f com m unica t ion  and not to a  specific  ut ter-

ance under discussion.

 There a re

 a l so assu m pt ions tha t

 are

 more  specif ic

  to

the com m unicat ive s i tuat ion. For exam ple, w hen w e talk to a co l league , we

assume that they have shared knowledge about people

  and

  events

  in the

workplace that a re relevant to the day- to-day op erat ion, the l ines of respon-

s ib i li ty , and so on. However, e xac t ly how m uc h kno w ledge we share is not

known unt i l

  w e

  actu al ly enter in to

  a

 di scourse .

  Le t us

  take

  the

  fol lowing

piece

  o f

  conversa t ion—which

  is

  fabricated

 bu t

  w h i ch typ ica l ly  occurs—

between two col leagues , A and B, for exam ple:

7.1

1

  A: So the

  meeting's been changed

  to

 Friday?

2  B :  W hat m eet ing?

3 A: The   departmental meet ing.

4 B : Oh, I

 thought

  i t w as

  canceled.

5  A : A pparent ly not .

6 B: I don ' t know.  A sk R ebecca.  S he would know.

7  A: OK.  Thanks.

When  A asked B

 w hether

 the m eeting  had

 been

 changed  to  Friday (line

1 ), A had cer tain assum ptions . A assumed that , f irs t, B knew w ha t m eet ing

A wa s

 referring

  to ;

  second,

  B

  knew that

  the

  date

  of the

 m ee t ing

  had

  been

changed; third, i t was l ikely that B would be able to provide the answer.

However, A c ould not be abso lutely sure that these assum ptions were shared

unti l

 B

 responded.

  B 's

  response (l ine

 2)

 indica ted that

 A 's

 assump tion

 w as

not shared, and

 that

 B did not

 know w hich sp ecific m eeting

  A w a s

 referring

to. Bruner (1987) pointed out that reference typically "plays upon the

shared presupp osit ions and the shared con texts of speakers" (p. 87). A 's ref -

erence  to the  meet ing involved mapping  A 's

  subjective

  sphere onto B's .

Upon hearing B's question, "What meeting?"  ( l ine 2) , A real ized  that  th is

f irst assum ption wa s in

  fact

  not shared by B. A then im m ediately  clarified

th e referent, that is, "the

  meeting,"

 by explici tly identifyin g it as  "the depart-

menta l

 meeting"

 (line 3). B's  response  to A's clarif icat ion  ( l ine 4 )  showed

that A's second assumption that the meet ing had been changed was not

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 180/255

7.

  T H E  S H A R E D S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 67

shared, an d A 's subsequent response showed that B 's assumption that it had

been canceled

 w as no t

  shared

  either

 (line

  5).

 Finally,

 B 's response

  showed

that A's

  first

  assumption ( that B might know the answer) was not shared

( l ine

  6) . B directed A to another col league, R ebecca. B 's assumption w as

that A

 knew

 w ho

 Rebecca w as ,

 and

 that there

 w as a reason for

 directing

 the

question

 to

 Rebecca .

 This

 assum ption

 w as

 indeed shared,

 an d A

 thanked

 B

fo r  the  redirection ( l ine  7).

A t  the end of the

 exchange ,

  the

  comm on ground be tween

  A and B had

widened .

 There  w as  more shared knowledge between them  now  than  be-

fore. An y kind of discourse is a process of w idening the comm on ground be-

tween part ic ipants ,

 and

 this

 is one of the

 m ajor m ot iva t ions

 fo r

 people

 to

 talk

to

 each other. Discourse  is therefore a

 process

 in which m eanings are nego-

t iated  and disam biguated, as w el l as a process in w hich comm on grounds

are

 e s tabl ished

 and

 w idened (see Tsui , 1994).

CLASSROOM

  D ISCOURSE

AN D   E S T A B L I S H I N G

  C O M M O N  G RO U N D

In classroom discourse, there are also cer tain shared assum ptions betw een

the

 t eacher

 and the

 students.

 For

 exam ple, there

 are

 shared assum ptions that

students

  should

  fol low  the

  teacher ' s ins t ruct ion, that they should answer

que stions wh en c alled on to do so, and so on. These pertain to the general

classroom protocols that

 differ  from

  cul ture

 to

 cu l ture; cul ture

 in the

  sense

of ethnic cul ture

 as

 w e l l

 as

 social class culture .

 For

 example,

  in

 m any A s ian

classroom s, the protocol requires that s tudents s tand up when they answ er

the

 teache r ' s quest ions because

 the

 t eacher

 is a

 f igure

 o f

 authori ty.

 In

 class-

rooms where middle-class cul ture predominates , working-class chi ldren

are  ser iously disadvantaged because they do not share the same assump-

t ions as

 m iddle-class chi ldren. Classroom social izat ion

 a nd

 a ccu l tu ra t ion

are

 im portant aspects

 o f

 classroom learning,

 but in

 th i s vo lum e,

 we a re not

par t icular ly  concerned wi th the  shared common ground  in this respect . W e

are

 more concerned wi th

 the

 aspects

 of

 comm on ground tha t

 are

 pert inent

 to

the

  object

  of

  learning.

There are also assum ptions that the teacher an d the learners hold that are

specific   to the

  ob jec t

 of

  learning.

 It is not

  u n c o mmo n

  for the

  teacher

  to be

w orkin g on certain assum ptions that are not shared by the students, or vice

versa .

 W hen this happen s , the abi l ity of the teacher to negot iate and w iden

the

 comm on ground be tween h imsel f

 an d the

 students

 is

 cri t ical

 to

  effective

learning.

Let us

 take

 as an

 example some data

 from  a

 pr im ary Grade

 4

 (P4) Eng l ish

lesson on "The W eekly Plan." (For the backgroun d of this lesson a nd other

asp ects of this lesson, see Lo & Ko, 2002.). Let us cal l this class, C lass A.

The intende d object of learning in this lesson is the use of the sim ple present

tense to indicate hab itual act ion. Th e teacher, Teacher A , presented a diary

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 181/255

168  T S U I

of Mickey Mouse tha t shows tha t he  does

  d i f ferent

  act ivi t ies on  different

days of the week . H e also does some  of the act ivi t ies fo r m ore than one day.

After

 get t ing the s tudents to focu s on "How

  of ten

  does he do (ac tivity)?," the

teacher wanted to

  focus

  on the tense used.

7.2 [English Lesson/P4A/Weekly Plan]

30 T: . . . he

  goes

  to the

  l ib ra ry only one ,

  one

 t im e ,

 O K ? O n e

 t im e ,

31   OK? So we say once a week . Bu t why  [do]  w e add  "-es"  at

32

  the end of the word "go?" [Teacher po int in g at the  "-es"

33  ending  on the bo ard.]  Can you  tell  m e? [S s pu t up their

34

  han ds.] Yes?

35

  S 8:  B e c a u s e he,  she,  it.

36 T:

  Yes,

 h e.

  she,

  it. but

 w h a t

  is

 l inked

 to the

  tense?

 T he

  verb

  ...

37 the form of the verb? Sam uel?

38 S9:  Present tense.

39 T: Present tense . W hat kin d of present tense? Sim ple—

40 Ss:  Sim ple present tense .

41 T : W hy [do we   use] "simple present tense?" W h y?  W h y do

42 you   think  it is  about simple present  tense? B ecause  it is

43  abou t  _?

44 S1 0: [sit t ing in her seat] He do  af ter  school .

45 T: [po inting at S1 0] Yes?

46   S10: B ecause  it is about  ... he do

  af ter

  school .

47 T:

  Yeah ,

  it is

 abou t

  the

  th ings ,

 OK, he ,

 M i ck e y M o u s e ,

 he

48 M ickey M ouse, do [does] the thin gs  af ter  school . W e are

49 te l l ing people about h is hab i t . OK ?

From the studen t 's response in line 35 in the aforem ention ed excerpt, we

can see that w hat came to the   fore of this studen t 's aw areness on hearing the

question, "Why [do]

 we add

  '-es'

  at the end of the

 word 'go'?"

 i s

 "subject-

verb agreement."

 It is a

 pe rfectly appropriate response

 to the teacher's

 ques-

tion. How ever, i t w as not w hat the teacher w as loo king for. This is evidenced

by the teacher accepting the answer bu t continu ing w ith the  disjunct ive

 "but,"

which  is typical o f disagreement . T he answer

 that

 the teacher was looking fo r

w as "simple present tense" (see line 40). In fact  it is both the third person sin-

gular

 and the

  simple present tense that necessitate

  th e

  inflection

  of the

  verb

"go."  If the subject were the first or second person, there w ou ld have been n o

need

 for the

 inflection even

 if the

 verb w ere

 in the

 simple present tense.

 A s the

discourse un folds, w e can see that the teacher was ena cting a script in w hi ch

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 182/255

  THE SHARED

 SPACE

 OF LEARNING  69

th e

 teache r 's  focal aw areness

 w as

 very m uch

 on the use o f the

 sim ple present

tense in describing h abitual action (see line 49), and not  subject-verb  agree-

m ent . The

  latter, however,

  w as

  very much

  in the

  student's  focal  awareness

when

 they answered the question. The teacher, being unaw are of how the stu-

dents were experiencing the object of learning, dismissed valuable contribu-

t ions from the

 students

  as

 irrelevant.

L et  us consider another example taken  from  the physics lesson on the

reed relay repo rted  in chapters  1 and 5, and se e the difference  tha t w as m a de

by

  a teacher who shared a large comm on ground w i th the s tudents . In the

lesson

 on the

 reed relay, after

  the

 s tudents conducted

 the

 second experime nt

( in  w hich the teacher asked the s tudents to connect a m otor to both the s im-

p le

 ci rcu i t [w i thout

 a

 reed relay ]

 and the

 com plicated ci rcui t [w i th

 a

 reed

 r e -

lay]) ,

  the

  teacher explained

  the

 different  outcomes observed

  in the

 s imple

circui ts in the two experiments .

1

 First,  she explained why the m o to r did not

rotate in the

 s im ple c i rcu i t w i thout

 a

 reed relay. Then

  she

 w e n t

 on to

 expla in

wh y the LED l it up in th is c i rcu i t .

L et  us  examine  the  fol low ing extract:

7.3

  [Phys ics Lesson/S2/CM I]

1

  T :

  . . . W h y

 is it

 that

 it

 [the LE D] works

 in

 this circuit? [po inting

 at

2  the s im ple ci rcu i t w i th the LED] W hy was i t so w el l-behaved?

3

  W hy did i t

  [LED] l ight

 up?

  [nominate]

4 S :

  Because

 the

 resistance that

 th e

 LD Rneeds,

 no , the

 current

 that it

5

  needs

  is

  smaller than [that needed]

 by the

 m otor.

6 T: Sav that once again , you are nght. I unders tand you .

7

  S : LDR  needs

8

  T :  Wh i c h  is the L D R ?

 This

  one?

9 S :

  Because

  the

 electric that

 L E D

 needs compared

  ...

10  T:

  Elect r ic w hat?

11   S :  Electric curren t  is  smal ler compared wi th the  motor .

12

  T :  That's right. S o if I press i t [the push b utton sw itch] here, there

13  w i l l be enoug h current to make this LE D glow. OK? N o w , w e

14 found that

 if w e

 don 't

 u se the

 reed relay

 [in the

 circuit w i th

 a

15

  m otor], then there

 are

 problems because some circuits m ust

 use a

1 6  reed relay

  to

 operate.

 L e t me

  give

 you an

 example

  .. .

In

 this excerpt ,  the  teacher asked  an open "w hy-quest ion" that required

the

  s tudents

  to

  explain

  why the

  s imple ci rcui t connected wi th

  an LED

w orked (see Fig. 2a in chap. 5). Th e stude nt w as trying to construe the ex pe-

r ience   gained  from  the experiments  and present  it to the  teacher . She was

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 183/255

170  TSUI

trying to relate the

 reason

 fo r the device lighting u p with th e size of the cur-

rent

 that

 was

 needed

 by the two devices

 (the motor

 and the

 LED).

 In

 line

 4,

we can see the

 student struggling with this because although

 she was

 aware

of the

 fact that

 the

 size

 of the

 electric

 current needed

 by the

 motor

 was

 bigger

than that needed

 by the

 device that

 l i t up

 (LED),

 she had

 erroneously labeled

the LED as LDR, and had misrepresented electric current as resistance (see

lines

 4 and 7). It is

 clear that

 the

 student

  had

 some problems

  in

 construing

what she had experienced  in the experiment through a m ean ingfu l l ingu is t ic

representation. The teacher, instead of

 d i smiss ing

 the student's contribution

as wrong, understood that the student w as formulating he r thoughts as she

went along. S he assured the student that she understood what she was trying

to say and

 encouraged

 he r to

 represent

  her

 construal

 of the

 experience once

again.

 As

 soon

 as the

 teacher pointed

 at the

 diagrams

 on the

 board, where

the labels fo r the devices were given, the student realized tha t what she had

thought was an LDR was in  fact an LED, and she  corrected herself. The

teacher

  further

 helped  the student to represent he r construal in more precise

terms by getting her to revise "electric" to "electric current. In contrast to

the previous classroom excerpt, (7.2), th e common ground shared  by the

teacher

 and the students was much larger. As a result, the student was able to

construe

 h er

 experience

 in a mean in g f u l  w ay and to

 make

 a va lu ab le

 contri-

bution

  to the

 discussion.

STRUCTURE  OF  W RENESS

AND THE

 SH RED SPACE

 OF

 LEARNING

T o

 further elucidate

 the

 notion

 of the

 shared space

  o f

 learning,

 let us

 con-

sider

 the notion  o f structure o f awareness that w as discussed in chapter 1 .

W e

 argued that

 awareness

 is the

 totality

 o f

 experience,

 and

 th a t

 characteris-

tically only

 a

 limited number

 of

 things come

 to the  fore of our

  awareness

and the rest recedes  to the ground. Marton and Booth (1997) referred to this

structure  as figure-ground.  However, what comes  to the  fore  of

 one's

awareness varies. When  the teacher  and the  learners,  fo r various reasons,

are working  on d i f ferent  assumptions, their structure of awareness m a y b e

dif ferent .

  What  is figura l to the teacher m a y b e ground to the learners, a nd

vice versa; equally, what

 is f igura l  to

 some learners

 m a y b e

 ground

 to

 oth-

ers. When this mismatch occurs,

  we say

 that

  the

 space

  o f

 learning

 is not

shared, or only partly shared.

L et  us

 compare data

  from

 Class

  A

 (the English lesson

  on

  The Weekly

Plan"

 cited before) a nd another primary Grade 4 lesson on the same topic,

with exactly the same intended object of learning. Let us call the latter, Class

B. In

 Class

  A, the

 teacher

 put up o n the

 board

  the

 diary

 o f

 Mickey Mouse

that

 says

 what he

 does

 from Sunday to Saturday. Above the schedule is the

question, What does

 he do

  after  school? (see Table 7.1)

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 184/255

7.  T H E

  S H A R E D

  SPACE  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 71

T A B L E

  7 .1

English Lesson/Weekly Plan/Primary Grade 4/Class A

White Board Description

Mickey  Mouse s Picture  Wh at Does  H e D o  After  School?

Su n d ay

M o n d a y

Tuesday

W e d n e s d a y

T h u r s d a y

F r i d ay

S a t u r da y

Library

P i an o Lesson

B a ske t b a l l

S w i m m i n g

Baske tba l l

S w i m m i n g

Baske tba l l

T he  fo l lowing  is an

 excerpt

  from

 Class

  A, f rom the

 point

  in the

 lesson

wh en the teacher directed th e students' attention t o Mickey Mouse's diary

on   the

 board.

7.4

 [English lesson/P4/Weekly Plan/Class  A ]

12  T: ... So let us

 look

 at his

 [Mickey Mouse's] diary,

 O K?

 What does

13

  he do after

  school?

 OK.

  [Teacher

 points at the

 diary

 on the

14

  board.] This

  is the

 thing that

 he

 does

  af ter

  school.

 O K ,

 what

15  does he do  af ter school? [Teacher puts a strip  o f paper with

16

  question What does

 he do

 after

 school?

on the

 board.]

 C an

1 7  you tell m e,  What does he do  after  school? Anybody?

18

  [Students raise their hands]. Yes.

19

  S2: He

 goes

  to the

 library

  on

 Sunday.

20

  T :  Yes, h e goes  to the library  on ?

21   S :

  Sunday

22  T :  Sunday. Yes, after school. OK? He says he goes to the library

23  after school.

 "After school."

 OK, how about... What

 else?

 How

24  about others?

25

  [Students raise their hands.

 The

 teacher looks

 at one of the

26

  students signaling

 to her to

 answer

 the

 question.]

27

  S3 : He has a piano lesson  on Monday.

28  T:

  Yes.

 We

 sav—we

 don't say "on Monday" first. OK? The

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 185/255

172

  T S U I

29  quest ion is "What does he do af ter  school?"  So he says that he

30

  has a  piano lesson af ter  ?  School .

31   Ss+T: S ch oo l

32  T: OK. Do you

  unders tand?

33

  Ss: Yes.

34 T:  Yes, OK . How about th e other th ings? W hat does he do a f t e r

35 school? W hat else? [Stu dents raise their ha nds .]

36 S4: He

 p lay basketba l l af ter  school .

37 T:

  Yeah ,

 he

  says

 he

 p l ay s baske tba l l

 af ter  ...

38 Ss+T: S ch oo l.

39  T :  "Plays,"

 [stressing

  the "s"

  sound ]

 O K ?

  Remember "p lays ,"

40 OK? W hat e lse?  O n e mor e th ing . Sa m ue l?

41 S5 : He . . . he p lay s basketba l l on . . .

  af ter

  . . . ?

42   Ss+T: S ch oo l.

Here  we see  tha t when  the  s tudents were asked  to  descr ibe Mickey

M ou se 's d iary , they provided the act iv i ty and the day of the wee k (see l ines

1 9  and

  27) . However , what

  the

  t eacher wan ted

  to

  e l ic i t

  from  the

 s t u d e n t s

was wh a t M ickey M ouse does

  af ter

  school (see the teacher 's correct ion of

the

  s tudents ' answers

  in

 l ines

 22 to 23, and

  l ines

 28 to

 30).

 I t wa s

 on ly

  af ter

the

  teacher m ade

  it

 c lear

 to the

 s tudents tha t they sho uld

 not say the day of

the week ( l ine 28), tha t the s tude nts produced w ha t the teac her con sidered to

be the  correct answ er ( l ine 36) .

W h a t

  w as

  h a p p e n i n g

  in

  th i s p iece

  o f

  d i scou r se?

  H o w

  m u c h c o m m o n

gr ound

  w as

  shared be tween

  th e

  t eacher

  and the

  s t u d e n t s? W h e n

  th e

  s tu -

d en t s p r od uced

  th e  "correct"

  r esponse ,

  in

  w h a t

  w a y w a s t h e

  c o m m o n

gr ound w id ened ? Was th i s common g r ound pe r t inen t t o t he ob j ec t o f

l ea r n ing  of the  l e s son?

To unders tand the m ism atch be tween the s tudents ' answe rs and w hat the

teacher wanted to sol ici t

 from

  them , we need to see the i tem s on the w hi te

board as an integral par t of the s i tuat ion in w hic h the phen om ena of hab i tua l

act ion  and

  f req uenc y

 are

 em bed d ed.

 On the w

r

hite

 board were

 the

  fo l lowing

i tem s : M ickey Mouse ' s p i c tu r e ,

  th e

 d ia ry s t a t i ng w ha t M icke y M ouse

 did

each

 day of the

 w e e k ,

 an d the

 ques t ion "W hat does

 he do

 af ter

  school?"

 T o-

gether they con s t i tu te a p iece of tex t tha t says tha t M icke y M ouse does some

act iv i t ies

  tha t vary accord ing to the day of the w eek . The q ue s t ion "W hat

does

 he do af ter

  school?" provides

 the

 con tex t

 f or

 in t e r p r e t ing

 the

 ac t iv i ti e s

as ou ts ide school hours . T he aspects  o f M icke y ' s d iary tha t were d iscerned

by the

 s tudents ,

 a nd

 were

  focal  in

 the i r aw areness , were

 the

 asp ects th at var-

ied:

 ac t iv it ies

 and

 d ays

 of the

 w e e k

  (as can be

  seen

  from

  S2 's

  and

  S3 's

  re-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 186/255

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 187/255

174

T S U I

T A B L E   7 .2

Engl i sh  Lesson/Weekly  Plan/Primary G rade 4/Class B

Cind y  T i m m y

Weekly

Days

 of the

 w e e k

S unda y

M o n d a y

Tuesday

Wednesday

T hur sda y

Friday

Satu rday

Plan

T h i n g s  to do

Play  t ab le t enn i s

Go to the  l ibrary

Ha ve a

 p i an o

 lesson

Have

 a d an c in g

  lesson

Go to the   library

Ha ve a

 com pute r l e s son

Play

  t ab le

  t e nn i s

Have

 a  c ompu te r lesson

The i tem s on the w hi te board a l so co ns t i tu te a p iece of  tex t . This tex t says

tha t

  Cindy does some ac t iv i t ies each day of the week . The presence of

Tim m y m eans tha t there are two people involved in th i s p iece of tex t . The

variants

  in

  th i s p iece

  o f

  tex t

  are  the day  o f  the week  and  the  activity-.  T he

weekly p lan be longs to Cindy . T immy does no t have a weekly p lan . The

teacher had se t up a d ia logue be tween C ind y and T imm y wher eby T imm y

asks,

 "What

 do you do on (day of the week)'+ s'?"  and C ind y answ er s , "I

(verb + ac t iv i ty ) on  (day of the w e e k )  '+  s'."  For e x a m p le :

T i m m y : W h a t do you do on  Wednesdays?

C indy: I go to the l ibrary on W ednesdays.

B oth var ian ts, day of the w eek and ac t iv ity , w ere focu sed on s im ul ta-

neous ly by the s tudents and the teacher, and bo th w ere evalua ted by the

t eacher when she wen t t h r ough C ind y ' s week ly p l an w i th the s tud en t s .

W h a t

 w as

 invar iant

 was the

 quest ion

 from

 T im m y ,

 and the

 l in gu is t ic s truc-

ture of the

 answers

 from  Cindy that provided  the  scaf fo ld ing for the  stu-

dents . The teach er also drew the s tudents ' a t ten t ion to the plu ral form o f the

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 188/255

7.  T HE  S H A R E D  S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 75

days

 of the

 week

 to

 signify  recurrence, which

 is an

 im portant feature

 of ha-

bitual  act ion.

After  the teacher had gone through  all the days o f the week  and the cor-

responding act ivi ties ,

 th e

 teache r asked

 th e

 s tudents

 to

 sum m arize C indy ' s

schedule  fo r T imm y  and  sol ici ted  from  the  s tudents descr ipt ions  o f f r e -

quency,

 such as "you  go to the library tw ice a week," "you play table tennis

three tim es a

 week,"

 and so on. A s these s t ructures

 were

 solici ted, the fre -

quency  o f occurrence cam e to the fore o f the s tude nts ' awareness , an d the

day  o f the

  week receded into

  the

  ground. What

  the

  s tude nts held

  in

 thei r

awareness s im ul taneo us ly were the ac t iv ity and the f requency . Th is is evi-

denced by the fact that when the s tuden ts w ere asked to come u p w i th thei r

ow n

 weekly

 plans

 and to

 conduct

 a

 dialogue

 asking

 each

 other

  about their

week ly p l ans

  and how

  of ten  they conducted

  th e

  act ivit ies, they

  did not

have

  prob lems .

In

 Class

 B, the

 c ondi t ions o btained

  for

 discerning

  the

 c ri t ical aspects

 of

habi tual action, w hich were nam ely

  the

 activity itself

 and the

 f requency

  o f

occu rrence or recurrence. T he w ay that the teacher set up the s i tuat ion in

w h i c h the

 ha bi tual act ion

 w as

 embedded enabled

 th e

 students

 to

 experience

the object

 in the w ay

 that

 th e

 teacher in tended.

 The

 structure

 of

 aw areness

 of

the teacher and of the students converged, unlike in Class A, where the

teacher ' s

 and the

  s tudents '

 foci  o f

 awareness

 differed.  W e

 could

 s ay

 tha t

 in

Class

 B

 there

 was a

 w ider shared space

 o f

 learning tha n

 in

 Class

 A, and

 that

the  learning outcome—as evidenced by the s tudents ' oral performance of

the

  t ask—showed tha t

 the

  teacher

  in

 Class

  B w a s

  more  effective  than

  the

teache r in

 Class

 A in

 designing

 the

  learning experience,

  as far as the

 t each-

ing   of habi tua l act ion w as  concerned.

N E G O T I A T I O N O F

  M E A N I N G

A N D T H E  SH ARED SPACE  O F  L EARNI NG

In the 7.1

 conversat ion exam ple given

 at the

 very beginning

 of

 this chapter,

we saw

 that par t ic ipants

 A and B

 w ere involved

  in a

 process

  of

 neg ot iating

the

 referent for " the m eet ing" and widening the com m on ground  that w as

shared between them .

 In the

 classroom excerpts (7.2)

 and

 (7.3),

 we saw

 that

w hen cer ta in assum pt ions were

 n o t

 shared betwe en

  the

 teacher

 and the

 stu-

dents , valuable con tr ibut ions

  from

  students could

 b e

  dismissed

  or not

 val-

ued. Beca use of the un equ al power relationship between the s tudents and

the teacher, s tuden ts w ere often  unable to engage  in the k ind o f nego t iation

of

 m ean ing tha t

  is

 prevalent wh en

  the

  power relat ionship

  is

  sym m et rica l,

such   as

  w h a t

  we saw in the

  conversat ion between col leagues (7.1) .

  It is

therefore impo r tan t

 for the

 teacher

 to be

 vigi lant

 of

 assum ptions that

 are not

shared

 w ith learners,

  as

 wel l

 as to be

 sensitive

 to

 signals

 of the

 lack

 of

 com-

m on ground, and to be a ble to respond to such s ignals by en gaging in a ne-

gotia t ion of m ean ing wi th the  learners.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 189/255

1 76  T S U I

Let us take, for exa m ple, an excerpt f rom the lesson o n the s loth , w hic h

was discusse d in chapter 6. In this lesson, the teacher was r ev is ing w hat the

class

 had

 covered

  in the

 prev ious lesson ,  tha t

 is, the

 descr ipt ion

 o f a

 p a n d a ,

and w hat de ta i l s need to be covered w hen the s tude nts w r i te a descr ip t ion of

an

 a n i m a l .

7.5  [Chinese Lesson/P3/The Slo th]

1   T :

  [Ho ld ing

 up a

  t ex tbook showing

 the

  chap te r

 on the

2

  panda] OK,  today, we w i ll do  some wr i t ing . Our

3

  wr i t ing

 w i l l

  follow

  th e

 chap te r

 on the

 p a n d a . . ..

  I

 w a n t

4 you to

  tell

  m e ,  w h e n  w e w r it e abou t an a n i m a l , w h a t

5 do we need to  include in the co ntents?  ... Put up y o u r

6 han d s . W ha t d o we need to w r it e abou t

  first?

7 [A s tuden t r a i ses h i s han d . ]

8 SI:

  Body.

9 T:  "Body" is

 w h a t?

1 0  S I : Hu h? [M ean ing he d oes no t und er s t and the q ue s t ion ]

11   T: Bo dy.

1 2

  S s:

  B ody [Students

  say

  th i s

 in

 E n g l is h , from  their seats . ]

1 3

  T : B ody. W hat e l se? [W r ites "body" on the boa rd .]

14

  S2: The  brain , [meaning

 "head"]

15

  T : Yes , the head . W hat e l se? [W r ites "head" on the

16   board]

17

  S3:

  Four l im bs.

1 8

  T :

  Yes ,

  four

  l im bs . . . .

  any

 m ore? Yes .

19

  S4: The

  sk in ,

  th e

  color

  of the

 fur .

20 T: Color.

 Yes.

 [Writes "color" on the board]

21

  S 5:  Appear ance .

22 T : A ppearance . In  fact ,  the word "appearance." wher e

23  should we put it?  [Referr ing  to location on the board]*

24 Ss: In

  front .

25 T: In front of  four

  l imbs?

26 S5: In   front  of

  four  l imbs.

27 T: In

  front?

  In

  fact.

  I th in k . .. [pauses]

28

  Ss: The h e a d .

29 S6:

  Af t e r

 the

  head.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 190/255

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 191/255

178

  TSUI

dents apparently understood

 "appearance"

 as m eaning jus t the facial  appear-

ance

  of the

 animal.

 This is

 evidenced

  by the  fact

  that they suggested that

 th e

description

  of

  appearance should come before

  the

  description

  of the

  four

l imbs

  in the

  text,

  and

  after

  the

  description

  of the

  head.

 Finally,

  the

  teacher

m ade explicit

 th e

 relationship between

 "appearance" and the

 specific fea tures

(see lines 30 to 32, and lines 30 to 40).

The

  fact

  that there are certain groun ds that are com m on, but that there are

also others that

 are not

 shared betwe en

  th e

 teacher

 and the

 students

 is not u n-

usual  in classroom discourse. There could be a num ber of reasons w hy this

happens,  but  frequently such divergent perceptions are caused  by  linguistic

means,

  for

 example,

 by the

 questions

 the

 teache r asks,

 by the

 pragm at ic

 and

semantic presuppositions carried

 by the

 w ords used,

 by the way the

 teacher

responds

 to the students' questions  or responses, and so on. In this particular

excerpt, there w ere several cause s

 for the

 unshared ground. Firstly,

 the initial

questions asked

 by the

 teacher, "W hat

  do you

 need

 to

 include

 in the

 content?

.. ." "What  do you need to w rite about first?" (line 5 and line 6) were open to a

number

 of

 interpretation s.

 It

 wou ld have been approp riate

 for

 students

 to

 pro-

vide either

  a

 list

 of

 specific  features

 or the

  superordinate

 first. It is

 clear

 from

the data that the relat ionship between appearance, or "outw ard appearance"

to be

  precise,

  and the

  other

  features  w as

  very much

  in the

  teacher 's

  focal

awareness, whereas it w as the specific features that were very m uch in the stu-

dents '

 focal

 awareness. S econdly, the unsha red ground wa s also caused by the

semantic ambiguity

 of the

 word "appearance"

 in

 Ch inese (yeuhng

  m a a uh in

Cantonese) [ 4 f < . f f e ] can mean  facial appearance  or the entire outw ard appear-

ance.

 T he

 teacher

 w as

 using

 th e

 word

  in the

 sense

 of

 "outwa rd appearance,"

whereas

  the

 students were using

 it in the

  sense

  of "facial

  appearance."

W e can see, however, that the teacher  w as able to respond very quickly to

signals

 of

 assump t ions

 no t

 being shared

 by the

 s tudents

 by

 abandoning

 the

initial line

 of

 quest ioning

  and

  al lowing

 the

  students

 to

  list

 the

  specif ic

  fea-

tures . The teacher w as also able to negot iate the m ean ing of the w ord "ap-

pearance," with

  the

  students

  by

  ask ing them where

  the

  word should

  be

placed

  on the

 board,

  and

  guiding them

  to see the

 word

 "appearance" as a

superordinate ( l ines 30 to 32). In th is way, th e t eacher w idened th e c o mmo n

ground shared between him self and the s tudents , so that even tual ly they

were

 able to agree on a descriptive structure that form ed the scaffolding  for

the

  rest

  of the

  lesson.

  In the

  subsequent d i scuss ion am ong

  the

  s tudents

 in

sm all groups, i t w as clear that the students had a good u nde rstan ding of "ap-

pearance"

  as a

  superordinate that subsum es  specific  phy s ica l fea tures .

COLLABORATIVE  CO N STRUCTION  O F  M E A N I N G

A N D T H E  SHARED  SPACE  O F

  L E A R N I N G

In e lucidating  the notion of the  shared space of learning, we proposed  that

there is another sense in w hich the space of learnin g is a shared space; that

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 192/255

7.  TH E  S H A R E D  SPACE  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 79

is, w hen the space of learning is jo in t ly con st i tu ted by the teacher and the

learners throug h

  the

 co l laborat ive const ruct ion

  o f

 m ean ing .

The jo in t co ns t ruc t ion of m eaning be tween ch i ldren and adul t s i s w el l

documen ted . Ha l l iday ' s work on ch i ld l anguage deve lopmen t p rov ides

am ple exam ples o f how a ch i ld , th rough in terac t ion w i th an adu l t , is able

to

 con s t rue exper ience in to m eaning . Typ ica lly , the ch i ld ' s response to the

envi ronmen t

  is

  interpreted

  by the

  adu l t

 as

  i nd i ca t i ng communica t ive

 in -

tent ions ,

 and

 responded

  to

 according ly .

 In the

 course

 o f

 responding

  to the

chi ld ,  the a dul t con t r ibu tes to the co-cons t ruc t ion of the m ean ing in the

discourse

  (see, e .g . , H al l iday , 1973, 1975, 1 993). W el ls (1 986) provided

the

  fo l lowing

  exam ple o f co -cons truc t ion o f mean ing be tween a pa ren t

and  a

 2-year-old

  child.

7.6   [Wells, 1986, p. 46]

M ark (2 ;3)  is s tanding by a cen tral hea t ing radiator  and can

  feel

  the

heat coming from it. He initiates the  conversation by

 sharing

  this inter-

est ing

 in form at ion wi th

 h is

 m other.

M a rk : 'O t , M u m m y ?

M other: Hot? [checking] Yes, that ' s the radiator .

M ark : Been- burn?

M other : B urn? [checking]

M a r k :  Y eh

Mother: Yes, you know

 it'll

 burn don't you?

In this piece of data, as W ells (1 999) observed, the child respon ded to the

cen tral hea ting radiator

 by

 mak ing

 a

 comm en t.

 H is

 mother checked that

 she

had

 co rrectly interpreted the ch ild's u t terance , and told him that the radiator

w as

 the

 source

 of the

 heat .

 The mother's

 correct interpretat ion

 o f the

 chi ld ' s

ut terance enabled him to extend the exchange and bring  in the word "burn,"

w hich was in turn extended by the m other as "Yes, you know i t w i l l bum ,

don ' t

 you?"

 S ubsequent ly

 in the

 discourse (not shown here),

  the

 child

 w as

able

 to

 take over

 the

 m eanings that w ere ini tia l ly co-constructed

  and

 apply

them to a novel s i tuat ion where he saw a  bonfire  in the garden and w as able

to say  tha t i t w as hot and  that  it w ould burn  as w el l.

Let

  us consider how this happens in classroom learning. In the history

lesson that w e discussed  in chapter 6 , the concept o f

  clan

 w as taught  in the

context of ear ly rural com m uni t ies in Ho ng K ong. In th is context , a c lan is a

c o m m u n i t y

 o f

 people w i th

 the

 same surnam e

 w ho

 live togethe r

  in one

 area,

and are also somew hat related by blood. The  fol lowing  is an excerpt  from a

Chinese  medium  classroom (CMI),  where Chinese was the students'

m other tongu e, in which the teacher explored w i th the s tudents why clans

were formed

  and for

 wh at purpose .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 193/255

180

  T SUI

7.7 [History Lesson/S2/CMI]

1

  T: ... So,

 people with

 the

 same surname stay together....

 Why do

 they

2  stay

 together? Why do

 they stay together? Why?

3 S:  Convenient.

4 T: It's convenient. Would it be better

 for

 them to go their separate ways?

5

  Would it be better for you to go your own way? Who says it's

6

  convenient?

  ...

 What

 are the

 advantages

 of

 living together?

7 S:

 They

 can

 help each other.

8 T: Help each other. Yes, that's right.

 Very

 good.... What kind of things

9 do

 they

 need

 to

 help each other with?

 I

 want

 to ask all of

 you.

10  S: Farming.

11

  T: Yes, farming, they need help from each other. You work on this part

12  [of the land] and I work on another part [of the land]. We help each

13  other. What else, apart from

  farming?

14

  S:  Hunting.

15

  T:  Hunting, OK. But we know that it was an agricultural society already,

16  so there was not much hunting.

17  S:  Raising animals.

18  T:  Raising animals, that is, raising cattle and

 sheep.

 Yes. What

 else?

19

  What else?

20 S: Not to be bullied by others.

21 T: Not to be

 bullied. Actually

 this is

 very important

 To

 protect each

 other,

22 mutual protection, apart from farming together, living together.

2 3 Some students said looking for

 food

 together. There is also the function

24  of protecting each other. Why did they have to protect each

2 5  other? So that they would not be bullied, because they came to Hong

26 Kong from a

 faraway

 place and they did not know the place, right?

27 They did not know whether they would be disliked by other clans.

28  What if they were attacked by them? That's why they needed

 mutual

29 protection. They had to be armed. In addition, there were blood ties as

3 0  well. Just now I told you that they had the same surname ... Another

31 function was to preserve the traditional  l i f e  style.

In this excerpt, the teacher posed a number of open questions. Each time

a student responded, the teacher took on the response and opened it up for

inquiry. For example, in line 2, the teacher asked why people with the same

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 194/255

  THE SHARED SPACE OF LEARNING

1 8 1

surname stayed together and one student responded that i t was for conve-

nience ( l ine

 3). The teacher

 took

 on the

 response

 a nd

 asked

 w hy i t was con-

venient . When   no

 response

  w as  for thcoming,  the  teacher rephrased  the

word

 "convenient"

 as

 "the

 advantages

 of

 living

 together"

 (line

 6 ), and

 asked

the students

 to

 list

 the

 advantages .

 As w e can see,

 th is im m ediately elic i ted

th e

 response "help

 each

 other"

 (line 7) from an other s tudent. A t the end of a

sequence of in teract ion around the topic , a sem ant ic netwo rk relat ing to the

formation  o f clans w as collaboratively constructed by the studen ts and the

teacher: that  is,  mu tua l help ( fa rming) ; mutual protection  (so as not to be

bul l ied by others; line 20 and line 21); bloo d t ies (bec au se they a l l had the

same

 surnam e and therefore had the sam e ancestor; l ine 29 and l ine

 30);

 and

preservation of the tradit ional

 l ifestyle

  (line

 31).

The col laborative const ruction of the sem ant ic netw ork during the inter-

active  process  is  i l lustrated in

 Fig. 7.1.

Figure

 7.1 show s the way in w hich the teacher and the s tudents co-con-

structed the space o f learning by engag ing in a nego t iat ion of me aning . For

exam ple , the word

 "convenient"

  suggested by the s tudent w as too  vague ,

and so the teache r  clarified  and elaborated the m ean ing of "convenient" by

rephras ing  it as "the advantages of staying together." The

 teacher's

 interpre-

ta t ion

 o f the

 s tudent ' s in tended m ean ing helped

 to

 move

 the

 discuss ion for-

FI G. 7.1.  Co llaborative construction of a

  semant ic

  ne twork  of formation of  c lan.

Note:  Words

  underlined

  are

  contributions  from

  th e

  teacher

  and

  other  contributions

  are

from

  students.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 195/255

182  T S U I

w ard, as can be seen   from  the imm ediately for thcom ing response  from  an-

other student.  Had the  teacher misinterpreted  the  s tudent ' s meaning,  the

subsequent contributions  from  the s tudents w ou ld probably not have been

for thcoming

 or they w ould have been

 different

  (see W ells , 1999). B y ask ing

the s tudents what  the  advantages  o f  s taying together were,  the  teacher

opened up a dimension of variat ion for exploring the benefi ts that people

gained

 by

  staying together. When

  the

 students

 proffered "help

 each other"

as an

 ins tance

  in the

  dimension

  of

 advantages ,

  th e

  teacher took

  on the re-

sponse   and opened up a fur ther dim ension of variat ion f or ex plor ing th e di f -

ferent  ways in which members of a clan needed to help each other . The

students suggested "farming," "hunting," "raising animals," a nd "not  to be

bull ied by

 others,"

  as ins tances along this dimension. The teacher high-

l ighted the im portance  of not be ing bu l l ied and rephrased  it at a h ighe r

 level

of abst ract ion by u s ing the phrase "m utual protect ion." Final ly , the teach er

contr ibuted tw o m ore reasons for the form at ion of clans : b lood t ies and pre-

serving tradit ional  life  s tyle . The interact ion between the teacher and the

s tudents was a process in w hich que st ions posed by the teacher opened up

dimens ions  of variat ion that afforded  opportuni t ies  for s tudents to explore

the reasons for people to s tay together . These opp ortuni t ies w ere taken up

by the stude nts, and their con tributions w ere in turn us ed by the teac her to

open   up  further  dimens ions  fo r explorat ion. T he  space of  l earn ing opened

up was therefore a co l laborat ive effort  be tween the t eacher and the l earners .

In

  this sense, the space of learning was a shared space.

Embodied in the notion o f the jo in t con st i tu t ion of the space o f l earn ing is

also

 the

 col laborat ive c onst ruct ion

 of

 m ean ing am ong l ea rne rs . Th i s

  is

 par-

t icu lar ly evident w hen learners

 are

 engaged

  in

 g roup w ork .

 For

 example ,

 in

chapter 6 , we saw how learners b rou ght the ir own know ledge of the w or ld ,

and

 thei r previous experience,

  to

 bear

 on the

 ob jec t

 o f

 learn ing

 as

 they co l -

laboratively

 constructed texts on the story of the sloth.

LIN G UISTIC

 RESO URCES

A N D T H E

  SHARED SPACE

  O F

  LEARNING

A s w e

 have already pointed out , at tem pts m ade

 by the

 teacher

  to

 widen

 the

shared space of learning may not always be   successfu l .  The cause of th i s

mismatch could

 be

 l inguist ic,

 o r it

 could

 be due to the

 fact  that

 the

  structure

of the

  learning experience brought about

  different  foci

  o f

  awareness .

  In

classrooms

 where the  medium of  instruction  is not the  students ' mother

tongue (i .e. , a l anguage in w h i c h the  s tudents a re l ess com peten t ) , th e  fail-

ure to  widen the

 shared space

 of learning m a y be caused b y a lack of l ing uis-

tic

 resources (p ar t icular ly

 on the

 par t

 o f the

 s tudents , a l thou gh som et im es

 it

can also

 be on the

 part

 of the

  teacher).

Let us look at the fol lowing excerpt f rom an Engl ish medium lesson

(EMI; English

 was not the

 students'  mother tongue)

 on the

  same history

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 196/255

7.

  T H E  S H A R E D S P A C E  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 83

topic

 as the CM I

 lesson just ci ted (and

 in

 chap.

 6), on the

 early rural com m u-

nity. In this lesson,  the teache r asked the students to compare the diaries that

they  had writ ten befo re the lesson, w ith those w ritten by a father, Tang Tai

M a n ,

 and his

 son, Tang

 S iu

 M a n,

 in the

 days

 o f the

 rural com m uni t ies ( these

w ere g iven as han douts). T he teach er 's intention w as to bring out the

  differ-

ent

  l ifestyles

  of people today and

 people

 in the past .

7.8 [History Lesson/S2/EMI]

The

  teacher asked

  the

  students

  to

 w rite

  a

 diary prior

  to

 this lesson

  and

then asked them to compare their diaries with the two diaries on the

hando ut by Tang Tai M an and his son, Tang Siu Ma n.

1

  T: . .. OK ,

 look

 at the on e of

 Tang

 Siu

 Man , Tang

 S iu

 M a n .

 Is it

2  different from  yours?

3 Ss:  Yes.

4 T: How   different?  Can you  tell me how different?  Stephanie ,

5  give

 me one

  difference.

6 S: [speaking very  softly]  I have not the chicken .

7

8

  T: Yes , then w hat else? W hy [does] the boy have chickens ,

9 duck s , and pigs? W hat did he do?

10

  S:  [silent]

11

  T :

  Wh a t

  did he do? How

  come

  he has the

  chickens ,

  ducks , and

12  p ig s? W ha t did he do in his  dai ly life?

13  S:  [silent]

14

  T:

  Unders tand

  m y

  quest ion?

15

  S :  [speak ing very  softly,  inaudible]

16

  T :

  W h a t

 is his

  work, Stephanie? Angela,

 can you

 help?

  ...

In  line 4, the teacher took on Stephan ie's response that her diary w as  differ-

ent  from  Tan g's, an d tried to open it up for inquiry by

 asking

 her how they

were  different.  Stephanie tried

 to

 explain that

 she did not

 rear chickens.

 T he

teacher took  on S tephanie's response  and ag ain tried to open it up for  further

inquiry

 b y

 asking

 her why

 Tang

 S iu M an had

 chickens, ducks,

 and

 pigs .

 The

teacher was

 ho ping that Stephanie would point

 out the

  differences  between

life

  in

 ear ly rural comm uni t ies

 a nd

  life

  in

 mo dern u rban societies. How ever,

Stephanie w as tong ue-tied, even after prom pting f rom the teach er tw ice ( l ine

10

 and

  l ine 11 ). Finally,

 the

  teacher

 had to ask

 another student

 to

 help out .

Throu ghou t the w hole lesson, there w ere a num ber of ins tances in w hich

no   response  w as  for thcoming, even  af ter  the  t eacher  had  repeated  or re-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 197/255

184 T S U I

phrased the question. In other w ords, the teacher had considerable

  difficul-

t ies trying to co-construct the space o f learn ing w ith the stu den ts due to the

students' lack

 of

 l ing uist ic resources.

A quant i ta t ive analysis was conducted on the two his tory lessons (one

EM I and one

 C M I) that were discussed

  in

 this chapter

 and in

 chapte r

 6 and

two other h i s tory lessons (aga in , one E M I and one C M I) teach ing the same

topic to see how

 often

  the teachers w ere able to m ake use of the s tu den ts ' an-

swers  to open  up the  space  for further  inquiry, and  facili tate  the col labora -

tive

 construct ion of m eanin g betw een teacher and s tudents . The f indings are

presented  in  Table 7.4.

W e can see from Table 7.1  tha t in the two EM I  lessons, over half of the

students ' responses w ere s imp ly accepted or repeated by the teach ers w ith-

out

  further

  elaboration or  c o m m e n t. T he  s i tuat ion is a lmos t the m i rro r im -

age of the two CM I ( i .e . , m other ton gu e) lessons. It is not

 difficult

  to see tha t

in C M I  c lassrooms,  the teachers and the s tudents w ere engaged in

 m a k i ng

sense of the object of learning col laborat ively. By contrast , in EMI

(nonm other tong ue) c lass room s,  the  interact ion w as  ve ry much un id i rec-

tional an d the  learners played  a  much more passive role.

C O N C L U S I O N

In

 chapter  1 , we

 proposed

 the

 notion

  of the

 space

 o f

 learning b eing charac-

terized by c lassroom interact ion in the

 l ight

 o f a

 specif ic

  ob jec t o f

 lea rn ing .

A n d w e

 pointed

 out

 that

 th e

 space

 of

 learning

 is

 cons ti tuted

 by the

 po ssibil i-

ties  for lea rn ing brough t about b y w h a t i s takin g p lace in the  c lassroom in

relation

  to the

  object

  of

  learning.

  T he

 po ssibilities

  for

 learning ( i .e . , w hat

w e have re fe r red to as the enac ted o bjec t of lea rn ing ) cannot be brou ght

about

  by the

  teacher alone,

  or by the

  learners themselves; they must

  be

T A B L E   7 .4

Teachers

Responses

 to Students Answers  in E M I an d C M I Classrooms

Teachers  'Responses

  to

  Students '

Answers   to Al l  Teacher Quest ions

EMU EM

 1 2

CMI 1 CM I  2

Rejection

Accept ing / repea t ing

  s tudent ' s

answer

E xte nd ing  s tudent ' s answer

No com ment /new q ues t ion

Tota l num ber

 of

  responses

0 1  ( 1 % )  1 ( 1 % )  1 (3%)

5 0( 6 9 .5 % ) 4 2 ( 5 9% ) 1 6 ( 1 7 % ) 1 2 ( 3 4 % )

1 6 ( 2 2 . 5% ) 1 4 ( 2 0 % ) 6 9 ( 7 3 % ) 2 2 ( 6 3 % )

6 ( 8 % ) 1 4 ( 2 0 % ) 9 ( 9 % )  0

72 (10 0%) 71 (1 0 0 % )

  95

 ( 1 0 0% ) 3 5 ( 1 0 0 % )

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 198/255

7.

  T H E

  S H A R E D  S P A C E

  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 85

joint ly consti tuted by both the teacher and the learners. In light of this, we

proposed, in this chapter, that the space of learning is a shared space.

The joint consti tut ion of the shared space of learning has very mu ch to do

with

 the s t ructure of aw areness o f the par t ic ipants in a discou rse. If w hat co-

m es to the fore o f their awareness is

 different,

  participants talk a t cross pu r-

poses .

 In a

 classroom si tuat ion,

 the

  learners ' s tructure

 of

 aw areness

 is

  often

shaped

 by the way in

 wh ich

 the

  teacher organizes

  the

 learning experience,

and

  the way in

 w h ic h

 he

 presents

  the

 mater ials . Using data

  f rom two

 pri-

m ary Grade 4 lessons on "the w eekly

 plan,"

 we i l lustrated the way in w h i c h

the  learners ' s t ructure of awareness was shaped by the mater ials that the

teache r put on the  board, and the way in wh ich m ore

 effective

  learning w as

b rough t

 abo ut w hen the teach er ' s s t ructure of aw areness accorded w i th that

of the s tudents .

T he

  space

  of

 learning

  is a

  shared space

  in the sense

  that

  the

  interaction

between the teacher and the learners i s  fel ici tous  only when both part ies

share some common ground  on which further interaction can be based. W e

have  looked a t examples  of c asua l conversat ion  and  classroom data  to see

w h a t

 happens w hen there is little or no com m on ground shared between par-

t ic ipants .

 In the case of the con versation betw een  tw o co l leagues (7 .1 ) , th e

interlocutors

 had to

 work very hard

 to

 w iden

 the

 comm on ground

 in

 order

 to

advance  the co nversat ion. In the ca se of the En gl ish lesson on the w ee kly

plan in prim ary G rade 4A, an appropriate con tribution by a student w as dis-

m issed by the t eacher because the lat ter failed  to see the ra t ionale beh ind th e

s tudent ' s

 answer , and hence an ex cel lent oppo rtuni ty fo r learning w as  lost.

B y  co nt ras t, th e c lass room data from  the reed relay experim ent (7.3) and the

lesson

 on the s loth (7.5) showed h ow po ss ib i l it ies fo r l earn ing were opened

up wh en the teacher was aware of the comm on ground shared be tween h im -

self o r herse l f and the learners . In these tw o exam ples, w e also saw how the

shared space

  of

  learning

  w as

 widened

  by the

 n egotiation

  of

 m eaning

  be-

tween   the  t eacher  and the  learners  in the  lesson  on the  s lo th ,  and by the

teacher helping

 the

 learner

 to

 clarify

 h is or her

 thinking

 and

 lingu istic repre-

sentat ion

  in the

  lesson

 on the

 reed relay.

T he  space o f learning is a shared space also in the sense tha t th e po ssibili-

t ies for l ea rn ing are jo in t ly const i tu ted by the t eacher and the learner. In the

Ch inese m edium h is to ry l esson ,

 w e saw how the

 teacher opened

  up

 possi-

bi l i t ies  fo r  learning by  ask ing ques t ions  on the  basis  of the  s tudents '  re-

sponses (7.6) , and how, as a consequence of the teacher ' s quest ioning, a

semant ic  ne twork

 o f

 "clan"

 w as

 co l laborat ively con st ructed

 by the

 t eache r

and the s tudents .

T he  proposal that the space o f learning is a shared space join tly c ons ti tuted

by the teacher and the learners is an impo rtant on e from the perspec tive o f the

w ay

 we describe teac hing and learning in relat ion to one another. Investiga-

t ions of teaching an d learning m ust take into considerat ion not only the possi-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 199/255

186  T S U I

bilities for learning being opened up by the teacher, but also which

possibilities  are actually made use of by the learners.

E N D N O T E

'The  s imple c i rcui t wi thout  the  reed  re lay carries  a  very smal l current  and is

therefore able  to l ight up the LE D, w hich on ly requ i res a very sm al l current to l ight

up. The

  complicated c i rcui t w i th

 the

 reed relay

 can

 carry

 a

 very large current once

the  reed switch  is c losed  and can  therefore cause  the m o to r  to rota te .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 200/255

I V

O n   Improving Learning

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 201/255

  his page intentionally left blank

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 202/255

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 203/255

190 LO ET AL.

O ur

 point

  o f

 departure

  is

 that teaching

 i s— or

 sho uld be—a ra t ional ac t iv -

ity.  In  planning  a  lesson  or a  un i t ,  the  teacher must take into account  the

characterist ics  of the students (e.g. , age, general a bil i ty ) as w e l l as the phys -

ical conditions

  of the

  classroom (e.g . , c lass s ize, equip m ent ava i lable ) .

However,  a ll these

 concerns mus t

  be

  translated into actions that

  can

  help

achieve specific goals, that

 is, the

 exp l ic i t capab i l it i es tha t s tuden ts

 are ex-

pected

  to

 develop.

  W e

 call these cap abi l i t ies

 th e

 ob jects  of  l e ar n i ng . Even

 in

the

  m ost open classroom , students ' learnin g should

  not be

  acc id en ta l

 bu t

should

 b e the

 resul t

 o f

 conscious at tempts

  on the

 part

 of the

 teacher

  to

 br ing

about  the intended  learning  ou tcomes.  In  other words,  at tent ion must  be

paid

 to the

 w a y s

  in

 w h i c h

 the

 objects

 o f

 learn ing

 are

 deal t w i th

 a nd

 enacted

in

  the

  classroom.

Seen   in this l ight, curre nt debates (e.g. , concerns abo ut the relat ive m er i ts

of

 teacher-centered

  and

  s tudent-centered instruct ion ) that focus

 o n

 genera l

aspects, and not on

 ways

 of

 dealing w ith

 the

 specific c ontent

  of

 learning,

 are

of l im ited value both  in term s  o f providing learning op por tun i t ies  for teach-

ers and in  imp roving s tudent learning. T he t each ing a r rangem en t s ( such as

whole-class teaching

 vs.

 g roup teach ing ,

 and the use of IT in the

 c l a s s room )

should be of concern only i f d iscusse d in rela t ion to the sp ecif ic s of wh at is

taught

  and

  learned.

This

  l ine o f a rgum ent para l le l s what K i lpa t rick , S w afford . and Findel l

(2001) referred  to in the  con tex t  of ma them at i c s ed uca t ion :

M u c h deba te cen te r s

 on

 fo r ms

 and

 a pp r oa c he s

 to

 t e a c h in g : "d i re c t

  ins t ruc t ion"

 v e r s u s

" inqui ry ," " teache r -cen te red" ve r sus " s tudent-cen te red ," " t r ad i t iona l" ve r sus " re -

form." These labe ls m ake

  rhe to r i ca l d i s t i nc t i ons

  t ha t

 of ten

  m i s s

  th e

  po in t r e ga r d ing

th e qua l i t y o f i n s t ruc t i on . Ou r review of the

 r e se a r c h m a ke s

 p l a i n t ha t t he  e f f ec t iveness

of

 ma themat ic s teach ing

 a nd

 lea rn ing

 does no t

 rest

 on

 s im ple labe ls . Ra the r ,

 the

 q u a l -

it y

  o f  ins t ruc t ion  is a

  func t ion

  o f

  t e a c he r s ' know le dge

  and use of  m a t he m a t i c a l

  c on -

t en t ,  teache rs ' a t ten t io n

 to an d  h an d l i n g o f s tudent s , and

  s t u d e n t s ' e n g a g e m e n t

 in and

use of

 m a them at ica l ta sks,

  (p .

  318)

Several recent theories regarding  the profess ional developm ent o f t each-

ers also appear  to have focused on the different  w a y s o f deal ing wi th the ob-

j ec t

  o f

  learning

  in the

  c lass room. Bas ing the i r a rgument

  on a

  s tudy

  o f

California elementary school

  teachers  o f

  m athema tics , Cohen

  and

  Hil l

(2000) pointed out that teachers ' opportunities to learn are a key   factor  af-

fect ing

  classroom practice,

  and

  that such oppor tuni t ies exis t only when

teachers

  are

  asked

  to

  respond deeply

  to

  what they

  are

  supposed

  to

  teach .

They   fou nd that profess ional developme nt act iv i t ies tha t al low ed teachers

to

 leam about

  the

 cu rr iculum

  had a

 much

 greater

 effect

  on

 altering teaching

practices than those that appeared  to be either gene ric  o r per ipheral  to sub-

jec t m at ter (Cohen

  &

 H ill , 2000). They even sug gested that classroom prac -

tice  w as  m os t e f fec t ive ly enhanced  by  those t raining a ct iv i t ie s that gave

teachers c oncrete , top ic-spe cif ic learning op po r tuni t ies . Their data also

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 204/255

8.  T O W A R D

  A

  P E D A G O G Y

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  1 91

showed that

  the

  mathemat ics ach ievement

  of

  s tudents

  in

  schools where

teachers w ere provided with these learning o ppo rtunities

  w as

  considerably

high er than in those w ithou t such opportunit ies.  They con cluded that inter-

vent ion s tudies must change what teachers  do  wi th the curr iculum in the

classroom

 in

 order

 to

 affect  student learning. W hat matters, therefo re,

 is the

enac ted  objec t of learning, that is , how the obje ct of learning is ac tua lly

dealt

 w i th

 in the

  classroom.

It is in this context that we introduce the idea of the "learning study." T he

learning study is based on the Japan ese style "lesson study," w hich w e  feel

takes

 the

 object

 o f

 learning

 as its

 point

 of

 departure,

 bu t

 wh ich also incorpo-

rates

  the

 m ethodologica l concept

  of the

  des ign experiment

  pu t

  forward

 by

A. L .

 Brown (1992)

 an d

 Coll ins (1992).

 W e

 explain these ideas

 in

 turn

 in the

fo l lowing  sections.

LESSON

  STUDY

Stigler and Hiebert (1999) introduced  the  idea of the  lesson study  (jugyou

kenkyuu)  in

 Japan

  in

 their book,

  The Teaching Gap. T he

  lesson study, they

argued ,

  gives Japanese teachers a m odel of con t inuou s school-based pro-

fess ional  developm ent , w hich the two researchers see as one of the main

reasons

 fo r the

 success

 o f the

 c lass room prac t ice re form s

 in

 post-w ar Japan

( and

 the relat ively high achievem ent rates of Japanese s tudents in in terna-

t ional com parison studies). St igler and Hiebert (1999) explained the rat io-

nale of the

  lesson study

  as

 fol low s:

T h e  p r e m i s e

 b e h i nd

  l e s son

  s t udy

  i s s imple : I f you  w a n t  to improve teach ing , the

m o s t

 e f fec t ive

  p l a c e to do so is in the co ntex t of a c l a s s ro o m l e s s o n . If you  s ta r t

 w i t h

l e ssons ,

 t h e p r o b l e m  o f how to a pp ly r e sea r ch

 f i nd ings

  in the c l a s s ro o m d is a p p e a r s .

Th e im p ro v em e n t s a r e d ev i sed

 w i t h i n

 t h e c l a s s room  in the f irst p l a c e . Th e ch a l l en g e

n o w

  b e c o m e s

  tha t  o f

  i de n t i f y i ng

  the k inds o f

  c h a n g e s

  tha t

  w i l l

  i m p r o v e

  s tuden t

l e a r n i n g

 in the c l a s s r o o m a n d , o n c e th e c h a n g e s a re i de n t i f i e d , o f shar ing

 t h i s k n o w l -

ed ge  w i t h

  o th e r t e ach e r s

  w h o  face  s i m i l a r

 p r o b l e m s ,

 or

  s h a r e

 s i m i l a r

  g o a l s ,

  in the

c l a s s r o o m ,  ( p .

 I l l )

In

 a typ ical lesson study, a group o f teachers com e together and select a

topic to be dealt with during one or several "research

  lessons"

  (kenkyuu

jugyou}.

  Members

  of the

  group draw

  on

  each other 's experiences,

  and to-

ge ther

 they d ecide on the intended objec ts of learning and work out a lesson

plan designed to achieve these goals . On e teacher w i l l then teach the lesson,

whi le everyone in the group o bserves. Th e lesson is then e valuated and the

plan

 revised. Ano ther teache r w il l then teach the revised lesson w ith the oth-

ers observing, and a second round of evaluation and reflect ion wil l take

place. The research lesson is then docum ented so that the outcome can be

shared with other teachers. According

  to

 S t igler

 a nd

 Hiebert (1 999),

 a

 les-

son study

  is

 com posed

  of

  eight steps:

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 205/255

192 LO ET AL.

  Def in ing

 the

  problem

•  Planning the  lesson

•  Teaching  the  lesson

• Evaluating the lesson and reflec t ing on i ts

  effect

•  Revis ing the  lesson

  Teaching

 the revised lesson

  Evalua t ing

 and

 re f lec t ing again

  Sharing

 the

 results

D E S I G N

  EXPERIMENTS

A s such,

  the

 goal

  of the

  research lesson

  is not

 jus t

  to

 produce

  an

  effective

lesson, but , through   careful  p l ann ing  and  eva lua t i on , to  ensure tha t  the

teachers involved come to real ize why and how the lesson w orks . A lesson

s tudy is of

 course

 not a

 control led experim ent

 i n

 w h i c h

 one

 factor

 is

 a l low ed

to

  vary whi le others

  a re

  kept constant .

  O n e

  w ou ld ac tua l l y ques t ion

  th e

value and the p ossibil i ty  of a co nt ro ll ed exper im ent  in  such com plex a nd

con found ing

  envi ronments

  as

  those

  o f

  educat ional set t ings . However ,

  as

A .L. Brown (1992)

  and

  Coll ins (1992) suggested,

 it is

 s t il l w or th con duct -

ing

 design expe rimen ts

  in

 edu cat ion , espe c ia l ly

 in

 c lass room resea rch .

 A l-

though   w e cannot control  a ll aspects  of the environment , argue Brow n and

Coll ins , we can

 still

 g ain considerable ins igh ts in to the operat ion of the m a-

jo r

  variables through systematic intervention

  and

  unbiased observations.

Conclus ions

 c an still be

 drawn,

 b y

 com par ing

 the

 resu l t s ob ta ined

 from

 d i f -

ferent  conditions, although w e have to interpret those re sults and guard our-

selves against making general c laims based

  on

  l im i ted observations .

  T he

benef i ts

 o f design experiments  are that w e w i ll be able to contribute to the-

ory

  development ,

  and

  improve pract ice

 at the

 s ame t im e .

T H E   L E A R N I N G  STUDY

It

 w as in the spirit  of

 Brown's

 and C oll ins ' design ex perim ents tha t w e tried

to r e fo rm ula te th e Japane se lesson s tudy in the research tha t w e report in the

latter part

  o f

  this chapter. Marton (2001) defined

 the

  learning study

  in the

fo l lowing  w a y :

A  l e a rn ing s tud y is a s y s t e m a t i c a t t e m p t to a c h i e v e an

 educa t iona l ob jec t ive

 and learn

from

  tha t a t t empt .

  It is a

  d e s ign

 e x p e r i m e n t

  t ha t

  may or may not be a

  lesson

 s tudy.

Such a s tudy is a l e a rn ing s tudy in two senses . F i r s t , it a i m s a t

 b r ing ing l ea rn ing

 a b o u t ,

o r more cor rec t ly , a t

 m a k i n g

 l earn ing p o s s i b le . T he

 s tudent s

 w i l l

 t h u s

 learn ,  hopefu l ly .

Second , those teachers  involved try to learn

 f rom

  the

  l i terature, from

  each o the r , from

th e  s tudents , and n ot  leas t , f rom  the  s tudy  i tself ,  (p . 1 )

Specifically, in our own research  fo r this book, w e w an ted to learn the po-

tent ia l

 va lue

 of the

  theory

 o f

 variat ion from  these s tudies .

 T he

  lesson study

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 206/255

8.

  T O W A R D  A  P E D A G O G Y  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  1 93

is an appropriate m odel as i t a l low s researchers and teachers to see how the

objects  o f learning are de al t w i th in the classroom .  T he  learning s tudy dif-

fers

  from

  the Japanese lesson study model in that our research lessons are

based

  on a

 theoretical frame w ork

 of

 learning— nam ely,

  phenomenography

( c f .  Marton  & B ooth, 1997), and variat ion theory— and w e w i sh to find  ou t

how

 w el l the theory has w orked. In th is w ay the learning s tudy is a learning

study not in tw o, but in three senses, as the researchers are supp osed to learn

from   it as w e l l.

T he

 cycle

  of a

  learning study comprises

 the

  fol lowing

 steps:

1.  Choos ing

 and def in ing a specific  se t of educat iona l

 object ives . These

are the ca pab i l it ies or values to be developed during one or several

lessons .

2.

  Finding

 out the

 extent

 to

 w hich

 the

 students have developed

  the

 capa-

bili t ies

 o r

 va lues targeted before

 the

  t each ing beg ins .

3.

  De signing a lesson (or series of lessons) aim ed at develop ing these

capabi l i t ies

 or

 values .

 The

 p lanning work m us t t ake in to accou nt

 the

exist ing

 know ledge of the s tudents , the teache rs ' pr ior expe riences in

dealing with   the objects  of learning,  and the  research literature.

4.

  Tea ching the lesson (or lessons) according to the plan .

5.  Eva lua t i ng the lesson  (or lessons) to see the extent to w h i c h the s tu-

dents have developed  the targeted ca pa bi l i ties or va lues .

6 .  D o c u me n t i n g

  an d

  d i s semina t i ng

  th e

  a im, p rocedures ,

  an d

 r e su l t s

obtained.

In

 the nex t three sect ions , we desc r ibe three c yc les of a learning s tudy in

two separa te p ro jec t s . The f ir s t two cy cles w ere co ndu cted w i th in an ongo -

ing rese arch pro ject ent i t led "Cater ing  fo r Ind iv idua l Di f f e rence s (B u i ld-

in g

 on V ariation)," commissioned by the  Educat ion Department  of Hong

Kong   S pecia l A dm inis tra t ive Region of the Peop le 's R epu bl ic o f C hina ,

w h i l e

  the

  thi rd cycle consis ted

  of an

  independent p ro jec t tha t employed

the

  theory of var ia t ion in des ign ing c lass room lessons to he lp s tuden ts to

learn

  e c o n o m i cs .

CATERING   FO R  I N D I V I D U A L D I F F E R E N C E S

  B U I L D I N G   O N

  V A R IA TIO N )

The "Catering for Individual Differences (B ui lding on V ariation)" project

is  a 3-year, ongoing research  project undertaken by a team of researchers

from

  the

  F acul ty

  of

  Educat ion

 at The

  Universi ty

  of

  Hong Kong ,

  and the

Hon g Kong Ins t i tu te

 of

 E ducat ion , w i th Pong

 and Lo as

 principa l investiga-

tors and M arton as consu l tant . As there are different  w ays in w hich the t erm

ca ter ing for ind iv idua l

  differences

  can be interpreted, i t is there fore im por-

tant that

 w e

  explain

  our

  understanding

  of the

  term

  and its

 bearing

 on the

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 207/255

1 94 L O E T A L .

aims  of the  project .  T he  project members bel ieve that because  different

people

 are bom

 w i th different  abil i t ies, charac teristics,

 and

 orientations ,

 in -

dividual  differences are a natura l phenomenon and are not a problem in

themselves. Individual

 d ifferences

 only become  a problem in the  classroom

sett ing, when

 a

 teacher

 ha s to

 teach

 a

 large group

 of

 s tudents

 th e

 same con-

tent. Th us, the project aim s at helpin g teach ers to find wa ys to enab le all stu-

dents—in a typical mixed-abi l i ty c lassroom set t ing—to leam what is

intended by tak ing into accoun t the s tuden ts ' diverse exis t ing know ledge

and  unders tandings .

T o achieve this, the p roject team bel ieves that it is necessary to start wi th a

carefully  defined object  o f  lea rn ing—tha t is, the  cap abi l i t ies that s tude nts

are expected to acq uire . For exam ple , i f w e w ish the s tudents to u nderstan d a

certa in phenom enon,  it fol lows that in order to accomm oda te individual d i f -

ferences, the teacher m ust f irs t have a deep and thoro ugh un dersta ndin g of

the different po ssible ways in wh i c h th e s tudents m ight unde rstand this phe-

nomenon. Research   findings in  phenomenography have shown

  that

  al-

though s tudents

 are

 un ique individuals , there

 are

 a lways

 a

 l im i ted num ber

of  qual i ta t ively different  wa y s  in w hich they unders tand a par t icula r phe-

nomenon (Marton & Booth, 1997). These different  w ays of unders tanding

arise m ainly because

  a

 different

  set of

 cri t ical aspects

 of the

 p h e n o m e n o n

are

  discerned.

How do teachers gain access to such kno w ledge of s tudents '

 exis t ing

 u n-

derstandings?  The  team believes that teachers  can first  listen  to, and  leam

f rom, their studen ts. They  can also share their findings wi th o ther teachers

in the

  same school. Within

 the

 project , experience sharing w ith other teach-

ers takes place in col laborat ive groups co m pris ing teachers who teach the

same

 subject a t the

 same

 level in one school. In this way, ind ividual  differ-

ences becom e

  a

 resource

  for the

 teacher ra ther than

 a

 co ns t ra in t .

Bearing

  in

  mind

  the

 various

  understandings students

  m ay

  bring

  to the

classroom,  and having identified  the c ri t ical aspects of the objects o f learn-

ing,

  the

 group

 o f teachers

 move

  on to

 develop

  a

 research  lesson that meets

those ends.

In

 developing a research

 lesson,

 three aspects of variation are made use of

w ithin the project :

V a ri a t i o n

  in

  Terms

  of Students

U n d e r s t a n d i n g

  of

  W h a t

  I s

 T a ug ht

Students understand that w hich they are supposed to leam in a l imite d num -

ber of

 different ways .

 O ur

 research shows that teachers

 w ho pay

 close atten-

t ion to such differen ces (or varia t ions) , and w ho can bui ld on s tuden ts ' prior

understanding

  and

  experiences,

  are

  better able

  to

  bring abo ut

  mean ing fu l

learning for their students. Students also learn more

  effectively

  w hen they

are made aw are of the  different  w ays in w hich their c lassmates deal w ith or

understand

  the

  same object

 of

 lea rn ing.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 208/255

8.

  T O W A R D

  A  P E D A G O G Y  O F  L E A R N I N G  1 95

Variation  in

 Teachers'

  Ways  of  Dealing With Particular

  Topics

As a

 teac her gains expe r ience

 over the

 years ,

 he or she

 w i l l bu i ld

 up a

 good

work ing knowledge about the

  dif ferent

  ways in which s tudents dea l wi th

par t icu lar concepts

 or

 p h e n o m e n a ,

 and

 w i ll a lso develop wa ys

 of

 hand l ing

these

  dif ferences .

  Such knowledge i s ex t remely valuab le and should be

ident i f ied,  sh arpened, sy stem atical ly ref lected upo n, and above al l , shared

wi th

 o ther teachers .

Variation

  as a

 Teaching  Method

In

 previous chapters, we have shown that in any particular lesson there are al-

way s  things that teachers vary w hile other things are kept unch ange d, and that

this

 pattern of variation— w hat is varied and w hat rem ains un chang ed— is of

decisive

  importance  in  determining  how  effective  th e  lesson will  be. In the

"Catering   for  Individual Differences" project , teachers  are  encouraged  and

provided w ith opportunities to

 observe,

 reflect, and

 share ideas

 about their own

lessons as w ell as the lessons of other teachers. T he aim of this is for the teach-

ers to find

 optim al patterns

 of

 variation

 for

 dealing with

 the

 intended obje cts

 of

learning  in a

 lesson,

 a nd

 thus develop m ore pow erful w ays

 of

 helping their stu-

dents to  learn.

In  the first yea r of the p ro jec t , tw o pr im ary schools and a team  o f abou t 1 0

researchers were involved.

 In

 each school,

 two

 learning s tudy groups w ere

fo rmed, each com pr is ing

 five

 teachers .

 T he

 fo l lowing tab le sum m ar izes

 the

backgr ound in fo r m at ion

 of the

  groups (see Table 8.1) .

W orkshops on the theore tica l f r am ew ork of the pro jec t were conduc ted

wi t h  the teachers of each school before the learning s tudy. Fu ndin g was ob-

tained to allow an

 extra teacher

 to be

 employed

 in each school, so that each

of the 1 0

 teac he rs involved

  in the

  s tudy gained

 a

 n u m b e r

 o f

 extra

  free

 pe r i-

ods , thu s enab l ing them to take part. The m ee t ings w ere

 off icial ly

  scheduled

at

 suc h t im es that al l the teachers involved w ere free, and able to a t tend the

m e e t i n g .

T A B L E

  8.1

Ba c k gro u n d In f o rma t io n o f t he Le a rn i n g S t u d y G ro u p s

D u r i n g t he F i rst Year  of the Project

L e a r n i n g N u m b e r

  o f

  Teachers

School  Study Gro up  Leve l o f  Study  Subject  Part icipat ing

A

A

B

B

1

2

3

4

Pr imary   Grade 4

Pr imary

 G r a d e 4

Pr ima r y

 G r a d e 1

Pr imary  Grade 4

Chinese L a n g u a g e

Mathema t i c s

Ma the ma t i c s

Mathema t i c s

5

5

5

5

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 209/255

1 96 L O E T A L .

Ea ch o f the learning study group s consisted of at least one researcher, a se-

nior research assistant,

 and all

 teachers teach ing

 the

 same subject

 at the

 same

level in  that school. They m et once every we ek for 1 hour to plan the research

lessons. The group m eetings w ere audiorecorded, and summ ary notes were

kept

 of

 each m eet ing. Each cycle

 of the

 learning study consisted

 o f

 about

 10

meetings,

  and the

  whole process

  w as

  structured according

 to the six

 stages

described previously. Each research lesson  w as  taught , in r u m , by all of the

teachers

  in the

  group.

  A t

  least

  tw o

  lessons were videotaped,

 and

 then ana-

lyzed

 and

 discussed

 in the

 group.

 A n

 im proved version

 of the

 lesson

 was

 then

developed and w ritten up. The s tudents ' perform ance w as a l so m easured be-

fore

 and after  th e lesson. O n average, each group com pleted tw o research les-

sons

  in the first

  year. School-based

  and

  joint school seminars were a lso

scheduled during  the year to  facilitate  th e  sharing of experiences am ong the

four

  groups

 of

 teachers.

 Tw o of the

  learning studies

 are

 described here.

 W e

believe that these studies no t on ly illustrate th e concept of the learning s tudy

and the use o f variation theo ry, bu t also th e l inguis t ic con st i tu t ion of

  the

 space

of

  l earn ing

 (wh ich

  is

 dealt with

 in

 chap.

 3 of

 th is volum e) .

A  PRIMARY  GRAD E  4  MATHEMATICS  LESSON

O N   UNITS AN D  U N I T I Z I N G

T he

  fol lowing

  is a

  learning study that

  w as

  carried

  out by a

  team

  o f  five

teachers

 w ho

 taught m athemat ics

  at the

 primary Grade

 4

 level.

 T he

 entire

cycle (from the incuba tion of ideas, to reflection and eva luatio n) took a total

of

  nine meetings, which were distributed over

 a

 period

  of 3

 m o n t h s.

The Learning Study  Cycle

Choosing  th e  Object

  of

 Learning .  The first  step  was to  choose  a  topic .

T he  teachers chose  fractions, as  they  felt  tha t  fractions  presented some o f

the

 m o s t diff icul t  concepts

 for

 school ch i ldren

 at

 this level.

 T he

 seco nd step

was to f ind out why the  s tudents had  diff icul ty  lea rn ing fractions; in other

words ,

 w h a t

 i t was

  a b o u t

  fractions

 tha t m igh t

 be  di ff icul t  fo r

 s tudents .

Understanding

  W h a t

  Is

  i f f i c u l t

  and  Critical.

  The

  teachers reflected

  on

their ow n experiences of teaching fractions in the past, and shared these experi-

ences

 in the

 team m eetings.

 The

 follow ing questions w ere co ntinually raised:

•  W h a t is  actually learned, when learning about fractions?

• W hy i s

  learning fractions

  difficult?

  W h a t

  is it

 that

 makes

 learning

  fractions

  difficult?

• How can we conceptualize these  difficulties?

The un iversity consultant also introduced the

 teachers

 to the relevant re-

search literature, in

  particular

  Lamon's

 (1999)

 book

  about teaching

 and

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 210/255

8.  T O W A R D  A  P E D A G O G Y  O F

  L E A R N I N G

197

learning fract ions . Lam on argued that learning fract ion s con st i tu tes

 the ba-

sis for proportional reaso ning ,  and that the teaching of fract ions  should have

a

 pr imary

 focus o n concep tual unders tanding

  ra ther

 than procedura l know l-

edge.

 S he

 also pointed

 out

 that

 a

 cr i tical aspect

  of

 fractions  that s tudents f re-

quen t ly  fail

  to grasp w as  units

  an d  uni t iz ing.

T he

  team then reviewed

  th e

  students ' textbook

  fo r

 pedagogica l adv ic e .

To

 their surprise, they fo un d that

 the

 textbook

  not

 only lacked

 a

 form al treat-

m e n t  o f  uni ts  and  uni t iz ing,  bu t  that  its  presentat ion  o f  f r ac t ions  ac tua l ly

cou ld

  have contr ibuted

  to the

  s tudents ' misunders tand ing .

  T he

  chap te r

 o n

addit ion o f  f ract ions ,  fo r

 example , began w i th

  the

  fo l low ing d iag ram ( F ig .

8.1) ,

 accom pan ied

  by a

  question that asked

  for a

  fract io nal notat ion

  of the

shaded area:

F I G . 8 . 1 .

  T he

 d i a g r a m u se d

 in the

 c h a p t e r

 on add i t i on o f

 f rac t ions ,  from  Mathemat-

ics (Primary  4 , Firs t  Term)

 (

1997),

 p. 49.

 C o p y r i g h t

 ©

  1997

 by

 C a m b r id g e P u b l i s h e r s

Ltd.  Re p r in t e d w i th pe r mis s ion .

Having iden t i f ied un i t s

 and

 un i t iz ing

 a s a

 problem area,

 t he

 teache rs then

not iced that s tudents could have made

  tw o

 per fec t ly leg i t im ate guesses

 in

respon se to this que stion in the textb ook (i .e. , 14/8 or 1 4/16), dep en ding on

w hat they unders tood

 as one

 u nit . However,

  the

 textbook exp ected only

 o ne

answer (i .e. , 14/8);

 the

 tac i t assump t ion

  in the

 textbook bein g that each ci r -

cle  cons t i tu ted one un i t .

A t

  this stage, the teachers

  we re

  convinced that they should

  focus

  on

t each ing

 un i t s

 and

  uni t iz ing

 and

  make this

  the

  object

  o f

 learn ing

 of the re-

search lesson. They were also eager  to f ind out w hat p rio r know ledge the i r

s tudents would br ing

  to

  their study

  o f

 f r ac t ions .

Despite

  the  fact

  that several aspects

  o f

 f r ac t ions

 had

 a l ready been taug ht

in

  p r imary Grade  3, not all  teachers were sure that  the  s tudents  in t h e i r

c lasses unders tood

  the

  concepts wel l .

  In

  par t icular , they

  fel t

  tha t

  the

  s tu -

dents

 needed to

 understand

  the

 idea

  o f equal

  shares

 in

 fract ions ,

 as

 w e l l

 as

being ab le to con duct a s imple compar ison of f r ac t ions , befo re they could

unders tand

  the

  idea

  o f  uni t .

Developing   a  Test.  The

  teachers then designed

  a

  test

  for the

  s tudents .

T he purpose was to co l lec t in format ion about th e s tude nts ' way s o f th ink ing

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 211/255

198 LO ET AL.

about f ract ions , especially w i th regard  to those aspects that w ere expected

to be

 difficult. Furtherm ore,

  the

 test

 w as to be

 used

 a s a

 pretest

 to

 provide

 th e

team w ith a basel ine against wh ich the learning outcom es of the research

lesson

  (as

 measured

 by the

 same tes t ) could

 be

 com pared.

 T he

 test consisted

of three  main themes (exemplar questions

 are

  shown below):

1 .  The

  concept  o f

  equal shares  in

  fraction,

  for

  example ,

Ques t ion : What  fract ion  of the diagram  is

shown   by the  shaded area?

2 . Th e   c o n c e p t  o f fraction

  size

  ( r e s t r i c t e d  to t he  c o m p a r i s o n  o f

f r a c t i o n s

 w i t h e i th e r th e s a m e d e n o m i n a t o r o r t h e s a m e n u m e r a t o r , f o r

e x a m p l e ,

Question: Is

  1/2

 of a cake (greater  than/smaller than/equal to)

of  a cake?

3.

  T he  concept  of a uni t being referred  to by a  f ract ion ,  for example .

Quest ion: When   4 1 / 2 cakes are equa l l y d iv ided am ong 3 people,

how m any cakes does each person ge t? (You m ay draw diagram s

to

  find

  the answer.)

The resu l ts of the pretes t con fi rmed some of the teach ers ' spe cula t ions ,

and showed that there w ere few er s tudents

 who had

 m as tered

 the

 concept

 of

uni t in

 f ra ct ions than those

 w ho

 unders tood

  the

 concept

 of

 eq ua l shares ,

 o r

were capable

  of

 com paring simple frac t ion s (see T able 8.2).

Planning

 the Research Lesson .

  Inform ed by the pretes t resul ts, the

teachers began to plan the lesson. They agreed tha t there sho uld be som e re-

vision of equ al shares bu t decided that th is should be t a u g h t in a separa te les-

son prior to the research lesson. The research lesson shou ld beg in w i th

compar ing   fract ions  bu t

  focus

  ma in ly on  un i t s and  un i t i z ing .  There were

several rounds of lively discussion .  T he result w as a fairly detai led plan that

w as

  careful ly

  del iberated

 and

  ful ly

  di scussed.

A .  T h e Lesson  O p e n e r — C o n t e x tu a l iz a ti on

The plann ed lesson began by es tabl ishing a relevan ce s tructure ( i .e ., le t -

ting

 the

 s tudents view frac t ions

 from a

 po in t

 of

 view that they could im m edi-

ately

 experience

  as

 mean ingfu l ) .

 T he

 s tudents w ere

 to

 di scuss

 the

  fo l lowing

problem

 in

  small groups (see

 box on p.

  200):

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 212/255

TABLE   8.2

The

 Results

 of the Pretest

Them e Question

Freq.  o f  Correct

Responses

Classes

 A— E

(N

  =

  151)

E qua l

  sh are s W h at

  f ract ion

  of the diagram is

s h o wn  by the  shaded area?

Shade

  1/9 of a

  square.

Shade

  1/8 of a

 c i r c l e .

Shade

  1/3 of the  eggs  in the

  basket .

69

87

16

87

C o m p a r i n g

  1/2 of a

  cake

  is

  (greater than

  /

  eq u a l

 to /

  smal le r

f ract ions

  tha n) 1 /4 of the

  same

  cake

2/4 of a cake  is  (greater tha n  /  equa l  to /  smal le r

than)

  1/2 of the

  same cake.

2/5 of a kg is (g rea te r than /  equa l  to /  smal le r

t h an ) 4/5 of a kg.

1 1 2   of an hou r is (greater than / equal to /

smaller than) 1/200

  of an

  hour.

U n i t s

  and

  Divide

 2

  cakes in to three equal parts .

  How

U n i t i z i n g

  ma ny cakes

  are

  there

  in one

  part?

W hen 4 '/2 cakes

  are

  divided among three

persons ,  how  many cakes does each person

get?

W h a t

  fract ion  of

  squares

  is

 shown

  by

the

  black squares?

W h a t

  f ract ion

  of the

  to ta l number

  of

objects

  is

 shown

  by the

  black

  squares?

63

58

1 1 1

39

13

3

20

22

199

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 213/255

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 214/255

8.  T O W A R D

  A

  P E D A G O G Y

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  201

were

 in 1 /6 of

 their pac k.

  In

 this instance,

  the

  size

 of the

 pack varied w hi le

the

  fract ion

  rem ained the same. The teacher then posed the quest ion: W hy

are the  resul ts  different  w h e n  the  fract ion  is the  s ame?  T he  s tudents  ob-

served

 how the

 number

  o f

 candies changed w i th

 the

 changing un i t .

D .  Waff le Cakes

In

  the next s tage, the teacher gave each group a paper disc that repre-

sented a round

 waf f le

  cake . In thei r groups ,  the  s tudents had to cut out 1 /6

of the  cake . When they

  f in ished,

  they w e re given 2 discs ,  and 1 i/2 di scs in

tu rn , wi th

 th e

 same task

 o f

 cu t t i ng

 out 1 /6 of the

 am oun ts g iven . They then

had to comp are the am ount o f cakes o b ta ined, and see how the s ize o f the

uni t

 m a t te r e d .

 T h e

  d i f fe rence

  be tween th i s ac t iv i ty

 and the

  p rev ious

 o n e

was to

 al low s tude nts

 to see

 tha t

 th e

 concep t  o f

  uni t

 em braces bo th cou n t -

able

  an d

  uncoun tab l e amoun t s .

E .

  Back to the First

 Question

A t  the en d of the

  lesson,

  the

  teacher then returned

  to the two

 ques t ions

abou t the

 fund-rais ing event that were discussed

 at the

 beg inn ing

 of the

 les-

son. The s tudents w ere to w rite out thei r own answers to the qu est ions m ak -

ing

 use of

 w hat they

 had

  learned

  in the

  lesson.

Quest ions:

• W ho has donated  the m os t?

• W ho w as the   most generous?

Teach ing   t h e Les s on and

  Reflect ing

  on the Resu l ts .

  A ll f ive

  teachers

t au g h t their own classe s with the same plan. They also tried to observe the ir

col leagues teaching

  the

  same lesson,

  and

  shared their observations

  after-

ward w i th

 a

 view

 to

 m odi fy ing

 and

 improving

 the

 p lan . Al though there were

some s l ight var iations w hen each teacher im plem ented the lesson plan , th e

teachers

  a ll

  fol lowed

  the

  structure

  and

  sequence

  of

  act ivi t ies

 in a  fai thful

manner .

T he sam e tes t that w as  used  for the pretes t w as  adminis tered  to the s t u -

dents

 a few

 days

 after  the

 lesson

 had

 been taug ht .

 T he

  assessment i tems

 re-

lating

 to the

 notion

  o f

 un i t

 are

 shown

  in

 Table 8.3.

 T he

  difference  be tween

th e

 pretes t

  an d

 post tes t re sul ts indicates considerable learning gains

 by the

students .

After

  all of the

  teachers

  had

  t augh t

 the

  lesson, they

 met on a

 n u mb e r

 of

occasions to evaluate th e entire process. A l though the teachers felt that they

had

 been overam bitious

 and had

 packed

 too

 m uch in form at ion in to

 th e

 dou-

ble lesson (totaling 80 min.), they

 were

 in general q uite satisfied  w i th w ha t

they had achieved, and were pleased to learn about the students ' learning

outcom es. S uggest ions were m ade o n how to f ine- tune the lesson plan w i th

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 215/255

TABLE   8.3

The

 Dif ference  B e twe e n

 the P retest and

  Posttest Results

Theme  

Quest ion

Freq. of Correct

Responses

Classes

  A-E

(N

  =

  151)

Pretest/Posttest

(%  ga in

  o r

  loss)

Equal  shares

Comparing

fractions

Un i ts and

Uni t iz ing

What  fraction  of the diagram is shown

by  th e  shaded area?

Shade

  1/9 of a

  square.

Shade 1/8 of a

 ci rcle .

Shade

  1/3 of the

  eggs

  in the

  basket.

1 /2 of a cake is (greater tha n / equal to /

smaller than)  1/4 of the

 same

 cake.

2/4 of a cake is (greater tha n / equal to /

smaller than)

  1/2 of the

  same  cake.

2/5 of a kg is

  (greater tha n

 /

  equal

  to /

 smal le r

than) 4/5 of a kg.

1 /2

 of an hou r is (greater than / equal to /

smaller than) 1/200 of an hour.

Divide 2 cakes into three equal parts. How

man y cakes are there in one par t?

W h e n

  4 1 / 2

 cakes

  are

  divided among three

persons, how

 many cakes

  does

  each person

get?

W hat f raction of squares is shown by

the

 black squares?

What fraction

 of the

 total number

 o f

objects i s shown by the black squares?

45.7/68.9

*(+23.2%)

57.6/64.9

(+7.3%)

76 8/86 8

57.6/67.5

*(+9.9%)

41.7/50.3

*(+8.6%)

38.4/60.9

*(+22.5%)

73.5/81.5

(+8.0%)

25.8/45.0

8 7/4 7

(-4.0 )

2.0/44.7

*(+42.7%)

13.3/31 .3

14.7/32.0

*A  significant  change

 in

 percentage

  is observed

 w i t h / ?

  <

  0.05.

202

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 216/255

8.   T O W A R D

  A

  P E D A G O G Y

  OF

  L E A R N I N G

  203

more consolidation work   at differen t points  in the  lesson.  T he posttest  re-

sults  show ed that al l the classes obtained sub stantial gain when com pared

with the pretest res ults (see Table 8.4). T he  effectiveness of the lesson w as

thus

  evident.

A

  PRIMARY G RADE

  4

  C H I N E S E L E S S O N

O N

  TEXT CO MPREH EN S I ON

This  w as  another learning study carried  out in the  first  phase  of the  "Ca-

tering

  for

  Individual

 Differences

  (Bui ld ing

  in

  Variation)" project . Seven

m eet ings, one hour each, were used for the com plete cycle .

Stage  1: Choosing a

  S peci f ic  Object

 of

  Learning

After  m uch de l ibera tion , the teache rs decided to choose chapter  10 of their

course book,

  A

 Pen cil End ,  as the m ain teaching con tent . This chapter was

chosen

  for two

 main reasons:  First, according

  to

  their teaching schedule,

this

 lesson

 w as to be

 tau gh t around la te N ovember,

  and so the

 t im ing wo uld

be about right. Second, the text contained some special features that the

teachers con sidered to be w orthw hile for the s tudents to learn. The text was

in  th e form of a story w ritten  by a boy who

 felt

 that a 2-inch long pen cil end

w as

  too

  short

  to be

 use ful ,

 and so

 threw

  it

  away.

  He

  threw

  th e

 pencil

  end

away three t im es, and yet each t im e i t reappeared on his desk. Fina lly he re-

alized tha t i t w as his m other who had picked up the penc i l and put i t back on

his

 desk, hoping that

 he

 would

 u se it

 again

 and not be

 w as tefu l .

 The boy was

then ashamed  of his ow n action  and used  the pencil  to do his  homework.

In  subsequent m ee t ings , the  teachers  identified five w or thwhi le ob jec ts

of learning :

TA BL E   8.4

The

 Average

 Scores of Different Classes in the Pretests and Posttests

Cl a ss

 A

Class B

Class  C

Class

  D

Class  E

Whol e class

Pretes t :

Average Score

6.38

6.89

3.77

3.94

3.63

5.13

Posttest:

Average

  Score

8.78

9.14

4.83

5.31

4.78

6.85

Both

  Tests:

Total

  Score

12

12

12

12

12

12

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 217/255

204 LO ET AL .

1.

  In the

  text,

 th e

  same action (throwing away

 the

 penci l end) appeared

three t im es, yet the w ay in w hich the sen t ime nt of the boy w as conveye d in

each

 case

 w as

  different.

  T he

 t eachers w ished

 to

 draw s tuden ts ' a t ten t ion

 to

the way in

 w h i ch

 the

 cho i ce

 of the

 ve rb s m a tched

 the

 ac t ions

 and the

 verbal

express ions ,

  in

 order

  to

  show esca la t ing

 feel ings  o f

  d is l ike .

2.  In the text , there were  six words wi th the

 "hand"

 rad ica l [-f- ] or [ ^ ].

These

 words can be considered  to be in the s ame sem an t ic f ield as the word

n a h (Cantonese)  [^  ] ( m e a n in g

 "take"); al l the

 w o r ds

 a re

 associa ted w i th

some act ion us ing the hand . Yet , each w ord has a s l ig ht ly

 dif ferent

  m e a n -

ing .

 For e x a m p le , the w o r d  taih  ( Ca n t o n e s e ) [ -^ ] has the  m e a n in g of

"carry , take ho ld of , or

 l i f t i ng

  s o m e t h i n g o u t " ; t h e w o r d

 g im

  ( C a n t o n e s e)

[^ct]

 has the

 m e a n i n g

 o f

 "select

 a nd

 p ick up" ;

 wing/\ihng

  ( C a n t o n e s e ) [ty]

has the m ean ing o f "throw";  laai  (C an tonese ) [ 4& ] has the m ean ing o f

"puH";y/p (Cantonese) [4lc] has the m e a n in g o f "catch  o r receive";

  touh

(Cantonese)  [}£) ] has the m e a n in g

 offish

  out ." In w r it ten l an gua ge , de-

pending   on the con tex t,  it wou ld be

 more

 ap propr ia te  to use one of  these

specif ic  w ords ra ther than the w o r d

 "take"

 [ ̂ ] in order to convey m ore

p rec i s e mean ings a n d m a k e th e w r i tt en wo rk more e l eg an t . Y et in spoken

Ca n t o n e s e ,

  it w o u l d b e p e r f ec t l y accep t ab l e , and in

  fact

  m o re n a t u r a l , to

use the

 spoken  form

 of "take" [ ̂ ],

  w h i c h

 is l ing

 (C an tonese ) [^] .

  The

teachers w i shed  to teach  th e  s tudents how to di s t ing uish th e me a n i n g a n d

usage

 o f

 each

 o f

 these w ords . Also, they w anted

 the

 s tudents

 to

  appreciate

tha t in w r i tt en l anguag e , the use of w ords h as to be mo re precise  in order to

bring   out the  exact meaning  to be  conveyed.

3.  T he text showed the different  personal i t i es of the bo y and his m other.

The boy was

 very imp at ien t

 and

 showed

 h is

  l ikes

 and

 dis l ikes ove rt ly .

 The

mother  w as  more gen t le  and  sub t le .  T h e  t eachers sugges ted  that  the

mother's  example could show

  the

  students

  how to

 relate

  to

  other

 people,

such

  as

  their classmates.

4. In the

 last paragraph ,

  the

 mother in troduced

 the

 idea

 o f

  conservation.

The

 teachers w ished

 to

 cu lt ivate

 in

 their students

 an

 awareness

 o f the

 envi-

ronm ent and a posi tive at ti tude tow ard con servat ion.

5.

  I t was the  po l icy  of the  school that  the  Chinese panel head should

specify

  the  l earn ing ob jec t ives  fo r  each chapter  at the  b e g i n n i n g  of the

school term. For th is chapter , a num ber of w ords and senten ce pat terns had

already been iden tif ied as teaching object ives , and these w ere to be assessed

in

 th e exam ination .

On ly  tw o periods h ad been  ass igned  for the  t each ing  o f th is chapter ,

and the t eachers  fel t  t ha t the re w ou ld no t be eno ug h tim e to cover eve ry -

th ing t ha t they w i shed

 to

 t eac h . They adm i t t ed t ha t they of ten

  had to

 rush

t h r o u g h t h i n g s  super f ic ia l ly ,  an d  tha t there  w as  l i t t le  t i m e  fo r  i n -dep th

t r ea tme n t . As a r e su l t , m any s tuden t s w ere no t ab l e to l ea rn w e l l . Th i s

had  a lways  been  the teachers' problem.

  Af t e r

  much di scuss ion , they

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 218/255

8.

  T O W A R D A P E DA G O G Y O F  L E A R N I N G  205

agreed tha t

  fo r the

 research lesson, they w ou ld

 try to

 f o c u s

 o n

 f e we r

 o b-

j ec t s

  o f

 lea rn ing,

  and

 dea l wi th each

 on e in

  grea te r depth . They dec ide d

no t

 to

 f o c u s

 on the

 affect ive aspec ts (objects

 o f

 le a rn ing num bers

  3 an d 4)

a t

  th i s poin t , because they

  fel t

  that these ideas should be

  in f i l t r a ted

  in

the i r l e ssons th ro ugh ou t  th e  year .  O f the  three remaining aspec ts , they

fel t  tha t

  th e

  choice

  o f

 w h i ch

  to

  f o c u s

 o n

 shou ld depend

  o n the

  s t u d e n t s '

pr ior exper ience and knowledge . There fore , to he lp them make an in-

fo rmed  decis ion, they decided  to  wai t unt i l they could obta in informa-

t ion about the s tudents

  from

  the pre tes t .

Stage

  2:

  F i n d i n g

 Out the

  E x t e n t

  to

  W h i c h

 the  S t u d e n t s

H a v e

 Developed

  th e

  I n t e n d e d C a p a b i l i t i e s

A  pretest w as  administered  to all five  classes  of 148 students. O n average,

over 85% of students in each class (m ore than 70% in the w eakest c lass) dem -

onstrated an un derstanding of the words and sentence patterns

  specified

  by

th e

  panel  head (object

  of

  learning number

  5), and

  were able

  to use

  these

words

 to fill in

 app ropriately

  the

 blanks

 o f a

 text. Therefore,

 the

 teachers

 de-

cided

 that

 it

 would

 not be

 necessary

 to

 focus

 o n

 these aspects

 in the

 lesson.

To

 test

  the

  s tudents ' understanding

 of the

  w ords w i th

  the

  hand radica l ,

two que stions were set. In the first question , three sentence s w ere given . For

each sentence, three   characters

 o f

 very sim ilar m eaning w ere provided

  and

the  s tudents had to choose  the m ost appropria te o ne for the par t icula r con-

text.

 In the

 second question, students were required

 to f ill in

 seven blank s

 to

com ple te a

 text. Seven w ords w ith

 the

 hand radical were pro vided

 and

 s tu-

dents

 had to

 choose

 the

 m ost appropria te w ord

 for

 each b lank.

 The

 resul ts

 of

the

  test showed that students

 had difficulties

  dist inguishing words with

 the

hand

 rad ica l .

 The

  word that

 w as

  found

  to be

 m o s t

  difficult

  for the

 s tudents

w as "fish out" [$0]. In the first que stion, on ly 10% of the stu dents cou ld use

th e

 word "fish out" [$0] appropriately.

 In the

 second qu est ion, only

 tw o

 s tu-

dents in one class, Class 4C, got all the answ ers correc t . A ga in, the character

"fish  out"  [ $0 ] w as found to be the most  difficult,  as only  10% of the stu-

dents used it correctly. Other  difficult  words were "take"  [

  ^ ]

 (20% co r-

rect),

 "carry"

 [H]  (30% correct), and

 "pick

 up" [^]  (30% correct). Th us, it

becam e clear that w ords with  th e h and radical needed to be given special a t-

tention,

 especia l ly

 the

 words

 "fish

 out"

 [

 - J £ )

 ],

 "take"

 [ ̂ ], and

 "carry"

 [^ ].

Another question required

  the

 students

 to put two

 groups

 of

 w ords

  in or-

der of the strength of  feel ing  expressed. The first group consisted of the

words

 "excited"

  [ - *

-f^], "pleased" [Ht -§-], and "happy"  [ ' j Jf£].The second

group co nsisted of the wo rds

 "angry"

 [£_

  i j ( 3 ,

  "discontented"  ffi  / * £ ] and " fu-

rious" ['1^ ^J]. On ly abo ut 30% of studen ts w ere ab le to put the first group o f

words  in the

 correct

 order, and about 60% of  students  were able  to put the

second gro up o f wo rds in the correct order.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 219/255

2 06 L O E T A L .

To  test w hether students w ere able

 to

 m atch

 the

  verbal expressions cor-

rectly with  the  feel ings o r  sentiments expressed, they were asked  to m a tch

three phrases with three verbal expressions.

  For

  example, students

  had to

match the phrase, "M other said kind ly" with

 "Don't

 give up, you just have

to  work harder next time." I t was  found that about  60% of  students could

ma tch

  the

  phrases with

 the

 verbal expressions correctly.

Th e resu lt of the pretest prom pted the teache rs to ma ke the decision to fo-

cus on the

  first

  tw o  objects of  learning:

• To be able to appreciate that the choice of verbs should m atch the actions,

and that the verbal expressions should effectively  convey sentim ents.

• To be able to dis tingu ish the m ean ing and usage o f some w ords w i th

the hand radical , and to apprecia te that in writ ten lang uag e , the use of

words has to be  precise.

Stage 3:

  D e s i g n i n g

  a

 Lesson  (Lessons)

  A i m e d

  to

  D e v e l o p

th e

  I n t e n d e d C a p a b i l i t i e s ( T h e I n t e n d e d

  Objects  of

  L e a r n i n g )

Th e teachers shared their usua l m ethods of teach ing a text , w hic h typical ly

included going through

  the

  fo l lowing

 five

  stages:

1.  B r ie f ly expla in ing

 the

  text

  as a

 w h o le

2. R eading the text a loud

3. E xp laining new characters and w ords

4.

  Going through

 the

 questions

  at the end of the

 text

 and

 looking

 at

 each

paragraph in m ore deta i l

5.  Teaching some  of the  sentence patterns  and  special  l inguis t ic  fea-

tures  in the text .

T he

 teachers

 in

 general

  felt

  that this routine, w hich they

 had

 been us ing

all

 a long, w as som etim es boring, and that it failed to provide a structure that

helped

 the

 students

 focus on the

 objects

 o f

 learn ing. They hoped

  that fo r the

research lesson , they could com e up w ith a better w ay of structuring the les-

son   p lan  in  order  to  help  the  students  focus  on the  objects o f  lea rn ing by

means of suitable variations.  I t was decided that tw o lessons w ould be used

fo r  teaching—one  for each object of lea rn ing. For the purpose  of th i s chap-

t e r ,

  only

 the

 lesson deal ing w ith

 the

 second objec t

 o f

 l e arn ing— w ords w i th

the hand radical— is described. Th e lessons of two c lasses (Class 4C and

Class 4H) were videotaped, and o nly data from these tw o c lasses are used

fo r

  the analysis.

The  Lesson  Plan:  Teaching

  W o r d s W i t h

  the Hand Radical .  The  l e s s o n

plan con tained the follow ing essential features, wh ich w ere aim ed at bring-

ing

  ou t  similari t ies a nd

 differences,

  contrasts, variation a nd invariance:

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 220/255

8.

  T O W A R D A

  P E D A G O G Y

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  207

1 .  Students shou ld  find all the w ords showing act ion that had a s im ilar

meaning

  to

 "take,"

 and

 that used

 the

 hand radical . Then they sho uld

com m ent on the text

 after

 all

 these words

 had

 been replaced w ith

 the

word

 "take."

2.

  W ords w i th

 th e

 hand radica l should

 b e

 d iscu ssed toge ther ,

  to

 show

the i r s imi la r i t i e s and cont ras t the i r d i f fe rences , for example ,

"drop"

 [- •]

  and

 "throw" [#}];  "pickup"  [ j f c ] ;  "pickup"

 [^] and

"take"[$r].

3.  V ariations shou ld be used to help students discern the m eanin g of the

words ,

 for

 exam ple , using exam ples

 and

 nonexam ples.

 F or

 each pair

of words, a sentence was to be used and i ts structure kept invari-

ant—only   the  word with  the  hand radical would be  changed.

  This

w as

  intended

  to

  enable students

  to

  discern

  the

  subtle

  difference  in

m eanings of the

  different

  words.

Stage

 4:  Teaching the Research Lesson

( the E na cted

 Object  of

  L e a r n in g )

The

  Lesson

  of

 Class

  4 C .  The  teacher  first  asked  th e  students  to  find  all

the words w ith

 the

 same m eaning

 as "take" [ ̂ ]. She

 then pointed

 out that it

w as

  c o m m o n

  for the

  students

 to

  take

  all

  these w ords

  as

  synonymous ,

  and

that they tende d to use only the word "take," instead of m aking use of these

different

  words .

 She

 told them th at

 if

 they a lw ays

 did

 this, however,

 the text

w o u l d

 becom e very boring. S he used the m etaphor of their m other cooking

th e  same dish for every m eal; even thou gh i t was  good  to eat at the begin-

ning,

 it

 w ould become bor ing after

  a

 w hi le .

 T he

 teach er then

 focused  on the

five

 wo rds:

 "receive"

 [ ̂ ], "fish out" [ ̂ ) ], "take" [ ̂ ],

 "carry"

 [ ̂ %] , and

"pick   up" [  % fc ]. These w ere writ ten  on the  board.

She attended to the w ords one at a time, always

 com paring

 each o ne w i th

the w ord

 "take"

 [ ̂ ]. She used a sentence in which the use of e i ther w ord

was poss ib le . The  difference  in m eaning of the two w ords was m ade more

prominent by put t ing them in the same context . By keeping the sentence

structure invariant and varying  the wo rds,  the

 teacher

 focused the  students'

attention   on  th at w hich varied, that  is, the  difference  in the  m eaning con-

veyed by the tw o sentences becau se o f the

 difference

  in m eaning of the tw o

words . Fo r example , sh e dealt with the tw o w ords "take" [

 ̂

  ] an d "receive"

[ ̂ - ] in the

 fol lowing

 way.

The

 teac her asked

  the

  students

 to

 com pare

  the two

  sentences:

He  took  the penci l  from  her hand.

He  rece ived the penci l f rom h er hand.

She expla ined that w hen the word

 "received"

 [  ̂ ] was u sed in the second

sentence, there was an im plication that the one holding the pencil (sh e) was

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 221/255

208 LO ET AL.

giving

 it to the recipient

 (him).

 When the

 word

 "took" [ ̂ r ] was

 used,

 it had

a  different  imp l icat ion. The teacher added that the wo rd "took" in the first

sentence m ight

 give

 us the feeling that "he"  was no t very p olite because he

took wi thout asking,

  o r

  w i thout be ing g iven

  the

  penci l

  in the f irst

 p l ace .

"Took" in the context o f the first sentence m eans that the ac tor is "he,"  and

"she"  w as passive.

 "Receive"

 in the context  of the  second sentence means

that

  the actor  is

 "she,"

 and "he"  is the pass ive recipien t .

T he  teacher dea l t w i th the  other words  in a s imi l a r way .

The  Lesson of Class

 4H .

  The

 teacher

 first

 read

 out

 loud

 a

 piece

 o f

 text.

 In

this

 text , the word

 "take"

 was used m any t im es, som etim es inappropriately.

T he

 teacher asked

  the

 students

 i f

 they noticed w hich w ord

 w as

 used most

 of-

ten. Many students were able

  to  identify  th e

 word

 "take."

 Then

  th e

 teacher

asked, "What would y our teacher think  if you wrote a  composition with so

m a n y

 "take's?"

 M any students responded that the t eacher would n ot

  like

 it.

T he

  teacher then asked

  the

 students

  to find all the

 words associated w i th

the

  act ion

  of the

  hand. Nine words were  found.

  S he

  then explained

  the

meaning  o f each w ord, role-played  the  act ion conveyed,  and analyzed  the

action of the han d. Fo r each pair of w ords, she also kept the sen tence struc-

ture invariant ,

 b u t

 changed

  the

 verb

 and

 asked

  the

 s tudents

 to

 c o m p a re

 the

tw o

  resu l t ing sen tences .

 For

  example , w i th

 the

 w o r ds

 "drop" [ ̂  ] and

"throw"

 [ /}],

 she gave the fol lowing sentences :

  Don ' t  drop  banana skins onto

  the

  floor.

  Don ' t

  throw

 banana skins onto

  the

 floor.

She asked som e s tudents to com e out and act out how they "drop" some-

thing, and how

  they "throw" som eth ing .

 In the

  subsequen t d i scuss ion ,

 she

focused

  on the

  action

  of the

 hand

 —

 w hether

  the

 action

  w as

 large

 or

 sm al l,

the  size of the  swing , and the angle made by the arm. Then  she said that o f

the tw o words that were used  to fill in the blank  in the  same sentence struc-

ture, only

 one of

 them

 w as

 correc tly used

  and

 asked

 the

 students

 to

 ident i ty

the correct  one.

S he

 dea l t w i th

 the

 rest

 of the

 words

 in

 pai rs

 and in a

 s imi l a r way .

 In

 each

case, she focused on the physical movem ent of the ac t ion. For exam ple, she

stressed that with the wo rd

 "carry"

 [ H; ] , the arm should be held s t raight

down .

Final ly , the

 teache r reread

  the

 text tha t

 she had

 read

 ou t at the

 b e g i n n i n g

of the lesson. This t ime, the s tudents suggested approp riate w ords to replace

the word "take" in each ins tanc e.

The  En ac ted

 Ob ject  of

  Learning .

  W e see  tha t bo th t eachers seemed  to

have taug ht

 in

 very s im i lar w ay s. How ever, there

 w as a

 very im po r tan t d i f -

ference

  in  the i r ways  o f  dea l ing wi th  th e  ob jec t  o f  l ea rn ing . Wi th  th e

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 222/255

8.  T O W A R D  A  P E D A G O G Y  O F  L E A R N I N G  209

t eacher

 of C lass 4C, the  focus w as on the subt le d ifference  in m ean ings o f

the

 di f ferent  w ords. By keeping the sentence s t ructure invariant and vary -

ing

 the wo rd used,  she

 helped

 the s tuden ts to focus on the d i f fe rent mean -

ings

 of the words . These m eanings w ere made expl ic i t by the ir re la t ion to

the l in gu ist ic con text , that is , the rest of the sen tences. Fo r exam ple the ac-

tor versus rec ip ient meaning—impl ic i t

  in the

  words  "took"  versus "re-

c e i v e , " r e s p e c t i v e l y — wa s m a d e e x p l i c i t

  b y t h e

  i n v a r i a n t s e n t e n c e

structure.

  On the

 other hand,  despite

 the

  fact  that

  the teacher o f

 Class

 4H

also  presented

 tw o

 wo rds

 in the

 same l ingu is t ic context ,

 she

 focused

 on the

m eanin g of each word o n i ts ow n and the ac t ion of the hand conveyed by

each w ord. Th e teach er did not re la te the wo rd to the l ing uis t ic con text to

help

  s tuden ts d iscern

  the

  d ifferences

  in

 mean ing . Th is me thod

 o f

 dea l ing

wi th

 the mean ings o f w ords in a

 decontextual ized m anner

 c an create prob-

l e m s  fo r

  learners

  o f

  Ch inese ,

  as one

  charac te r

  m ay

  change

  its

 m e a n i n g

w hen u sed toge ther w i th another charac te r (or charac te rs ) to form a w ord

or a phrase . For exam ple , when the teacher fo cused on the mean ing of the

wo rd

 "carry"

  [ _ ] ,  sh e em phas ized tha t i t had the mean ing tha t the arm

should

 b e

 held straight do w n. This

 is a

 correct interpretation

  if the

 word

 is

used

 in the co ntex t of

 "carrying

 a br ie fcase /shop ping bag," bu t not correc t

wh e n  it is used in the con tex t of

 "car ry ing

 a lantern," for example , in w h i ch

case

  the arm

 w ould p robab ly

  be

 ben t

 a t the

  e lbow.

Stage 5:

  F i n d i n g

 the Students

L e a r n i n g

 Outcomes

Because

 the obje ct of learning w as to help the students to discern the sub tle

differences  in the m ean ing of the different w ords associa ted w ith the act ion

of the

 hand ,

 and to be

 able

 to use

 them approp riately,

  the

 pos ttest also tested

such

 a capab ility in the students. Judging  from  the enacted o bject of learn -

ing

 of the tw o c lasses (and i f the way in w hich the teacher deal t w i th the ob-

j ec t

  of

  learning

  is  inf luent ia l ) ,  it

  would

  be

  expected that

  the

  s tudents

 o f

Class 4C should perform bet ter than those of Class 4H. This was indeed

borne out by the resul ts of the post test. The quest ions w ere the same in both

the pretest  and the  posttest. Table  8.5  shows  the  results  of the  quest ion

where s tudents were given seven words with the hand radical , and were

asked  to fi ll in seven b lank s in a text with the approp riate w ords.

Th e resu lts of the post-test show ed that the students of Class 4C dem on -

strated

  significant

  imp rovem ent . In the pre test, on ly two students were able

to

  choose

  all

  seven words correctly,

  but in the

  posttest,

  1 7

 students

  got all

seven

 w ords co rrect . Of the five w ords that the teach er attended to, al l stu-

dents in the

 c lass w ere able

 to fill in

 three w ords correct ly: "receive"

 [ ̂   ],

"fish out," [^)], an d "carry" [ ^ % ] .

 S ignif icant

 im provement w as a lso found

in   Class 4H, however the improvement was not as great as in Class 4C.

About

  1 0

 students

 go t all seven w ords

 correct.

 For the

 w ord "fish out"

  [

 $0],

the re la tive improvem ent w as about 47% in Class 4H, w hereas the im prove-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 223/255

TABLE   8.5

Table  to Compare the Results of the Pretest and the Posttest

Fil l ing

  in the

  Blanks

  With  th e

 Appropriate

  Wor ds

N u m b e r  of  S tuden t s C orrec t  (Percentage)

St u den t

n u m b e r

Class  4C   pretes t  33

Class 4C

 posttest

  33

Rela t ive improvement

4H

  pretest

  3 1

4H

  pos t tes t

  3 1

R el a t i ve

  i m p r o v e m e n t

[1 ]

 

pick

  u p

14

( 41%)

24

(73%)

54%*

7

(23%)

1 1

(35%)

51%*

[2 ]

#5

t h row

26

(74%)

27

(82%)

31 %

1 1

(35%)

18

(58%)

35%*

[3]

 

receive

24

(68%)

33

( 1 00%)

100%*

9

(29%)

22

( 71%)

59%*

[4]

 

pul l

27

(82%)

30

( 9 1 % )

50%

17

(55%)

24

(77%)

49%*

[5 ]

ft

fish  ou t

9

(27%)

33

( 100%)

100%*

2

(6%)

16

(52%)

47%*

[6 ]

 

t ake

14

(42%)

32

(97%)

95%*

9

(29%)

18

(58%)

4 1 % *

[7]

 

carry

14

(42%)

33

(100%)

100%*

10

(32%)

18

(58%)

38%*

Key.  Rela t ive  i m p r o v e m e n t

 =

  i n e r e a s e

 in % -

  m a x i m u m p o s si b l e in c r e a se

  in %.

* Words th at h ad  been  spec i f i ca l ly  dea l t w i t h  in the  lesson  by the  t eacher .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 224/255

8.   T O W A R D A  P ED A G O G Y  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  2t t

m e n t was  100% in C lass 4C. For the word

 "carry"

 [ t̂. ], the relative im-

provement

  in

 C lass

 4H w as 38% and in

 C lass

 4C, i t w as

  100%.

In the  question that required students  to choose the  m ost appropriate

word

  to be

  used

  in a

  sentence,

  i t was

  also

  found

  tha t

 the

  students

  in

 both

classes made significant improvement

  in the

  understanding

  of the

  word

"fish  out"

 [ ̂ ]. In 4H, the

 improvement

 was

 from

 10% to 61% of

 s tudents

getting

 it

 correct,

 whereas in

 Class

 4C, the

 im provement

 was

 again greater,

from

  21% to

  94%.

A s w e m ent ioned

 earlier,

 these  tw o studies w ere some  of the first  studies

(carried  ou t

 w i th in

  a

  research project with developmental ambit ions)

 that

w ere based on the part icular theory of learning described brief ly in chapter

1.

  Both s tudies proved

  to be

  successfu l

  in

  te rms

  of

  gains

  in

  s tudent ou t-

comes. Furthermore , they showed

  a

  phenomenon tha t

  w as

  also

  found  in

later

  s tudies: Al though  the  teachers  had  agreed  on the  intended object of

learning, the enacted object o f learning varied betw een the groups, and that

the

 res ults (the l ived object

  o f

  learning) varied acco rdingly.

Since these s tudies w ere carr ied ou t , m any others have fo l low ed

 (22 by

th e end of the second year of the project), m ostly w ith results that are very

m u c h  in

  l ine with

  the

  resul ts

 of the two

  studies described

  in

  th is chapter.

(Reports

  on other

 learning studies conduc ted

  in

 this project

 can be found on

the Internet at this address: http:/ /cidv.hku .hk)

The two

 studies

 w e

 have jus t looked

  at

 cam e

 f rom a

 project w ith

 a

 strong

emphas is

  on the

  enhancement

  o f

  teachers ' professional development .

  In

this way, the a im s  of the project were close  to those o f the Japanese lesson

study

 m ode l.

 The aim of the

  study described

  in the

  fo l low ing sect ion

 w as

primari ly to test theoretical con jecture s, and thus this s tudy w as c loser to the

idea o f the design experiment .

A

  SECONDARY

  GRADE

  4

  E C O N O M I C S

  LESSON

O N T H E

 I N C I D E N C E

  O F A

  SALES

 TA X

The a im of

  this learning s tudy

  w as

  threefold:

  to  find  a pow er fu l  way for

teachers  to help secondary Grade 4 students h andle a rather diff icult bu t im-

por tant economic con cept ;

 to

 evaluate

 th e  effectiveness  o f a

 learning s tudy

in

 the

 improvement

 of

  teaching

 and

 learning

 (in comparison to the

  lesson

s tudy

  approach) ; and to test the tenet o f a specific  theory  o f  learning

 (i.e.,

the  theory

 of

 variation— that learning

 to

 experience

 som ething

 in a

 certain

w ay  is

 cont ingent

 on the

 pattern

 o f

 variation

 in the

 c ri t ical aspects

 of the ob-

j ec t

 of

  learning).

 The

  investigation also promised

  to

  demonstrate that

 a

learning

 s tudy cou ld

 be a

 design experiment (B rown, 1992; C ol l ins , 1992)

and

  serve primarily

  as a

 research tool .

T he ob jec t of learning for this study was the capa bili ty o f taking into ac-

count the no tion of the relative elastici ty of dem and and sup ply in determ in-

ing   the

  distribution

  of a tax

  burden between buyers

  and

  sellers result ing

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 225/255

2 1 2 L O E T A L .

from the

  introduction

  of a

 sales tax.

 I t was

  envisaged that

 if

 students could

develop

  an

 economic

 perspective  in  understanding  th e  topic  of the  inci-

dence

  of a

  sales tax,

  the  different

  w a y s

  in

 w hich they were able

  to

 under-

stand

 the

 econom ic phenom enon w ould increase.

Th e  Economic

  Topic

 Used  in the

  S t u d y

The

  economic topic used

  in

 this study

 is

  "the incidence

 o f a

 sales tax," taken

from the

  Hong Kong economics curriculum

 for

  secondary Grades

  4 and 5.

When

 a

 governm ent levies

 a

 sales

 tax on

 goods

  and

 services,

 a

 certain percent-

age

 of the

 cost

 of our

 purchase

 as

 consumers

 is

 paid

 to the

 government.

 A t

 pres-

ent, Hong Kong

 does

 not

  have

  a

  general

  sales

  tax,

  but a

  limited number

  of

commodities such

  as

  tobacco

  and

  liquor incur excise  duty  (i.e.,

  a

 particular

form

 of

 sales ta x).

 The

 Hong Kong government

 is

 cu rrently considering

 a

 gen-

eral sales tax,

 and

 this

 has led to

 heated discussions am ong

 the

 general pub lic.

M ost people believe that

 if the

  general sales

  tax is

 introduce d,

 the

 sellers

 wil l

transfer

  th e

  entire

  tax

 burden

  to the

 buyers,

  and

 simply

 raise the

 price

  of the

commodities

 by the

 amount

 of

 sales tax.

The

 object

 of

 learning

  for

 this study

 was the

 capa bil i ty

 o f

 taking into

 ac-

coun t the notion of the relat ive elast ici ty of dema nd a nd su pply in determin -

ing the distribution of a tax burden betw een buy ers an d sel lers tha t results

from   the introduction  of a sales tax . I t was  envisaged that if students could

develop  an  economic perspective  in thei r unde rs tanding of the  topic of the

incidence

 of a

 sales tax ,

 th e

 different  w a y s

 in

 which they were able

 to

 under-

stand

  the

  econom ic phenomenon w ould increase .

The view th at if a general sales tax is introduce d, the sellers  wi l l transfer

the entire  ta x burden  to the buyers by raising  th e price  of the com m odities  by

the

 amoun t

 of

 sales tax,

 is not  fully

  supported

 by

 econom ics . Econom ists

 ar-

gue that instead of mak ing the buyers shoulder the

 full

  tax burden in every in-

stance,

  th e

 distribution

 of the tax on a

 part icular com m odity depends

  on the

relative elasticity

 of

 demand

 and

 supply

 of

 that com m odity. A ccording

 to the

law  of demand and supply, whe n prices of a com m odity increase,  th e quan tity

demanded  falls, an d the qu antity supplied rises. Sellers are w il ling to supply

more

 of a

 com m odity w hen they

 can

 earn more

  for

 wh at they sell; buyers

 are

less keen  on purchasing a com m odity w hen they get less for their money. The

effect

  on prices is therefore not a unifo rm price increase on all goods.

People need some

 com m odities

 more

 than

 others;

 w ater

  and

  staple foods

are the classic exam ples. Even when the price o f these com m odities

 nses,

 the

quanti ty demanded wil l n ot drop sign ificantly. In other w ords,  th e demand is

said

 to be

 inelastic,

 or the

 elasticity

 of

 demand

  is low

 ( the quan ti ty dem anded

does

 not

 change

  as

 mu c h

 as the

 price).

 For

 other com m odities such

 a s

 luxury

cars an d jewelry, demand  is highly

 susceptible

 to

 price

 change,  and the quan-

t ity demanded changes m ore than

 th e

 change

 in

 price. Such dem and

 is

 said

 to

be

 elastic,

 or the

 elasticity

  of

 demand

  is

 said

  to be

 high.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 226/255

8.

  T O W A R D

  A

  P E D A G O G Y

  O F  L E A R N I N G  21 3

Based

 o n a

 com m onsense understanding, sellers

 of

 com m odities with

 in-

elastic

 deman d w ill sim ply add the new ly introduce d sales tax to the current

prices,

 and the

 buyers w ill then

 bear th e

 entire

 tax

 burden. A ccording

 to

 eco-

nomic analysis, however, when determining   the  sharing  of the tax burden,

th e

 seller 's condition should also be taken into accou nt. For instance, for sta-

ple

  foods (such

  as

 potatoes

 o r rice),

 people's demand tends

 to be

  inelastic,

but  in the case of an especially good harvest , sellers have piles of unsold

stock

  in

  hand that they need

  to

  sell before

  the

  food  spoils.

  The

  supply

  is

more

  or

  less  fixed  over

  th e

  period

  and so is not

  very sensitive

  to

  price

changes .

 In

 such

 a

 case

 of

 oversupply,

 the

 supply

 is

 said

 to be

 very inelastic

and sellers m ay need to arrange to absorb part o f the sales tax in their p rice,

instead of passing i t on to the consum ers.

In general , the m ore elastic the dem and and the less elastic the supply, the

greater likelihood of the sales tax being absorbed in the price, and so the

sellers will share more of the tax burden than the buyers. Conversely, the

less elastic the dem and and the more elastic the supply, the greater the likeli-

hood of the w hole sales tax being added to the previous prices, in w hich case

the buyers w il l bear the w hole tax burden. How ever, in a s i tuat ion w hen both

the demand for and the supply of a commodity are elastic or inelastic, we

mu s t  compare the elastici ty of demand and the elastici ty of supply. If the

elasticity

  of

 dem and

  is greater

 than

 th e

 elasticity

  of

 supply,

 the buyers

 have

the upper hand, and vice versa. In the case of staple foods,  for exam ple, de-

m a n d

  is

  usually more inelastic than supply,

  so the

  buyers have

  to

  carry

  a

greater part of the tax burden. O verall , the distribution of the tax burden b e-

tween buyers  and  sellers  is a  function  of the  relative elasticity  of  demand

and supply.

Des i g n

  of the

  S t u d y

Two groups, each with

  five

  economics teachers, part icipated in the main

study. On e of the groups fol low ed the Japanese lesson s tudy m odel . In this

group,

 the teachers and a researcher

 (who

 did not

 take

 the lead)

  discussed—

during three preparatory meetings, each of which lasted for around 2

hours—the ways in which the object of learning could best be handled.

Draw ing on their ow n experiences and the resul ts of a pi lot s tudy in w hich

students ' qu alitatively

  different

  understandings

  of the

 incidence

  of a

  sales

ta x

  w ere assessed, the group developed a jo in t lesson plan for a series of

four  lessons, w hich were then taugh t  in  five  different  classrooms.

The procedure was basical ly the same for the other group of teachers ,

with  one ma jo r difference .  In this group , the researcher actively participated

in the discussion, and introduced the theory of variation a s a tool for devel-

oping a lesson plan to the teachers. B oth the lesson p lan and the enacted ob-

jects

  of

  learning demonstrated teachers ' understanding

  of the

  theory

  of

variat ion

 as

 applied

 to the

 part icular object

 of

 learning.

 In the

 fol lowing  sec-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 227/255

214 LO ET AL.

tion, this grou p is refe rred to as "the learning study group," whereas the

  first

group

  is

 referred

  to as

  "the lesson study

 group." T he

  lesson study group

served

 as a

 reference

 to

 reveal

 the effect  of the

 instru ction al design based

 o n

the

 theory

 of

  variation.

Using

  th e

  data obtained

  on

  teaching

 and

  learning, both inter-

 a nd

  intra-

group

 comparisons were

 conducted in

 order

 to

 explore

 the relationship be-

tween

  th e

  enacted objects

  o f

  learning

  and

  student understanding. This

investigation therefore com prises

 a

 learning study

 and a

 lesson study, with

 th e

primary em phasis being on the comparison between these tw o studies. A sec-

ondary emphasis

 is on

 com parisons between classes w ithin each study.

T en

  teachers participated

  in the

  main study,

  five  in the

  learning s tudy

group

 a nd

 five

  in the

 lesson s tudy group.

 T he

 teachers

 in the two

 groups

 had

8

 years

 and 7.2 average

 years

 of teaching

 experience,

 respectively. Fu rther-

more ,

  in

  Hong Kong, secondary school s tudents

  are

  divided into

  five

"bands,"

 where Band

 1

 represents

 the

 high est level

 of

 academic a t ta inmen t

and  B a n d  5 the  lowest . Four  of the five classes  in the  lesson s tudy group

comprised Band

 1 and 2

 s tudents ,

 and one

 c lass

 was of

 Band

 3

 students.

 The

learning s tudy group included one  B a n d  1 , two  B a n d  2 and two  B a n d  3

classes. Students  in the  lesson s tudy group were thus classif ied  as hav ing

somewhat higher expected attainment.

 There

 were altogether

 356

  students

in the age

  range

  16 to 1 8

 years

 and

  they

 all

  s tudied econom ics

 a s a

 school

subject .

A ll  the

  lessons were videotaped

  and

  subsequ ent ly ana lyzed

 in

 term s

  o f

the enacted objects of learning. A f te r  the series of lessons, all 356 students

w ere required to com plete a wri t ten task, and f ive s tudents from each c lass

were chosen random ly

 for

 interview s.

 The

 w ri tten tasks

 and the

 interview s

were

 to

 evaluate student understanding

 of the

  topic concerned.

Prior  to the  main s tudy, a pi lot s tudy w as  carried  ou t  w i th tw o  groups,

each co nsisting   of two teachers  and their stude nts, in order to try ou t the re-

search design   and  test  the  ins t ruments set for  eva lua t ing student lea rn ing.

M ore im portan tly, this pilot study attemp ted to identify  the qual i ta t ively dif-

ferent

 w a y s

 in

 w hich s tudents m ade sense

 o f the

 distribution

 of tax

 burden,

so as to  provide inp ut for the m ain s tudy. A t the en d of the p i lot s tudy, th e

participating students ' understanding

 of the

  topic

 was

  probed

  by

  asking

them   to answer  the  fo l lowing  quest ion in w ri ting:

In

  a

 r ecen t

 Legis lat ive C o u n c i l

  m e e t i n g ,

 the

 proposal

  to

 b r i n g

 in a

 sales

  tax to

 H o n g

Kong was rejected. A new spaper then conduc t ed i n te rv i ews w i t h m e m b e rs of the p u b -

li c t o

 canvas s op in ion s

 on the

 t ax . M os t people opposed

  it.

 The y sa id

 tha t

 pr ices

 w o u ld

increase  by the  a m o u n t of the  t ax . Do you  agree

  that

  p r ices w ou ld increase by the

a m o u n t

  of the

 t ax?

 W h y o r w h y

 n o t ?

Answers from 158

 students participating

 in the

 pilot study

 were

 analyzed

in

 a

 phenom enographic

  way in

 order

  to

 reveal their conc ept ions regarding

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 228/255

8.  T O W A R D

  A

  P E D A G O G Y

  O F  L E A R N I N G  21 5

the incidence

 of a

 sales tax .

 In

 addition

 to

 answering

  th e

 written qu estion,

 a

subsample   of 16 students w as  interview ed about their understand ing o f the

phenomenon. The analyses of the data   from  this interview facil i tated the

analysis

 of the

 written data.

Th e pilot study revealed six qualitatively different w ays of conceptualizing

th e  incidence

 of a

 sales

 tax

 (Co nceptions

 A

 through

 F on

 Table 8.6),

 a nd

 these

were used

 as the

 basis

  for the

 main study.

 A ll six

 conceptions w ere

  identifi-

able in both the written tasks and student interviews, and

 represent

 the varia-

t ion in the w ays in w hich the students experienced the phen om enon .

The set of categories of description described revea l the  different  under-

s tandings am ong s tudents of the incidence of a sales tax, m aking up the di f-

ferent

  conceptions or

  "ways

  o f

 seeing."

  Corresponding to the

  different

referential aspe cts, w hich represent

  different

 m ean ings

 o f the

 incidence

 of a

sales tax, there are   different  structural aspects, representing what the stu-

den ts discerned and  focused  on. B y com paring the concept ions , the cr i t ical

features  of the concept ions w ere iden tified, nam ely the features that dis tin-

guished them   from  one another.

During the preparatory meetings for the main study, teachers in both

groups drew

  on the findings of the

  pilot study (i .e.,

  the

  students ' quali ta-

tively

 different  ways of understanding tax incidence), and agreed that the

cr i tical aspect of an econom ic w ay o f unders tanding w as the relat ive elas t ic-

ity

  of

  demand

  and

  supply. They then started

  to

  plan

  a

  series

  of

  lessons

around this

 concept.

 As

 regards

 the intended object of  learning, two  com-

monal i t ies

 a nd

 three critical difference s between

  the two

 groups were iden-

t if ied.

  In

  terms

  of

  similari t ies, both groups tried

  to

  establish

  a

 contex t

 for

learning this topic

  by

  employing current news about

  the

  int roduct ion

 o f a

general sales tax in Hong Kong, as wel l as an exam ple of an authen t ic gaso-

l ine bil l . This b uilding

 up of

 context

 is in

 accordance w i th

 the

 principle

 of

building up a relevance structure for students, as espoused by Marton and

Booth (1997).

Furthermore,

  i t was  found

  that

  all

  teachers

  had

  made systemat ic

 use of

variat ion; in a sense the intuit ive use of variat ion w as a key  feature of all the

teaching in the study. Both groups introduced variat ion  sequen t ia lly  in the

dimension

  of

 elasticity

 of

 demand

 and

 supply.

 T he

 teachers

 first

 varied

 the

elasticity

 of dem and against the invariant elast ici ty of supply, and then var-

ied the elast ici ty of supply a gainst the invariant elast ici ty of dem and , in or-

der to

 di rect s tude nts '

 focal

 awareness toward

 the

 effect

  o f

 such e last ici ty

 on

th e

 distribution of the tax  burden.

However , there were three major  differences  between the two groups:

First , al l the teachers in the theory-inspired group sought to f ind out the

w a y s in

 w h i ch

 the

 students understood

 the

 phenomenon

  of the

 incidence

 o f

a sales tax, and use these as the basis for developing their lessons. In con -

trast, only

  one

  teacher

  in the

  comparison group at tempted

  to do the

 s ame .

Second,

 a ll the

 teachers

 in the

 theory-inspired group wa nted

 to

 in t roduce s i-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 229/255

TABLE

  8.6

Categories

 of

 Description

 for the Incidence of a Sales Tax

Leve l  o f

Unders tanding   Con ception Referen tial Aspect  

Structural  Aspect

O ne

O ne

O ne

Two

D

Tw o

Three

Tax is

  ful ly

  borne by

bu y e r s ,

 and the tax

burden

  is

 related

  to the

demand side

 o f the

marke t .

Tax is  fu l ly

  borne

  by

buyers,

  and the tax

burden

  is

 related

  to the

supply s ide  of the

marke t .

Tax is

  fu l ly

  borne by

sellers, and the tax

burden

  is

 related

  to

dimens ions o ther than

d em an d

  and

 supp ly .

Tax is

  shared between

bu y e r s

 and

  sellers,

  and

the tax burde n is re lated

Focused

 on the demand

condi t ions of the market in

w h i c h

  th e

 goods

 a re

s i tuated. Variat ion

  is

brough t abou t

  by one or

m o re  of  these factors , such

as the

 n a tu re

 of the  goods.

Focused

  on the

 s u p p ly

conditions

  of the

 m arket

 in

w h i c h

  the

 goods

 a re

s i tua ted . V ar ia t ion

 is

bro u g h t

  abou t

  by one or

more

  of

  these factors , such

as the  m arke t power  of the

sel lers .

Focused on the nonma rke t

mechan isms

  of the

 market

in

  w h i c h

 t he

 goods

 are

s i tuated.

  Var ia t ion

 is

b r ough t

  abo u t

  by aspects

o ther than marke t op era t ion ,

for  ex am p le , g o v e rn m en t

i n t e rven t ion ,  and so on.

Focused

  on the

  e las t i c i ty

 of

demand

  of the

 goods.

Varia t ion

  i s b rou gh t abou t

to the dem and s ide of the by chan ges in dem and

marke t .

Tax   is

 shared

  between

buyers

  and

  sel lers ,

 and

the tax

  bu rd en

  is

  related

to the

  supply s ide

  of the

m ark e t .

Tax is shared between

buyers

  and

 sellers,

  and

the tax burde n is re lated

to

  s imultaneous

interact ion

  be tween the

demand  and  supply s ides

of the m ark e t .

condi t ions .

Focused

 on the

  elasticity

  of

supp ly

 of the goods.

Variat ion

  is

  brought about

by

  changes

  in

 s u p p ly

condi t ions .

Focused

  on the

 re la t ive

elasticity

  of

 demand

  and

supp l y

 of the

 goods.

Variat ion

  is

  brough t ab ou t

by

  r e l a t i v e ch an g es

  in

  both

aspects.

2 1 6

AH

H

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 230/255

8.

  T O W A R D

  A

  P E D A G O G Y

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

  21 7

mul taneous

  variation in elasticity of demand and supply, a l though none of

th e teachers in the  comparison group planned to do so. T hird, all the teach-

ers in the theory-inspired group

 endeavored

 to introduce  the variations in a

contextual ized

 m anner, and by  using the same produ ct ( red w ine) in every

case. In contrast, none of the teachers in the co m parison group tried to pro-

vide a context  for students wh en dealing w ith variation  in the  elasticity of

demand and

 supply,

 nor did

 they

 use the

 sam e goods

 as a

 context

 for

 learn-

ing th roughou t th e  lessons.

Res ul t s  of the  S t u d y

The key

  findings

  of this study can be categorized into three areas: nam ely,

the

  enac ted objects

 o f

  learning, student learning,

  and the

  relat ionship

 be-

tween  the enacted objects o f  learning  and  student learning.

The   En ac t e d  Objects

 of

  Learning

A ll  the  teachers, in both  the  learning study group  and  lesson study group,

focused

  on the

  object

  of

  learning that

 had

  been agreed

  on ,

  that

  is, to

 help

students

  to

 develop

 the

 capability

 of

 taking into  consideration

  the

  relative

elast ici ty

  of demand  and  supply when looking  at the  incidence of a sales

tax .

 Th e teachers did not begin by thinking abo ut c lassroom organizat ion;

or  the teaching arrangements to be used; whether there would be group

work or

 ind ividual study;

 or

 w hether they w ould

 use

  in format ion technol-

ogy

 o r

 no t. Instea d, they paid great attention

 to the w ay in

 w hich they should

deal  wi th  the  objec t o f learning in the  lessons.

The two

  commonali t ies between

  the two

 groups that were  identified

  in

the intended object o f learning (i.e., the bu i lding up of a relevance structure

for

  students and the  introduction  of variation  in  elasticity  of demand  and

supply

  in a

 sequ ential m anner) w ere also  found

  in the

 a ctual enactment

 of

the intended object o f  learning (i.e.,  the enacted object o f  learning) . How -

ever,

 of the three critica l

 difference s

 be tween the tw o groups tha t w ere iden-

t ified  in the intended object of learning, only the

  second

  and third critical

differences  were

 found

 in the enacted object of learning. The teachers in the

theory-inspired   group introduced simultaneous variation  and  contextu-

alized  variation whereas those  in the  comparison group  did not . The first

critical

 difference,

 w h ich is the solicitation of studen ts' existing understand-

ing   of phenomenon,  was not  found  because  the  students  in the  theory-in-

spired grou p were inhibited by a num ber of things, including the presence o f

a

 cam era, an observer, and the use of Eng lish as the m ediu m of instruction.

In the  intragroup comparison regarding  the  building  up of a relevance

structure for

 students

 ( as

 shown

  in

 Table 8.7), three teac hers

  in the

 learning

study

 group (Teachers 1 ,2,

 and 3) did not

 follow

 the

 agreed lesson plan,

 an d

did not  employ  the example  of an authentic gasoline bill to  illustrate how

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 231/255

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 232/255

8.

  T O W A R D

  A  P E D A G O G Y  O F  L E A R N I N G  21 9

sales tax appears in daily transactions. Although al l of the teachers at-

tem pted to reveal the studen ts '

  different

  w ays of unders tanding the dis t ribu-

tion  of the tax burden between buyers  and  sellers (using  th e  exam ple  of a

sales tax imposed on red wine), only three of them (Teachers 1, 3, and 4)

con duc ted a m inisurvey inviting students to express their view s. This part of

the lesson plan wa s not very

 successful

  in any of the classes bec au se o f the

reluctance of students to speak up generally in class.

However, al l teachers in the learning study group introduced simulta-

neo us variat ion

 in

 elasticity

 o f

 demand

 and

 supp ly (al thou gh Teacher

 3 did

it m ore  successfu l ly  and wi th more s tudent con tr ibut ion than other teach-

ers). W ith regard

  to the

  contextualizat ion

  and

  consis tency

  of

 contexts ,

 all

teachers in th is group organized their lessons us ing red w ine as the ex am ple

product . Three of the teach ers (T eachers 1 ,2, and 3) kep t this pro du ct invari-

ant throug hou t al l the lessons in order to m ake the s im ul taneou s variation in

elasticity

  of dem and and supply eas ier to discern, w hereas Teacher 4 and

Teacher 5 did not u se it in the  last lesson .

Table 8.8 show s that all  teachers in the  lesson s tudy group succe eded in

carrying out

 w h a t

 w as

 stated

 in the

 lesson plan ,

 and

 tha t som e

 o f

 them in tro-

duced a ddit iona l tasks to enha nce the o riginal lesson plan . In order to bu ild

up a relevance s t ructure for the students to learn the top ic , Teacher 8  further

added

  h i s own

  personal experiences

  of

  sales

  tax  (from  the

  t im e when

  he

lived in Can ada som e years before) , and brough t in a "giveaway" i tem from

a gasol ine s tat ion (a box of t i ssues) , which he showed together wi th the

w orkshee t on the gasoline bil l . Teacher 7 designed an ex tra w orkshe et in or-

der to he lp s tude nts to acqu ire the ski ll s to t ransform nu m erical data in to

graph   format ,  and  find  out the tax  burden be tween buyers  and  sel lers .

Final ly ,

 w hen int roducing variat ion

  in

  elasticity

 of

 de ma n d

 and

  supply

 s e-

quent ial ly , Teacher

 9

 further

  included extrem e cases that were

 no t

  covered

in

 the

 lesson plan, such

 a s

 perfect ly elas tic dem and,

 perfect ly

  inelas t ic

 de -

m a n d ,

 perfect ly

  elas t ic supply

 and

 pe rfect ly inelas t ic supply.

S t u d en t  Learning

  (The

 Lived

  Object

  of  Learning)

In

 phenomenography ,

  learn ing

  is defined  as a cha nge in the dynam ic s tate

of  awareness

  or the way of

  experiencing (Pong

  &

  Marton, 2001) . Hen ce,

the  present study describes student learning

  in

  accordance wi th s tudent

ways of experiencing the phenomenon or concept ions

  identif ied,

  rather

than  merely count ing the percentage of s tudents who gave the correct

w ords or num bers as answers to quest ions set in a diagnost ic tes t . Studen ts

are said to have shown evidence of learning if they can  successfu l ly display

advanced and pow erful concept ions of the phen om enon in quest ion.

A total of 356 students part icipated in the m ain study, in w hich Scho ol A

to School

 E

 belonged

 to the

 learning study group,

 and

 School

 P to

 School

 T

belonge d to the lesson s tudy group. A l l s tudents were required to com plete a

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 233/255

[3  TABLE  8.8

®

  Comparison With in

 the Lesson

 Study

  Gro u p

 —

 The

  Enacted Objects

  o f

  Learning

Teacher 6

B ui l t

  u p

  re levance st ructure

  Y es

Revealed varia t ion in the s tudents ' No

ways o f  u n de rs t an d i n g

Int roduced varia t ion in the Yes

d i men s i o n s

  o f

 e l a s t i c i t y

 o f

  deman d

and

  su p p l y

  in a

  sequent ia l manner

In t roduced varia t ion

  in the

  c r i t ica l

  N o

aspect  of  re la t ive e last ic i ty o f

demand  and  su p p l y  in a

s i mu l t an eo u s man n e r

Co n t ex t u a l i / a t i o n  a nd  consistency  N o

of contexts

Teacher

  7

Y es

N o

Y es

(Add-on:

  an

  ex t ra

worksheet

  o n

  g rap h i ca l

representa t ion

  of tax

i n c i den ce )

N o

N o

Teacher 8 Teacher 9

Yes Yes

(Add-on :

  personal

stories about  sa les t ax

an d  g i veaw ay s f ro m

g aso l i n e s t a t i o n )

N o N o

Yes Yes

(Add-on: discussion

on

  ex t reme  cases)

N o N o

N o

  N o

Teacher

  10

Y es

Part ia l ly effected

(Di d

  no t

  co n du c t

  a

class

 su rvey)

Y es

(A dd- o n :

  an

  ext ra

workshee t on

g rap h i ca l

representa t ion   of tax

i n c i den ce )

N o

N o

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 234/255

8.  TOWARD  A  PEDAGOGY  OF  LEARNING  221

written task at the end of the series of lessons, and five students  from  each

class were chosen randomly

  to

 attend

  an

 interview

  on the

  same

  day as the

last

 lesson.

 T he

 data that were co llected

  from

  these

  tw o

 sources were an a-

lyzed, and the way s of experiencing that w ere iden tif ied w ere then catego-

rized

  in

  accordance wi th

  the

  outcome space (which

  w as

  described

  in the

previous sect ion) .

  T he

  learning outcomes

  ( in

 term s

  of the

 concept ions

  de-

veloped by s tudents) are reported on a group basis , wi th a comparison m ade

between

  the

 learning study group

 an d the

 lesson study group,

 as

 we l l

 as be-

tween classes w i thin each group.

T he

  major qual i t a t ive difference

  in the

  unders tanding

  of the tax

  inci-

dence

 lay in the

 s tuden ts ' unders tanding

 of the

 s im ul taneous interaction

 be-

tween the demand and supply s ide of the market ( i .e . , the s imul taneous

r e la t ionsh ip

 betw een the elas t ic i ty of dem and and supply) . Abou t 73% of

th e  learning study

 group managed  to show this

 u nderstanding,

  and

  mani -

fested  Concept ion

  F ,

 w hich took into accou nt

  the

 relat ive elast ici ty

 of de-

m and and sup ply in determining the dis t ribution of the tax burden betw een

buyers and sel lers. In the lesson study g roup, ju st un der 30% of the studen ts

reac hed th is level of und erstan ding. The deta i ls are shown in Table 8.9.

A s

  regards intragroup variat ion in the learning study group, there were

marked

 d i f ferences  in the

 s tudents ' learning ou tcom e am ong

 the

 classes.

 A s

w e

 can see in

 Table 8.10, School

 C had the

 bes t per formance w i th

 the

 w ri t-

ten

  task, with 80.0%

  of the

  students expressing Conception

  F in

  thei r

 an-

swers . S chool A ( 77.5%) fol low ed, and then School B (72.5%), School D

(66 .7%) ,  and

  School

  E

  (60.7%).

T A B L E

  8.9

Distribution of Conceptions for the Written Task

The Learning

  Study  Group

  The Lesson

  S tudy G roup

(181

  students)

  (175

  students)

Group

Conception

A

B

C

D

E

F

Occurrence

9

4

8

25

4

131

Percentage

5.0%

2.2%

4.4%

13.8%

2.2%

72.4%

Occurrence

19

15

17

66

7

51

Percentage

10.9%

8.6%

9.7%

37.7%

4.0%

29.1%

Note.  Chi - square

 =

 67.553

 (df=

  5;p<  0.001) .

 N o

 cel ls  (0.0%)

 have expected

  coun ts

 of

  less than

5.

 T he

  m i n i m u m  expected

  count is

 5 . 41 .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 235/255

2 2 2 L O E T A L .

TABLE   8 . 1 0

Distribution of Conceptions W i t h in  th e

 Learning

  S t u d y G r ou p —

Th e

  Written Task

School

  A

Conception (40)

A 1

(2 .5%)

B 1

(2.5%)

C 1

(2.5%)

D 5

(12.5%)

E 1

(2.5%)

F 31

(77.5%)

B

(40)

2

(5.0%)

1

(2.5%)

1

(2.5%)

6

(15.0%)

1

(2.5%)

29

(72.5%)

C

(40)

2

(5.0%)

0

(0.0%)

2

(5 .0%)

4

(10.0%)

0

(0.0%)

32

(80.0%)

D

(33)

2

( 6 . 1 % )

1

(3.0%)

1

(3.0%)

6

(18 .2%)

1

(3.0%)

22

(66.7%)

E

(28)

2

( 7 . 1 % )

1

(3 .6%)

3

( 10 .7% )

4

(14.3%)

1

(3.6%)

17

(60.7%)

Total

N u m be r  o f

Conceptions

9

(5 .0%)

4

(2 .2%)

8

(4 .4%)

25

(13 .8% )

4

(2 .2%)

1 31

(72.4%)

In the

 lesson study group,

 as

 shown

 in

 Table 8 .11 ,

 the

 percen tage

 o f

 Con-

cept ion

 F was

 s imi lar am ong

 the

 classes, w ith School

 T

 recording

 the

 h i g h -

est

 score  (38.5%), followed

 by

 School

  S

 (35.3%), School

 R

 (33.3%), School

Q

  (20.8%),

  and

 School

  P

 (20.0%).

T h e   Rel a t i o ns h i p  Between  th e  E na cted

  Objects

of  L e a r n i n g  an d  S t u d e n t L e a r n i n g

T he d ifferences  in the enacted object of learning (as show n by the  different

patterns

  of

  variat ion made avai lable

 in the

  classrooms), were

  ref lected  in

student learning  in term s of the ways of experiencing that were m anifes ted

by students

  after

  the lessons . The aspects of the phe nom eno n

  that

 s tudents

discerned m irrored the pattern of  var ia t ions, m ore

 specifical ly,

 the obje ct of

learning experienced

  by the

 students

  in the

 class.

 A s

 seen

  in the

 data

 o n

 stu-

dent learning, the s tudents in the learning s tud y group (w ho were presen ted

with this par t icular pat tern of var iat ion) , learned more   effectively  than

those

  in the

  lesson study group.

Furtherm ore, w ithin the learn ing study group, the d ifferences  in s tudents '

performance also seemed

  to

 coincide wi th

 the  differences  in the

 pa t tern

 of

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 236/255

8.   T O W A R D

  A

  P E D A G O G Y

  O F

  L E A R N I N G

223

T A B L E

  8 . 1 1

Distr ibut ion

 of Conceptions W ith in the

 Lesson

 S t u d y

  G r o u p —

The

 Written Task

School

  P

Concept ion (25)

A

  4

(16.0%)

B   3

(12 .0%)

C 1

(4.0%)

D 12

(48.0%)

E  0

(0.0%)

F   5

(20.0%)

Q

(53)

5

(9.4%)

3

(5 .7%)

3

(5.7%)

29

(54.7%)

2

(3.8%)

1 1

(20.8%)

R

(24)

4

(14 .3%)

3

( 12 .5% )

5

(20.8%)

3

(12.5%)

1

(4.2%)

8

(33.3%)

S

(34)

3

(8.8%)

2

(5.9%)

6

(17.6%)

8

(23.5%)

3

(8.8%)

12

(35.3%)

T

(39)

3

(7.7%)

4

(10.3%)

2

(5 .1%)

14

(35.9%)

1

(2.6%)

15

(38.5%)

Total

N u m b e r

  o f

Concept ions

19

(10 .6%)

15

(8 .6%)

17

(9.7%)

66

(37.7%)

7

(4.0%)

51

(29 .1%)

variation m ade available in each classroom . For instance, Teacher 3 allow ed

more

  flexibility

  for  students  to  introduce simultaneous variation  in  both

elasticity

 of

 demand

 a nd

 supply,

 and at the

 same time used

 the

 context

 o f the

red

 wine throughout

 all the

 lessons.

 In

 con trast,

 Teacher

 5 did not

 allow

  as

m u c h

  flexibility

  for  students  to  introduce simultaneous variation,  and did

not keep the product invariant for the last session. It w as co nseq uen tly found

that the students tau gh t by T eacher 3 achieved a better u nderstanding of the

topic than the s tudents taugh t by T eacher 5, a

 finding

 that m ight be attribut-

able

 to the

 pattern

  of

  variation made available

  in the

  lessons.

Reflec t ions

 on the Study

From an analy sis of the teach ing in terms of the dim ensions of variation that

were opened  up in the  lessons—that  is,  those aspects that were varied  si-

mul taneous ly   and  those aspects that remained invariant—the pattern  of

variation critical

  to the

 developm ent

  of an economic

 un derstanding

  of the

incidence o f a sales tax became apparent. T he teachers in the learning study

group handled the object of learning in a w ay that was pow erful in enhanc-

ing studen t learning. They did this by

 focusing

 o n the object of learning and

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 237/255

224 LO ET AL.

opening  up  s imultaneous var ia t ion  in its critical aspects—that is, they

opened  up the

 relative

 elasticity  of demand  and supply  in a contextual ized

manner ,  and deliberately kept  the  product (red wine) invariant so that stu-

dents could

 experience

 a change in the way that they were aw are of the phe -

nomenon

  in

  quest ion. This  approach wi l l

  be  he lp fu l  to

  teachers dealing

with

  the

  same object

  of

  learning.

In this study,  the theory  of variation  w as examined  in relation to its

  effi-

cacy

 when used

 to

 design

  an effective

  learning environm ent . From

 the

 data

on the

  teaching methods

  and the

  students' learning,

  the

  learning study

(which bu i lt

 on the

 theory

 of

 variat ion)

 w as

 found

  to be

 very

 effective  in en-

hancing learning

 in the

 econom ics classroom ,

  in

 term s

 o f the

 possibil i ty

 fo r

teachers

  to

 identify

  critical aspects related

  to

 different

  w a y s

 o f

 unders tand-

ing and to design the learning si tuation arou nd these cri t ical aspects. T his

m ethod w as foun d to considerably enhance the s tudents ' unde rs tandin g of

the phen om enon in que stion, that is , the inciden ce of a sales tax. Th e study

thus supports the tenet of the theory o f variat ion, tha t lea rnin g to see som e-

thing

  in a

 certain

 w ay is a funct ion  o f

 experiencing s im ul tane ou s variation

in   critical aspects

 of the

 object

  of

 l earn ing .

Finally, the  f indings  of the  study seem  to  suggest that the col laborat ion

between teachers

 in a

 learning s tudy prem ised

 on a specific

  theory

 o f

 learn-

ing

 ( in this case, the learning study group prem ised on the theory of varia-

tion),  is more  effective  than teachers w ork ing together w i thou t an expl ic i t

theoretical grounding

  (as in the

 lesson study group).

  A s w e

 m ent ioned ear-

lier,

  more than twice

  as

  many s tudents

  f rom the

  learning s tudy group

reached a good u nders tanding of the topic of the incidence  of a sales tax as

students in classes of the lesson study group. Th is

  further

  supports the no-

tion that

  the

  learning study

  w as more

 effective  than

  th e

  lesson study

  in al-

lowing teachers to im prove their teach ing , and thereby h elp students to leam

more

 efficiently.

C O N C L U D I N G   REMARKS:

L EARNI NG FROM L EARNI NG S T UDI ES

In

 th is chapter, w e presented three ex am ples of s tudies aimed at im provin g

learning from  the po in t o f view of the theore t i ca l background brief ly  intro-

duced

 in

 chapter

  1, and the

 descript ive em pirical studies presen ted

  in

 chap-

ters

  2

  through chapter

  7 . Our

  basic idea

  fo r

  fac i l i ta t ing

  more

  effective

learning  is thus as fol low s: Take as the po in t o f depar ture w hat it is tha t the

students

  are

 expected

  to

 learn ( the object

 o f

 learning);

 f ind out

 w h a t m a k e s

the difference betw een the stude nts w ho have a good grasp, and those w ho

do not; construct  a space of l earn ing tha t makes i t po ssible  for all s tudents to

have a good grasp of wh at is to be learne d; and finally, an aly ze the relat io n-

ship

 between the

 space

 of

 learning

 a nd

 wha t happened

  in the

 classroom (the

enacted object  of l earn ing) , and the w ay in w hic h th i s re la t ionsh ip affected

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 238/255

8.  T O W A R D  A  P E D A G O G Y  O F  L E A R N I N G  225

the learning outcom es ( i .e . , w hat th e s tudents have achieved). In our view, it

is th is procedure— w hich  w as fol low ed by all the teachers  in the three learn-

ing  studies reported

  in

 th i s chap ter—that

 led to the

 success fu l  learning out-

comes

 as

 evidenced

  by the

  study findings.

A ll

  the

  studies

 in

 this chapter

 had

  certain  features

  in

 c o mmo n :

  the

  same

theoretical grounding   and systematic  evaluation,  a  focus  on the  object  of

learning,

 and

  condit ions that enabled teachers

  to

 work collaboratively.

T he

 first

  study  in this

 chapter

 was a clear dem onstration  of how  teachers

m ade u se of the theory of variat ion as a ped ago gical tool . However, we saw

that  even

  if the

 teachers

  had

  previously agreed

  on

 wha t

  to do in the

  class-

room , when they carr ied

 o ut the

 actu al lesson, ea ch lesson

 differed  in a

 n u m -

ber of important respects (as i s wel l demonstrated by the second s tudy

reported

  in

 this chapter).

 T o the

 extent that such

 d i f ferences  are

 recogn ized,

they can be   seen  to  accoun t  for di f ferences  in the  learning outcomes ( the

l ived ob jec t

 o f

 learning) .

 In the

  second study,

 the

 better perform ance

 o f the

studen ts in Clas s 4C over those in Class 4H in the po sttest can be at tributed

to the difference s  in the enacted objects of learning in the respective lessons.

Learning to  focus on the c ri t ical aspects o f the object o f learning through the

use

 of an

 appropriate pattern

  of

 variation

  is not

 easy,

 and

 does

 n ot

 come nat-

ural ly  even

 to

 experienced teachers .

 T he

 th i rd s tudy quo ted

 in

 th is chapter

showed that teaching  and learning w as m u c h more

 effective

 when the teach-

ers

 were gu ided

 by the

 theory

  of

 variat ion.

B y creating   the co nditions  fo r teachers to work together a s a team using

an  act ion research approach, we enable teachers to learn and develop to-

gether. In  fact,  the bene f i ts  go

 beyond this .

 T he

 three learning studies show

that the process of designing, co nduct ing, and evaluat ing research lessons

benef i t ted  a ll  three parties:  the  students,  the  teachers,  and the  researchers .

The students gained a better understanding of the object of learning, the

teachers learned

  how to

  handle

  the

  object

  of

  learning

  in a

 more

  effective

way, and the  researchers gamed further insight into the ways  in wh ich the-

ory  can be

  translated into practice.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 239/255

  his page intentionally left blank

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 240/255

Epilogue

There  is a  widely held bel ief today that knowledge  is  becoming increas-

ingly  impor tan t

 in the

 l ives

 of

 individuals

 and in the

 lives

 o f

 nat ions .

 I f

 this

is t rue, wh at kind of conclusion can be drawn

  from

  the studies reported in

this

 book

 w i th regard to faci l i tat ing the developm ent  of know ledge  on indi-

vidual  and c ollec t ive levels?

The quest ion of prom oting the growth of know ledge can, of course, be

addressed

 from

 p ol i tical , econom ic,

 a nd

 social points

 o f

 view .

 Our

 perspec-

tive,

 however,  is pedagogical;  w e are concerned w ith teaching  and  learning.

Teaching is a human act ivi ty involving someone teaching something to

someone else . There

  is

  someone teaching

  (a

  teacher) , someone  t augh t

  (a

learner or learners) and something taught ( the content) . In grammatical

terms,  th e learner  is the dative (indirect o bject), an d the content  is the accu-

sative (direct object).

 T he failure to

 focus

 o n

 both

 of these tw o

 objects

 at the

same t im e has hau nted educat ion for centuries . Foc us has , in the ma in, been

on on e or the other, but rarely on both at the sam e t im e. In one instan ce, the

emphas i s

 has

 been

  on the

  teacher

 an d the

  learner;

  in

 another ins tance,

 the

emphasis

 has

 been

 on the

 teacher

 and the

  content

Learner-focused e ducat ion, w here the

  focus

 is on the learner, and on the

w a y s

 in

 wh ich teach ing

 is adjusted to the

 learner 's nee ds,

 h as

 very m uc h co l -

ored  the  20th century movement cal led "progressivism." T he  m ajor ity of

university educationalists  in the W est probably subscribe  to this orientation.

Where the

  focus

  is on the content , that i s , on the knowledge to be

learned, we ca l l th i s the " t radi tional i st " approach to educa t ion . A cco rding

to this approach,  the  mos t im por tan t th ing  is  that  the  t eacher  has a  good

mastery of the

  content;

 he or she has to be

 good

 at

 w h a t

 her

  students

 are

supposed

  to

  become good

  at . For the

  t radi t ional is t approach,

  it is of

 pri-

mary impor tance tha t the s tudents a re exposed  to cer tain k inds o f con t en t ;

227

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 241/255

228  E P I L O G U E

w hat i s covered in t each ing i s v i t a l . Th e "back to bas ics" m ove m en ts be-

long to this school of  thought .

In the   politics  of  education, these  tw o  orientat ions—progressivist  and

t radit ionalist— have replaced each other

  in an

 ever-recum ng cycle

 o f

 peda-

gogical fashio n. This w e bel ieve is a m a jo r obs tac l e to improv ing pedagog i -

cal pract ices .  In  fact,  pedagog ica l p rac t i ces w i ll  o n l y i mp r o ve w h e n equa l

considerat ion

  is

 given

 to

 both

  the

 learner

 a nd the

 c o n t e n t .

 T he

 reason

 w hy

both or ientations m ust be considered together i s very s im ple: There can be

no   learning without  a learner,  nor can there be any l earn ing w i thout some-

thing learned. W e therefore ha ve to foc us both on the learner and o n the co n-

tent

 o f

  learning

 at the

  s ame tim e .

Let us

  look

 a t the

  qu e s t io n "W h a t

  is to be

  l earned?"

  In the

 p a s t ,

 sy l l a -

buses w ere fo rm ula t ed

 in

  te rm s

  o f

 ca t ego r i e s

 of

 c o n t e n t .

 T he

  qu e s t i o n

 o f

W h a t  is to be  learned?  w as answered  in t e rm s o f d i f ferent  par ts o f ma t h e -

matics, history, English, philosophy,  and so on  (e .g . . f ract ions , second-

grade  equat ions ,

  th e

  French Revolu t ion , Wor ld

  W ar I I,

  i r regular verbs .

Ch uang Tzu , e t c . ).

 B u t

 w h a t

 is to be

 l earned

 i s

 never s im p ly

 a

 case

 o f

  f rac-

t ions

 o r

 C h u a n g T zu ,

 o r

 a n y t h i n g

 of the

 l ik e . W h a t

 is to be

 l ea rned

 is

 c a p a -

b i li ti es , tha t i s , w ha t the s tude nts a re expec ted to becom e cap ab le o f do in g

( in the   widest sense).  F or  example , be ing ab le  to  express quant i t i es in

terms o f rat ional num bers ,  an d carry  o u t ar i thme t ic o pera t ions w i th ra t io-

na l

 numbers;

 o r

 having

 an

 unde rs tanding

  o f

 Chuan g Tzu 's v i ew

 of

 h u m a n

exis tence ,

 an d

 be ing ab le

  to see

 c o n t em p o r ar y p h e n o m e n a

 in l i g h t  of his

concep t ion o f Tao i sm.

Such ca pab i l i t ies  are w h a t w e  re fer red  to in th i s book  as the o bjec ts  o f

learning.  The concept of an

  objec t  o f  learn ing

  inc lud es bo th the learner

and the  con tent;  it is def ined in terms of the con tent i t sel f ( referred to as the

direct object  o f  learn ing  in chap . 1) and in term s of the learn er ' s way of

handl ing

 the

 c on ten t ( re fer red

 to as the

 ind irec t objec t

  of

  learn ing

  in

 c h a p .

1 ).

 Thus ,  w e  canno t ta l k abou t th e ob j ec t o f  l e a r n in g w i t h o u t r e f e r ri n g to

th e  learner an d the con t en t a t the s ame t i me . O u r p o i n t i s

 tha t

  th e p r i ma r y

focus  o f educat ion should be on bo th the l earner and the co nten t , and no t

on one o f them alone. Fai lure to focus o n bo th w i l l resu l t  in lopsided per-

spect ives .

  N o t

  p a y i n g a t t e n t i o n

  to the

  l earners ,

  to how

  t h e y ma k e

sense—or should make sense—

 of the

 c o n t e n t , r e s u l t s

 in an

 e m p h a s is

 on

j u s t w ha t i s covered— or shou ld be covered— by t each e r s "who know the i r

sub jec t ," and no t how the con ten t is handled, o r shou ld be ha nd led.

Currently, however, content tends

  to be

 u nderp layed

  in

 W estern educa-

t ional thinking, result ing in the resurgence of tw o i l lus ion s, albeit in

  differ-

ent  forms .  O ne  i l lus ion is the old  dream of  f inding  "the  art of  t each ing all

th ings  to all  men." This phrase  first  appeared  in the

  subti t le

  of C o m e n iu s '

book   The

 G reat Didact ics  in

  1657, w hich

 i s

 seen

 as the

  first  sys t em at ic

 at-

tempt to develop a science of teach ing. B ut the dream that w e can teac h any -

thing to anyone, i f only w e can  find  the r ight m ethod, i s probably older tha n

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 242/255

E P I L O G U E  229

that .

 It is

 s t i l l very m uch al ive today, a l tho ugh

 the

 m ethods people com e

 up

wi th

 differ,  of course , f rom t im e to time. Cu rrent ly m any people a re ta lk in g

abou t  cooperative learning, IT-supported forms of learning, project work,

problem-based learning, and so on as the

 pa ths

 to the perfec t art of

 t each ing .

However ,  after  reading this

 book;

  it  should be  quite c lear that there  are

specif ic

  con dit ions necessary  fo r learning spe cif ic objects  o f lea rn ing . T he

condit ions  differ  from  one ob jec t o f learning to another , and we have to  find

out  in each par t icular case w ha t these con dit ions are .

 Peer

 learning , IT sup-

por t , project work,

  and

  problem-based learning

  may be

  a r r angements

 fo r

learning

 that have great po tent ia l , but no general approach to instruct ion can

ever ensure that th e spe cif ic con dit ions necessary fo r the lea rn ing of

 spec i f ic

objects

 o f learning are brou ght about . In order to do this , w e m ust take the

specif ic  ob jec ts

 o f

  learning

 as our

 poin ts

  o f

 depar ture .

The o ther i l lus ion i s about  "gener ic  capabi l i t ie s . " The cur rent l ine of

r ea son ing

  goes

  som e th ing like the  f o l l o w i n g : W e a re  f ac ing  an  e x p o n e n -

t ia l  growth of knowledge . Widespread use of IT and the In te rne t a round

the g lobe , m eans th a t bas ica l ly a l l of th is know ledge i s ava i lab le to every -

one . As we do no t know w ha t k ind o f know ledge our s tuden t s w i l l need

wh en they grow up , the bes t th in g w e can do i s to equip them w i th ca pa bi l i -

t ies

  fo r

 ob ta in ing know ledge ins tead

  o f

 e q u i p p i n g t h e m w i t h k n o w l e dg e .

Thus

  the  e m p h a s i s  is on  gener ic capabi l i t ie s , such  a s  learning  to  l e a rn ,

r ead ing  to lea rn , lea rn ing s t ra tegies , th inking s t ra tegies , coopera t ive

sk i l l s ,  com m un ica t ive sk i l l s , flex ib i l i ty , c rea t iv ity , and so on .  W h a t  is of-

ten

 f o rgo t t en is tha t knowledge is

 fu n dam en ta l ly

  w a y s of seeing the w o r ld .

Gener i c cap ab i l i t ie s

 do

 exist ,

 but not as

 ind iv id ua l t ra i ts ,

 or as a

 type

 of in-

t e l l ec tua l

  musc le tha t deve lops independent ly f rom knowledge

  a nd

  c o n -

t en t .  Gene r ic capab i l i t i e s  a re  w a y s  o f  dea l ing wi th  dif ferent  top ic s ,

con ten t ,

  knowledge ; they do not re fe r to what  peop le have o r w ha t they

are ; they re fe r to w ays in w hich

 peop le

  ac t . Gener ic cap abi l i t ie s a re do-

m a i n

  spec i f i c . T he

  fact

  tha t a

 person p ossesses

 the  capab i l i ty o f, say, ha n-

d l ing  words c rea t ive ly ,  does

  n o t

  necessa r i ly imply tha t

  th e

  pe r son

possesses the capabi l i ty of dea l ing wi th num bers , m usica l or l ing uis t ic

tones , or econom ic t ransac t ions in the same w ay. Gen er ic capa bi l i t ie s de-

velop th roug h hand l ing som e th ing  spec i f ic :

  th e

 m othe r tongue , c a lcu lus ,

q ues t ions

  abou t the ecolo gical survival of our planet , and so on.

There seems to be another widely held bel ief , par t icular ly am ong educ a-

t ional

 pol icymak ers , tha t language

  is the

 object

 o f

 inquiry

  o f

  l inguis ts ,

 and

p ay in g  a t tent ion to lang uag e pedag ogica l ly is the responsibi l ity of l an g u ag e

teachers

 only . Discuss ions about lang uage in the context o f educa t ion have

la rge ly

  f ocused on  i s sues o f  ef fect iveness a nd

  efficiency

  in  a t ta in ing lan-

guage prof ic ien cy. W e hear concerns expressed by the com m unity about the

dec l in ing  langu age standards of

 both

 learners and teachers , and that som e-

th ing

 m ust be done about the language prof ic iency of language teachers and

learners .

 W e

 seldom hear com pla in ts about

 the

 lack

 o f

 a t tent ion paid

 to

 I an-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 243/255

230

  E P I L O G U E

guage   by  subject teachers, such  as  teachers  of  mathemat ics , science,  and

history.

 N or do we

 hear discussions abo ut

 the effects  of the use of

 l anguage

on the

 learning

 of m athemat ical concepts, scientific concepts, and h istorical

concepts,  fo r exam ple. T he relationship between lang ua ge and learning has

been narrowly confi rmed

  to

  language learning.

The exposit ion of the importance of the role of langua ge in bringing abou t

learning

 is not

 som ething new. Several decades ago, especially

 in the

 1970s,

 a

number

  of

 scholars

  had

 already  drawn attention

  to the

  importance

  of

  lan-

guage   in education,  and had made  a strong case  for p aying at tent ion to lan-

guage across

  the

  curriculum (see, e.g., Barnes, 1976; Barnes, Britton,

  &

R osen, 1969; B ritton, 1970). P articu larly  inf luential

 w as the

 work

 of

 M ich ae l

Halliday, wh o

 showed that w hen

 a

 c hi ld leams

 a

 l anguage ,

 he or she is not

 j u s t

engaged

  in one

 kind

 o f

 learning,

 bu t is

 learning

 the

 foundations

 of

 learning

 it -

self: Learning

 is a

 process

 of

 making meaning ,

 an d

 l earn ing language

 is

 learn-

ing how to  m ean (see, e.g., Ha lliday , 1973, 1975, 1978, 1 993). T here fore , it

w as

  argued, every teacher

  is a

  language teacher .

 At the

  t im e, wh en

  the

 lan-

guage awareness movem ent

 w as

 l aunched

 in the

 U ni ted Kingdom , l anguage

in

 education  w as made a compulsory m odule  in teacher education programs

(see Hawkins, 1984).

 T he

 "language across

 the

 cur r icu lum "

 initiative,

 w h i ch

is one o f the

 most important moves

  in

 educat ion ,

 w as

  unfortunately

  not

 sus-

tained (see Hawkins, 1999).

Wha t

  w e

 have tried

 to do in

 this book

  is to

 re vital ize

 the

 discuss ion about

th e

 role

  of  language  in  learning  by  showing  th e  critical role that language

plays in bringing abou t the necessary co ndit ions fo r l earn ing . O ne  necessary

condit ion

 for

 bringing abou t learning

 is

 that students

 are

 able

 to  focus on the

object

 of

 learning

 and

 discern

 i ts

 c ri t ical features.

 W e

 have dem onstrated that

language

 is

 crucial

 in

 structuring studen ts ' aw areness

 so

 tha t they become

 fo-

cally aware

 o f the

 critical fea tures

 of the

 object

 of

 learning,

  in

 bringing about

simu ltaneo us awareness, both sync hron ically

 and diachromcal ly , and in

 ma k -

ing  important conceptual dist inctions. W e have dem onstrated  tha t  because

language m akes meaning,

 the

 ability

 of the

 students

 and the

 t eacher

 to

 neg ot i-

ate

  meaning

  and to

  const ruct meaning col laborat ively through language

makes qualitative d ifferences in the sem antic dime nsion of the space of learn-

ing

 that

 is

 being c onsti tuted. A nother necessary co ndit ion

 for

 b r ing ing about

learning

 i s

 that

 th e

 space

 of

 learning

 is

 shared

 by the

 s tudents

 and the

 teacher.

W e

 have dem onstrated that language plays

 a

  cri t ical role

  in

 es tabl ishing

 as

wel l

 as in

 widening

 a

 shared comm on ground. W hen

  the

 ground

 on

 w h i c h

 a

specific

 object

 o f

 learning

 is

 enacted,

  is not

 shared,

  th e

 qu al i ty

 of

 learning wil l

be

 adversely affected.

 W e

 have shown

 the difference

  l anguage makes

 by

 com-

paring

  the

  classroom discourse

  of

  students learning through their mother

tongue w ith that

 o f

 students learning through

 a

 second

 and

 w eaker language.

When students

 a re

 learning through

 a

 languag e that they

 can

 barely

 use to ex-

press

 themselves, they  are  inevitably handicapped because they do not have

the  necessary resources  for mak ing mean ing . There is a com m only he ld b e-

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 244/255

E P I L O G U E  231

lief, especially am ong edu cational policym akers, that learning throu gh a sec-

ond

  language is no  more  than learning the technical vocabulary in that

language. This simplistic view shows

  a

 lack

 of

 u nderstanding

 of the

 role

 of

language

  in

  learning.

W hat we have demonstrated in th is book is that there are spec if ic  condi-

t ions that

 are

 necessary

  for the

 learning

  of specific

  objects

 of

 learning.

 T he

w ay

  in which these o bjects of learning are handled and the extent to w hich

the necessary condi t ions are brough t about through languag e in the class-

room are of decisive importance for what the students can possibly learn

(and w hat they actua l ly learn).

There  are, no doubt, general condit ions necessary for learning, such as

l ight , space, exposure, feedbac k, and a min im um level o f activity am ong the

learners.

 There

 m ight also be necessary condi t ions of learning that are spe-

cific  to different  groups. Some learners m ay not be able to learn som ething

wi thou t

 sensu ous ex perience , for instance, w hile others are able to learn the

same thing

 by

 sym bol ic m eans ;

 fo r

 some there

 m ay be a

 great difference

  be-

tween  actual ly  seeing som ething and ju s t hear ing som ething descr ibed,

w hereas for others there m ay not be an y  difference  at all . B ut we are not talk-

ing

 ab out necessary condi t ions

 for all

 kinds

 o f

 learning,

 or

 about necessary

condit ions for  specif ic  groups  of  learners,  but  about necessary condi t ions

for  the

 learning

 o f

  specific  objects

 of

  learning . These con dit ion s have

 to be

discovered   for  every  specific  object  of  learning. Teachers should  be en-

gaged in f inding  out w h a t the spec if ic  condit ions a re in every  specif ic  case,

and

 h ow they can be  brought about.

 When these

 con ditions  are discovered,

teaching  w i ll become

  a

 much m ore pow er fu l

 and

 m uch m ore profess ional

enterprise .

In the

  last chapter

 of

 this book,

  w e

  described some teachers engaging

 in

such

 an

 en terprise. They cam e together w i th spec if ic  objects

 o f

 learning

 as

their

 po in t o f departure. They did not start w ith questions about general ar-

rangements

  and

  general teaching methods,

  bu t

  asked questions about

  the

specif ic  objec ts of learning to be dea lt w ith during a lesson or during a se-

quence

 of

  lessons. They also asked questions about

 the

 nature

 o f the

 capa-

bilit ies

 they wanted

 to

 develop

 in

 their stude nts,

 the

 necessary condi t ions

 for

developing these capa bi l it ies , how those condi t ions cou ld be m et , and the

kind o f

 ar rangements

 and

 metho ds that could

 b e

 used

 to

 create these co ndi-

t ions. These

  teachers

 did not

  start w ith questions abo ut generic at tributes;

they

 asked quest ions about

 how

 domain

 spec if ic

 g eneric at tributes co uld

 be

embedded  in the

  specific

  objects of  learning.

W e

 hope that

 the

 ex am ples described

  in

 th is book have succee ded

  in il-

lus t rat ing

 w ha t w e believe to be the three core elem ents necessary for dra-

mat ica l ly  improving learning  in  schools.  T he  first  e l emen t  is a  pr imary

focus

 on the

 objects

 of

 learning.

 T he

 second element

 is a

 collaborative

  effort

am ong teachers in planning and enac t ing the o bjects of learning in the class-

r o o m, as we l l a s ref lect ions on the enactment that a re f i rmly  focused on the

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 245/255

232

  E P I L O G U E

objects of learning. The third element is   powerfu l  theoretical tools  that

guide this planning , enactm ent

 and

 ref lect ion:

 a

 theory

 o f

 learning

 and a

 the-

ory of the role o f languag e in learning. W e hope tha t th is book has dem on-

strated ho w

 these

 three elem ents together can m ake a

 difference

  in learning .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 246/255

References

Alexandersson ,

  M .

 (1994).

  Focusing teacher

  consciousness:

  W h at

  do

  teachers

  d i rec t

the i r con sc iousness towards du r ing the i r t each ing?  In I . Car lg ren ,  G .  H a n d a l ,  & S .

Vaage (E ds . ), Teachers 'minds and act ions: Research on teachers ' th ink ing and prac-

t ice

  (pp .

 139-149).

  London: Palmer

 Press.

Barnes, D.

 (1 976).

 From comm unica t ion to curriculum.

 Harmondswor th , England: Pengu in .

Barnes ,

  D .,

  B n t t o n ,

  J., &

  R o se n ,

  H .

  ( 1969) .

  Language ,

  the

  learner

  a nd the

  school .

Harmondswor th , England: Penguin .

Behr , M .,

  Hare l ,

 G .,

 Post ,

 T ., &

 L e sh ,

 R .

 ( 1 993). Ra t iona l num ber s : Toward

  a

 s e m a n t i c

a n a l y s i s — e m p h a s i s o n th e

  opera tor const ruc t .

 In T. P.

 Carpenter ,

  E .

 F e n n e m a ,

 & T .

Romberg (Eds . ) ,

  Rational  numbers. An

  integration

  of

  research

  (pp .

  13-48).

H i l l s d a l e ,

 N J :

  Lawrence Er lbaum Assoc ia t e s .

Bere i te r , C. , &

  S c a r d a m a l ia ,

 M .

 (1 993).

  Surpass ing ourse lves .

  A n

  inquiry into

  the na -

ture

  a n d

  implications  o f  expert ise .

  Chicago: Open Court .

Berry,

 M .

 ( 19 8 7 ) .

 Is

 t eache r

 a n

 u nana lyze d concep t?

  In M . A. K.

 Ha l l ida y

 & R . F a w c c t t

(Eds . ) ,

 N e w

 deve lopments

  in

 system ic l inguist ics

  ( V o l .

 I ., pp . 41-63).

 L o n d o n : F r a n -

cis  Pinter .

B iggs ,

  J . B .

  (1996) . Western mispercept ions

  of the

  C o n f u c i a n -H e r i ta g e L e a r n i n g C u l -

t u r a l .

  In D. A.

 W a t k in s

 & J . B .

 Biggs (Eds. ) ,  T he

 Chinese learn er: Cultural , psycho-

logica l  a n d  contextual in f luences

  (pp .

 45-67).

 H o n g

 K o n g / M e l b o u rn e :

 C o m p a r a t iv e

Educa t i on

  Resea rch Cen te r .

Brans fo rd ,  Y. D. , Brow n, A. L . , & C ock ing , R. R. (Eds. ) . (2000).  H o w

  people learn .

Brain, mind, experience,

  a n d school. W a sh i n g to n , D C :  N a t i o n a l  A c a d e m y Press .

B n t t o n ,

  J .

 ( 1 9 7 0) . Language  a n d exper ience .  M i d dl e s e x , E n g l a n d : P e n g u i n .

B r o w n , A . L .  (1992) . Design expe r imen ts : Th eore t ica l  an d m e t h o d o l o g i c a l  c h a l l e n g e s

in  c r ea t ing com plex in te rven t ions

 in

 c lassroom se t t ings .  T he Journal  o f  the  Learning

Sciences,  2(2) , 141-178.

Brow n, G. A . , &  E d m o n d s o n , R . ( 1984) . A sk ing ques t ions . In E . C . W r ag g ( E d . ) , Class-

room teaching sk i l l s  (pp . 97-120).  L o n d o n : C r oo m H e l m .

B runer , J. (1987) . T h e tr ansac t iona l

 self .

 In J . B r u n e r & H . H a s t e ( E d s. ), The

 child 's con -

struction

  o f

  the wor ld

  (pp .

 81-96).

 L o n d o n a n d N e w Y o r k : R o u t l e d g e .

233

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 247/255

234  R E F E R E N C E S

Cambr idge Pub l i sher Com mi t tee . ( 1997) . M athemat ics

  (primary

  4 , f irs t term)

  [ in

 C h i -

nese].

 H ong Kong: Cam br idge Pu b l i sher s Ltd .

Car lsson,  B .  (1999).  Ecologica l unders tanding .

  A

  space

  o f  va r ia t ion .

 L u l e a , S w e d e n :

Lu lea

 U n i ve r s it y

 o f

 T ech n o lo g y .

Carpenter,

  T. P., &

 M oser,

 I . M .

 (1984).

 T he

  acquisi t ion

  of

 addit ion

  and

 subt ract ion con-

cepts in grades on e

 th rough

 three. Journal

 fo r

  Research

 in

 Mathemat ics Educat ion .

  15,

179-202.

Cestar i , M . L . (2001 , Ap r i l

 19-21).  The

 co-product ion  of  misunders tandings

  in

  mathe-

matics instruct ional discourse. P aper presen ted

 at I . A. D. A.

 2001 — R ecen t T ren ds

 in

D ia lo g u e A n a l y s i s,

 Goteborg , Sw eden .

C h a n ,

  S. (2002).

  Task-based teaching and learn ing for ESL

 young

  learners .  U n p u b -

l i shed

 doc toral disser ta t ion,

 Facu l t y  o f

 E d u c a t io n ,

 T h e

  Un ive r s i t y

  o f

  H o n g K o n g .

Chase,

 W . G., &

 S imon ,

 H. A.

 ( 1 973). Perception

 in

 chess.

 Cog ni t ive Psychology,

 4,55-81.

Ch en g ,

 K . M.

 ( 1992) . China educat ional

  reform

  [ in

 C h i n e s e ] . H o n g K o n g : C o m m e r c i a l

Press.

Ch ik , P.

 (2002).

 Qu ali tative  differences  in teaching  and  learn ing:  A s tudy  of  six  lessons

in  Chinese language

  in

 H o n g K o n g ' s pr ima ry schools. U n p u b l i sh e d

  Master 's thesis.

T he

  U n i ve r s i ty

 o f

 H o n g K o n g .

Cleverley,

 J .

 ( 1 985).

  The school ing  o f  China .

  S y dn ey , A u s t r a l i a :

 A l l e n  &  U n w i n .

C o h e n ,

 D . K., &  H i l l , H. C . (2000).

  In s t r u c t i o n a l p o l i cy

 an d

 c l a ss ro o m p e r fo r m an ce :

T he

 m a t h em a t i c s r e fo r m

 i n C a l i f o rn i a .  Teachers Col lege Record, 102(2), 294-343.

Col l ins , A .

 ( 1 992) . Tow ard

 a

 design science

 of

 edu ca t i o n .

 In E .

 Scan lon

 & T . O.

 S h ea

(Eds . ) ,

 Ne w direc t ions in educat ional t echnology.

  Ber l in ,

 Germ any : Spr inger.

Cortazzi ,

  M .

  (1998) . Learning from  A s ian l essons :

 Cu l tu ra l

  exper i ence

 an d

 c lassroom

ta lk. Educat ion  3 to 13, 26(2), 42-49.

Cortazzi , M ., & J in , L . (2001 ). Large classes in C h i n a : "Good" teachers an d

 interaction.

 In

D. W a t ki n s & J . B.  Biggs (Eds.) ,

  Teaching

  the  Chinese l earners: Psychologica l  an d

pedagog ica l perspectives  (pp .  115—134). Hong Kong &   Aust ra l ia : C E R C &  A C E R .

D e Groot,  A. D. ( 1965) .  Thoug ht and choice in chess .  T he  H a g u e : M o u t o n .

Eg an , D. E ., &  Schwar tz , B. I . ( 1 9 7 9 ). C h u n k i n g in reca l l o f symbol ic d raw i n g s . M e m -

ory  and Cogni t ion ,  7,

 149-158.

Eh rl ich , K. , &

 So loway ,

 E .

 (1984) .

 A n

 em pi r ica l inves t iga t ion

 of the

 t ac i t

 p lan

 k n o w l -

edge  in  p rogramming .  In I.  T h o m a s  & M . C .  Schneider (Eds. ) , H uma n f a c to r s  in

computer sys tems  (pp. 113-134) . Norwood, N J:  A b l e x .

Gardner,  H. ( 1 989).

  To

 op en mi nds .  N ew  York : B as ic Book s .

Glaser , R ., & C h i , M. T. H. ( 1 988) . Overv iew. In M . T. H. C h i , R . Glaser , & M . I. Far r

(Eds. ) ,  T he

 na ture

  o f  expert ise  ( p p . x v - x x v i i ) .  Hi l l sda l e ,

 N J :

  L a w r e n c e

 E r l b a u m

A sso c i a t e s .

Gu, L .

 ( 1991) . Xuehui

  J iaoxue

  [Learn ing

 to

 teach] .

 Be i j ing , Ch ina :

 Peo p le 's  Educa t ion

Press.

G u rwi c h ,

 A .

 (1964) .

  The  ield  o f  consc iousne ss .

 P it t sb u r g h : D u q u esn e Univers i ty Press.

Hall iday, M . A. K.

 (1973). Explora t ions in the functions

  of

  l a ngua ge .

 L ondon : Edw ard

Arno ld .

Hall iday, M . A. K.

 (1975) .

 Learning

  how to

 mean: E xplora t ions

  in the

 deve l op men t

  o f

l a ngua ge .

  London : Edw ard A rno ld .

Hall iday,

  M . A. K.  (1978) .  La ngua ge a s social semiotic:  The soc ia l  in terpreta t ion o f

language   m e a n i n g . London : E dward A rno ld .

Hall iday,  M . A. K. (1993) . Towards a langu age-based theory of le arnin g. Lingu ist ics

and Educat ion ,

  5, 93-116.

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 248/255

R E F E R E N C E S  235

Hal l id ay , M . A . K. , & Matthiessen, C . (1999). Construing experience through meaning:

A

  language-based approach  to

 cognition.

 London: Cassell.

H a w k i n s ,

  E .

 (1984). Awareness

  o f

  language:

  A n

  introduction.  Cambridge, England:

Cambridge University Press.

H a w k i n s ,

  E .

  (1999). Foreign language study

  an d

  language awareness.  Language

Awareness, 8(3 & 4), 124-142.

Ho,

  D. Y. F .

 (1991, June 29-July

 2 ).

 Cognitive socialization  in  Confucian  heritage cul-

tures. Paper presented

 to

 Workshop

 on

 Continuities

 and

 Discontinuities

 in the

 Cog-

n i t iv e

  Socialization

 o f

 Minority Children, U.S. Department

  o f

  H ea l t h

  an d

  H u m a n

Services, Washington, D C .

Hoare ,

  P .

 (2003). Effective

  teaching  of science through English

  in

 Hong Kong second-

a ry  schools.

  U n p u b l i s h e d

 doctoral dissertation,

 Facu l t y

  o f E d u c a t i o n ,  T h e

  Un ive r -

s i ty o f Hong Kong.

H u a n g ,

  R .

 (2002). Mathematics teaching

  in

 Hong Kong

  a n d

 Shanghai:

  A

  classroom

analys is  from  the perspective  o f variation.  Unpublished doctoral dissertation, T he

Unive rs i t y  o f Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China.

K i l p a t n c k ,

 J .,

 Sw af f o rd ,

  J., &

 F m d e l l,

 B .

 (Eds.). (2001). Adding  i t up: Helping children

learn mathematics.  Washington, D C : National Academy Press.

Ko,

  P. Y. (2002).  The notion  of teaching excellence  in the People s Republic

  o f

  China:

The  case

 o f

 Chinese language teachers. U n p u b l is h e d doctoral dissertation,

 F a c u l t y

of

  E d u c a t io n ,

 T he  Un ive r s i t y  o f Hong Kong.

K w a n ,

 T, Ng, F. P. , & Chik, P . (2002). Repetition and variation. In F. Marton & P. Morris

(Eds . ) ,

  W h a t

  matters? Discovering critical conditions

  o f

  classroom learning.

Goteborg ,

 Sweden: Acta

 Un ive rs it a t is

 Gothoburgensis.

L a m o n ,

 S . J . (1999).

  Teach ing f rac tions

  a n d ratios  o r understanding: Essential content

k n o wl edg e an d   instructional strategies  f o r  teachers.  Mahwah,  N J :  Lawrence

E r l b a u m Associates.

Lee ,

 W . O . (1996). The cultural context fo r Chinese learners: Conceptions  o f learning in

th e

  C o n f u c i a n

  t radi t ion . In D. A.

 W a tk ins

 & J . B . Biggs (Eds.), T he Chinese learner:

Cultura l ,

 psychological  a n d  contextual influences  (pp. 25-42).  Hong Kong/Mel-

b o u r n e : Comparative Education Research Center.

Lesgo ld ,

 A. S .

 (1 988).

 Problem solving.

 In R . J .

 Sternberg

 & E . E .

 Smith (Eds.),

 The

 psy-

chology   o f human thought (pp. 188-213).  N ew York: Cambridge University Press.

L e u n g , Y . M . J . (1991). C u r r i c u l u m development in the People's Republic o f C h i n a . In

C .

  Marsh

  & P .

 Morris (Eds.), Curriculum development  in East Asia  (pp. 61-81).

Bris to l ,  E n g l a n d : Palmer Press.

Lo, M . L. , &  Chik, P . M . (2000, January).

 Tw o

 faces ofprogressivism.  Paper presented a t

13th  In te rna t iona l

  Congress  fo r  School Ef fec t iveness  an d  Improvement, HKSAR,

C h i n a .

Lo, M . L. , & Ko, P. Y. (2002). The

 "enacted"

 object of

  learn ing .

 In F. Marton & P . Morns

(Eds . ) ,

  W h a t

  matters? Discovering critical conditions

  o f

  learning  (pp.

 59-74).

Goteborg , Sweden: Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis.

L u n d g r e n ,

  U. P . (1977).  Model analyses

  o f

  pedagogical  processes.  Lund, Sweden:

G l e e r u p .

L y b e c k , L .

 ( 1 9 8 1 ) .

 Arkimedes  i klassen [Archimedes in the classroom]. Goteborg, Swe-

den :

  Acta Univers i ta t i s

 Gothoburgensis.

M a l i n o w s k i ,

 B . (1946). T he problem of meaning in primitive languages. In C. K . Ogden

& I . A . Richards (Eds.),  The meaning  o f meaning (8th ed., pp .  296—336). London:

Kegan   P a u l .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 249/255

236  R E F E R E N C E S

M arlon, F . (2001 ).  The

 learning

  s tudy.  U n p u b l i s h e d m a n u s c r ip t .

Mar ton ,  F., &  Boo th ,  S .

 (1997).

  Learning  a n d

  awareness.

  M a h w a h ,  N J:  L a w r e n c e

E r l b a u m

  Assoc ia t e s .

Mar ton ,  F., & M orris,  P . (E ds.). (2002).  W h a t matters

7

 Discovering cr i t ical cond i t ions

o f  c lassroom

  learning.

  Go tebo rg , Sweden : Ac ta Un ive r s i t a t i s Go thoburgens i s .

Mil l e r , G. A .

 ( 1956) .

 T h e

 m a g i c a l n u m b e r s ev e n , p l u s

 or

 m i n u s t w o . S o m e  l i m i t s

 o n o u r

capac i ty  to process i n f o r m a t i o n . Psycho logical Revie\ \ ,\ 6 3. 81-87.

M ok, I . A . C . (2000, J u l y 23-27). The ana tom y o f an "open" m a t h e m a t i c s le s so n . In T .

N a k a h a r

  & M .

  Koyma (Eds. ) ,  Proceedings

  o f

  24 th Con ference  o f  the  In ternat ional

Gr o u p

 for the

  Psycho logy

  o f

  M athemat ics Educat ion ,  H i r o s h i m a , J a p a n .

M ok, I . A. C . (2002) . Th e a naly sis o f an

 "open"

  m a the m at i c s l e sson . In Z . Da i ( E d . ) ,

Open -en ded

  questions:

  N ew m o de l s i n

  mathematics

  pedag o g y

  ( in Ch inese ; pp .

90-105). S h a n g h a i, C h i n a : E d u c a t i o n a l P u b l i s h in g H o u s e .

M o k , I. A. C.,  Ch ik , P . M ., Ko , P. Y., K w a n , T., Lo, M . L., M a r to n , F ., Ng , F. P. , Pang , M .

F .,

 & R u n e s so n , U . (1999, Decem ber) .

  B e i n g g o o d  a t language teach ing .

  Paper pre-

sented  a t  Depar tmenta l Conference  of the  Depar tment  o f  Cur r i cu lum S tud ies ,  T h e

Univers i ty

  o f

  Hong Kong .

Mok , I. A. C.,  Runesson, U ., Tsui , A. B. M., W ong, S. Y., Chik , P., &  Pow, S . (2002). Qu es-

tions  and  variation. In F. M arton & P. M oms (Eds.) ,  What

  mat ters

7

  Discover ing cr i t ical

cond i t ions o f classroom

 learn ing .

 Goteborg, S weden: A cta

 Un iversitatis

 Gothoburgensis.

Mox ley , S . E . (1979). Schema: The var iab i l i ty of prac t ice hy pothesis .

 Journal

  of

  Mo t i o n

Behavior ,

 2, 65-70.

M u l l i s ,

 I . V . S . , M ar t in , M . O . , Gonza les , E. J ., Gregory,  K. D. , Garden , R . A . , O 'C o n n o r ,

K .

 M . , Chrostow ski , S . J . , & Sm ith , T . A. (2000) .

  TIMSS

  1999 In ternational M athe-

mat ics Repo r t : Findings f rom   IE

 A

  's repeat

  o f

  th e  Third  In ternational M athemat ics

a n d

 Science

  Study at the eighth

  grade.

 C h e s t n u t  Hi l l ,  MA: Bos ton Co l l ege , Lynch

Schoo l o f

 E d u c a t io n ,

 T h e

  In te rn a t ion a l S tudy Cen te r .

N e u m a n ,  D . (1 987).  T he origin  o f

 arithmetic

 skills. Goteborg, Sw eden : A c ta Un ive r s i -

tal is  Go thenburgens i s .

Ng , F . P. ,

 Tsu i ,

 A. B . M ., &

 M a r to n ,

 F .

 (2001) .

 Tw o

 f aces

 of the

 reed re lay .

 In D .

 W a t k m s

& J . B iggs (E ds . ),  Teaching  the  Chinese learner  (pp . 135-160) . H ong Kong : Com -

para t ive Educa t ion Resea rch Cen t r e (CERC) , T h e

  U n i v e r s it y

  o f  H o n g K o ng .

Ogborn, J ., Kress, G ., M artins,  L , & McG i l li cuddy , K . (1996).  Explaining  science

 in the

c lassroom.  P h i l a d e l p h i a : O p e n  Univ e r s i ty  Press .

Pa ine , L .

 (1 990) .

  T h e

  t e a c h e r

 a s

 v i r tuoso :

 A

 C h i n e se m o d e l

  fo r

 t e a c h i n g . Teachers Col -

lege Record ,  92(

 1 ),

 49-81.

Pong , W . Y , & M ar ton , F . (2001) . Concept ions as ways  o f  be ing aware  of  someth ing—

accounting for in ter- and in tra-con textual  shifts  in the mean ing o f  f v v o  ec o n o m i c

phenomena.

 U n p u b l i s h ed m a n u s c r i p t.

Qian, M . (1985).

 The explorat ion  o n guided-reading approach  o n  Chinese language

  [ in

Chinese ] . Y una n : Yunan P eop le ' s Pub l i she r .

R u n e s so n ,

 U .

 ( 1 999) .  Variat ionen spedag ogik . Ski lda sal t

  a t t

 behandla

  e t t

  matemat i sk t

innehall

  [The pedagogy of var ia t ion .  Dif fe ren t  w a y s o f h a n d l i n g a m a t h e m a t i c a l

con ten t ] . Go tebo rg , Sweden : Ac ta Un ive r s i t a t i s Go thoburgens i s .

Sabers, D. S . ,

 Gushing ,

 K. S. , &

 Berliner,

  D . C .

 (1 991) . Dif ferences among teachers

 in a

t a sk cha rac te ri zed by s im u l t ane i ty ,

  m u l t i d im ens iona l i t y ,

 and imm ediacy .

 American

Educat ion Research Journal ,  28,

 63-88.

Saljo,  R .  (1982).

  Learning

  and unders tanding: A s tudy o f

  differences

  in

 constructing

meaning

 f rom  a  text.  Go teborg , Sweden : A c ta Un ive r s i t a t i s Go thobu rgens i s .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 250/255

R E F E R E N C E S  237

Sapir , E . (196 1 ) . Culture, languag e

  an d

 person al i ty .  Berkeley and Los A ngeles : Unive r -

si ty

  o f

 Cal i forn ia  Press.

Seybo l t , P. J . (1 973). Revo lut ionary educat ion in China. New York : In terna t ional Arts &

Science Press.

Stevenson, H. W ., & St igler, J . W. (1992). T he learning ga p. New York : Sum mi t Books .

Stevenson,

 W ., &

 Lee,

 S .

 (1997).

 T he

 E ast A sian version

 of

 w hole-c lass teaching .

 In W .

K.

 G u m m i ng

 & P. G.

 A l tback (Eds. ),  The chal lenge  of  Eastern Asian educat ion.

  A l-

b an y :  State Universi ty of New York Press.

Stigler ,

 J . W ., & Hiebe rt , J . (1 999).  The

 teaching gap: Best ideas from

  the

 world 's teach-

ers fo r

  improving educat ion

  in the

  classroom.  N ew

  York:

 T he

  Free Press.

Svensson , L . (1976) .

  Study sk i l l s and  l ea rn ing .

 Goteborg , S w eden: A cta Univers i ta t i s

Gothoburgens i s .

Ts u i ,

 A. B . M . (1 994).

  Engl ish conversat ion.

  Lon don : Oxfo rd U nive rs it y  Press.

Tsu i ,

 A . B . M . (1 995) . Introducing c lassroom interact ion.  London : Pen gu in .

Tsu i , A. B . M . ,

 A ldred ,

 D .,

 M ar ton ,

 F .,

 Kan ,

 F ., &

 Runesson ,

 U .

 (2001) .  The

 medium

  o f

ins truct ion and the space   o f  l ea rn ing .

 Un publ i shed m anusc rip t.

Voigt , J.

 (1 995) . Them at ic pa t te rns

  of

  interact ion

 and

  soc io-mathemat ica l norms.

  In P.

Cobb & H. Ba uersfe ld (Eds . ) , The emergence  of ma thematical me aning: In teract ion

in classroom cul tures  (pp. 163-201). Hi l l sda l e , NJ : Lawrence Er lbaum A ssoc ia t e s .

V u o l a b , K .  (2000). Such  a t reasure  of know ledge  fo r hum an survival . In R.  Phi l l ipson

(Ed.) , Rights  to  language: Equi ty , power a n d educat ion  (pp. 13-16), M a h w a h ,

 N J:

Lawrence Er lbaum Assoc i at e s.

Wells ,

  G. (1986) .  The meaning makers : Chi ldren l earn ing language and us ing lan-

guage   to  l ea rn . Po r t smou th , N H :  He inemann .

Wells ,

 G. (1999).  Dialogic inquiry.  Cam bridge , UK : Cam bridge U nivers i ty Press .

Wis ted t ,  I., &  Mar t in sson ,  M .  (1 994) . Kvaliteter  i e levers tdnkande over  e n  odndl ig

decimalutveckling  [Qua l i t i e s in h ow  ch i ld ren know abou t  an end less ly repea t ing

decim al ] . Report f rom  th e Pedago g ica l In s t i t u t e o f S tockho lm U n ive rs i ty , Sw eden .

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 251/255

  his page intentionally left blank

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 252/255

Author Ind ex

Aldred, D. , 157

Alexandersson ,

  M. , 165

B

B arnes, D. , 230

B e hr ,

  M. , 72

Bere i t e r ,

 C, 8

Ber l ine r , D. C., 8

Berry,  M . , 1 13

Biggs ,  J. B., 62

Booth ,

  S ., 8, 11 , 22, 89,

 153, 165, 170, 193,

1 9 4 , 2 1 5

Bransford ,

  Y. D., 8

Br i t t on ,  J., 26, 35, 230

B r o w n ,  A. L., 8, 191, 1 9 2 , 2 1 1

Brown, G. A. , 128

Bruner , J . , 166

Chros towsk i ,

  S. J., 66

Cleverley ,

  J., 60

C o ck in g ,

  R. R., 8

Cohen ,  D. K., 190

Col l i ns ,  A . ,

 191,

  1 9 2 , 2 1 1

Cor tazz i ,

 M . , 62

Gu s h i n g ,  K. S., 8

DeGroot ,  A. D., 7

Edmondson ,

  R. , 128

Egan,  D. E., 8

Ehr l i ch ,  K., 8

Findel l ,

  B., 190

Car l s son ,

  B., 23

Carpenter, . P., 14

C e s t a n , M . L., 32, 33, 34

Chan ,  S., 141

Chase , W . G., 8

Cheng, K. M., 61

C h i ,  M. T. H., 8

Chik , P . ,

  1 3 , 43 , 91 ,

  1 21

Garden ,  R. A., 66

Gardner,  H. , 60

G laser, R ., 8

Gonzales, E. J . , 66

Gregory ,

  K. D., 66

Gu, L., 56, 57, 58, 59, 60

G u r w i c h ,  A. , 19

239

 

H

E

F

G

 

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 253/255

240

A U T H O R  I N D E X

H

Hall iday,

 M. A. K., 24, 25, 26, 27, 28,

  139,

1 7 9 , 2 3 0

Harel ,

  G., 72

H a w k i n s ,  E., 230

Hiebert,

  J., 45, 66, 191

H i l l ,

 H . C . ,

  1 90

Ho, D. Y. F., 62

Hoare ,

  P., 143

Hua ng ,

  R., 62

J

J in ,

  L. , 62

K

Kan, F., 157

Kilpat r ick, J . , 190

Ko, P. Y.,  1 0 ,4 6 , 91 ,  167

Kress,

  G. , 117

K w a n , T . , 4 3 , 9 1

N g ,  P . P . , 43, 91 , 11 4

o

O ' C o n n o r ,

 K. M ., 66

O gb or n ,  J . , 117

Paine , L . , 6 1 , 6 2

Pa ng ,  M. F., 91

P o n g ,  W . Y . , 2 1 9

Po st, T., 72

Pow,  S., 121

Qian ,

  M ., 46

R

Rosen, H. , 230

Ru nesson, U. , 67, 91, 1 21, 157

Lamon, S . J . , 196

Lee, S., 61

Lee,  W. O., 62

Lesg old, A. S . , 8

Lesh, R . , 72

L e u n g ,

 Y. M . J. , 60

Lo, M. L . ,

  1 0 , 9 1 ,

  1 67

L undgr e n ,  U. P., 128

Lybeck, L. , 153

M

M a l i n o w s k i , B . , 27

M a r t i n ,  M. O., 66

Mar t ins ,  I . , 117

Mar t insson , M. , 153

Marlon, F., 8, 11, 22, 89, 91, 114, 153, 157,

165,  170, 192 , 193 , 1 94 ,2 1 5 , 21 9

M atthiessen, C., 24, 139

McGi l l i cuddy, K. , 117

Mil le r ,

 G. A , , 9

M o k ,

  I . A.

  C . , 7 5 , 9 1 ,

  1 21

Moser,  I. M., 14

Mo x ley ,

  S. E., 15, 16

M ul l i s ,

  I. V. S., 66

N

N e u m a n ,

 D., 7

Sabers ,

  D. S., 8

Sal jo ,

  R., 90

Sapir, E. , 27

Scardamalia ,  M . , 8

Schwar tz ,  B. I. , 8

Seybol t ,

  P. J., 60

S i m o n ,

  H. A., 8

Sm ith, T. A. , 66

Soloway,

  E., 8

Stevenson,

  H. W ., 66, 67

Stevenson ,

 W . , 61

Stigler,  J. W ., 45, 66, 67, 1 91

Svensson ,  L., 11, 58, 74

SwafFord,

  J., 190

T su i ,

  A. B. M. ,

  11 4 , 1 21 , 128, 157,

  1 67

Viogt ,  J., 36, 37

V u o l a b ,  K., 28

W

Wel l s , G.,  179,  1 82

Wistedt,  1., 153

Wong, S . Y, 121

R

Q

S

TT

V

L

P

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 254/255

8/9/2019 Classroom Discourse and the Space of Learning

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/classroom-discourse-and-the-space-of-learning 255/255

242

 

S U B J E C T

  I N D E X

M

M a t h e m a t i c s

  lessons, 32-34, 36-37, 62-63,

66-73,75-84,86,91-94,

140-141, 196-203

div i s ion ,  74

div i s ion-quot i en t , 92-93

Phy sics lessons , 1 17-1 21,  128-136,

169-170

reed relay , 114-121

Presuppos i t ions , 1 1 7 , 166,

  1 78