Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT...

26
Running head: CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 1 Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements in Detailed Illustrations of Storybooks. Yvet Diamanti-Knteis S1162535 Leiden University Master of Education and Child Studies Specialization: Learning Problems and Impairments Academic year 2014-2015 Supervisor: Takacs, Zs. K. (Zsofia)

Transcript of Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT...

Page 1: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

Running head: CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 1

Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements in Detailed Illustrations of

Storybooks.

Yvet Diamanti-Knteis

S1162535

Leiden University

Master of Education and Child Studies

Specialization: Learning Problems and Impairments

Academic year 2014-2015

Supervisor: Takacs, Zs. K. (Zsofia)

Page 2: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 2

Abstract

The present study compared looking behavior of preschoolers at different visual

elements in storybook illustrations. Fourteen Dutch children, aged from 4 to 6, were

recruited. Three detailed illustrations from a storybook were used, in which areas of interest

were created, distinguishing the elements between humanoid and non-humanoid figures,

elements highlighted and not highlighted by the oral text, and combinations of the two

categories. Children’s eye movements on the illustrations were recorded with an eye-tracking

device. For each picture, separate paired-sample t-tests were conducted and results indicated

that children integrated the oral text of the story and the illustrations that were accompanying

it. Children’s visual attention to the illustrations focuses mostly on humanoid figures that are

highlighted by the accompanied text of the storybook. Recommendations for future research

and practice implications are discussed.

Page 3: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 3

Introduction

There is ample evidence that storybook reading is beneficial for children (Chun &

Plass 1996; Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; Guttmann, Levin & Pressley, 1977; Hudson &

Lawson, 1996; Hyona, 2010; de Jong & Bus 2002; Justice, Pullen, & Pence, 2008; Sénéchal,

LeFevre, Hudson, & Lawson, 1996; Verhallen, Bus, & de Jong, 2006; Verhallen & Bus,

2011). Shared storybook reading is beneficial for children, because it makes a contribution to

the development of a child’s knowledge about the world, increasing their motivation for later

book reading, and functioning as a useful tool to stimulate the reader’s imagination and

creativity (Bus, van IJzendoorn & Pellegrini, 1995; Evans, Williamson, & Pursoo, 2008;

Fang, 1996; Greenhoot & Semb, 2008; Guttmann et al., 1977; Sénéchal et al., 1996).

Although there might be concerns that pictures in a storybook create distractions from

the linguistic content of the story and that children would depend only on the illustrations for

the comprehension of the story (Beck & McKeown, 2001), many researchers point out the

important role of pictures in storybooks for children’s comprehension of the story (Chun &

Plass 1996; de Jong & Bus 2002; Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; Guttmann et al., 1977, Justice

et al., 2008; Sénéchal et al., 1996; Sipe, 1998, 2012; Verhallen et al., 2006; Verhallen & Bus,

2011). Pictures in a storybook contribute to the learning of new vocabulary, if the words

from the oral text are depicted with supportive indication of their context (Evans & Saint-

Aubin, 2005). Another important role of pictures would be completing the text by giving

extra information that the text in the storybook does not provide and vice versa (Greenhoot &

Semb, 2008; Sipe, 1998, 2012; Verhallen & Bus, 2011).

According to Fang’s (1996) view on the role of illustrations in picture storybooks,

preschoolers usually have short attention spans, and, combined with their restricted

vocabulary and syntax, they rely more heavily on pictures to comprehend the meaning of the

written text than on the text itself. Comprehension of the story might be supported by

Page 4: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 4

illustrations that supply referential cues for the text. When the text has vague referents about

pronouns and location words, their interpretation depends on the way that they are depicted in

the illustration. Pictures can also determine the atmosphere of a storybook and may add a

different viewpoint than the one presented in the text.

Sipe (1998) discussed the relations between words and images in picture storybooks,

and characterized these relationships as “synergistic” and “complicated”. The different types

of information that children process during listening to a story and watching the

corresponding illustrations can be interpreted with Paivio’s dual-coding model (Sadoski,

2005). According to this model, there are two separate cognitive structures for information

processing; one for processing language and one for managing non-verbal information such

as images (Paivio, 1986; Sadoski & Paivio, 2004; Sadoski, 2005). These two separate

systems, both play their individual roles in the integration of the illustrations and the

narration of the story. At the same time, however, they are working together in an

interconnected fashion, (Mayer & Anderson, 1992; Paivio, 1986; Sadoski & Paivio, 2004;

Sadoski, 2005).

The activation of both the non-verbal and the verbal system (as explained in the dual

coding theory of Paivio), allows for the hypothesis that illustrations in a storybook support

children’s story comprehension (Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; Verhallen & Bus, 2011). The

illustrations are making the abstract language of the story more specific and understandable

by depicting the verbal information in a way that may contribute to the setting up of more

powerful, more complicated and better organized memory traces (Levin & Mayer, 1993).

It has been shown that children fixate more on the elements of the picture that are

highlighted by the oral text as compared to elements that are not mentioned in the oral text

during picture storybook reading (Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; Verhallen & Bus, 2011). It

has also been suggested that this preference for the parts of the illustration that are

Page 5: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5

highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral text, shows that children

follow the story line on the illustration with their eyes, and thus, integrate the visual and the

verbal information (Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; Verhallen & Bus, 2011). This is in line with

the dual coding theory (Paivio, 1986; Sadoski & Paivio, 2004; Sadoski, 2005). Story

comprehension is more effective when it is framed in both visual and lingual ways (Chun &

Plass, 1996; Sénéchal et al., 1996).

Buswell (1935) discovered that, within a detailed illustration, humans demonstrate a

strong preference for looking more at people in comparison to nonliving objects. This finding

has been replicated consistently confirming this strong visual preference for humanoid

elements or elements with anthropomorphic characteristics (Tatler, Wade, Kwan, Findlay,

Velichkovsky, 2010; Yarbus, 1967). Human figures are to be the most favored targets and a

likely explanation is the capability of the observers to understand the figures’ intentions and

goal directed actions and reactions (Fang, 1996; Verhallen & Bus, 2011). That means that

children might fixate longer on the human or anthropomorphic figures of the illustrations of

picture storybooks in their effort to have a better comprehension of the story by recognizing

features of the main characters and their acts (Fang, 1996; Verhallen & Bus, 2011).

Given the supportive role illustrations play in children's picture books and in

children's language and literacy development, the present study investigates where children’s

visual attention to the illustrations focuses by answering the following research questions:

1. Do humanoid/anthropomorphic figures in a picture attract the attention of

preschoolers more than the non-humanoid ones during storybook reading?

For the first research question it was expected that preschoolers would focus longer at

the humanoid elements of the illustrations compared to nonliving objects. Previous studies

showed that anthropomorphic visual elements attracted children’s attention more than the

Page 6: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 6

ones that were not anthropomorphic (Buswell, 1935; van der Geest et al., 2002; Fletcher-

Watson et al., 2008; Tatler et al., 2010; Verhallen & Bus, 2011; Yarbus, 1967).

2. Do children pay more attention to the visual elements of a picture that are

highlighted in the narration than the ones irrelevant to the oral text?

From the previously discussed research, it was hypothesized that preschoolers would

fixate more on the text-relevant visual elements in comparison to irrelevant ones in their

effort to follow the story and integrate the visual and the verbal information. Such a finding

would suggest that preschoolers are able to integrate the oral text of the story and the

illustrations accompanying it, as the dual coding theory suggests (Paivio, 1986; Sadoski &

Paivio, 2004; Sadoski, 2005).

3. Since the two questions above can be confounded, an additional question was

asked for a stricter test of the effect of the oral narration: When there are humanoid figures in

the pictures, do children prefer to focus more on the ones that are also highlighted in the text

of the story compared to the ones that are not mentioned?

It was predicted that preschoolers would fixate most often and longest at the elements

of the picture that are highlighted by the text and have an anthropomorphic figure based on

previous researches (Buswell, 1935; Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; van der Geest et al., 2002;

Fletcher-Watson et al., 2008; Tatler et al., 2010; Verhallen & Bus, 2011; Yarbus, 1967).

Method

Participants

Page 7: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 7

For this study 14 Dutch kindergartners were recruited from three regular primary

schools. The participants were all between 4 and 6 years of age (M= 4.57 years; SD = 0.76),

from both genders (seven boys, seven girls). Headmasters from each school were contacted

for approval in order to conduct the experiment at the school. Parents of children that

participated in the study gave written informed consent. Parents also filled out questionnaires

about their educational level, their socioeconomic status, and other background information

that is not discussed further in this study. There was missing background information from a

total number of five children; the parents of three participants did not hand in the

questionnaire, and two did not respond to all the questions of the questionnaire. The highest

educational level of the mothers varied among the participants and included 28.6% with a

bachelor or master degree, 21.4% with vocational education, and 14.3% with a high school

diploma. Paternal education levels were slightly higher included 14.3% with a master or

higher degree, 35.7% with a bachelor degree, and 14.3% with vocational education.

Design

The current study was part of a larger experiment where animated illustrations were

used in comparison to static. According to a within-subject design, children were exposed to

two of the three storybooks used in the study; one with animated illustrations and one with

static illustrations, both repeated three times. A third book served as the control condition..

The books were evenly distributed in the three conditions. The order of presentation was

counterbalanced, meaning that half of the children first encountered the dynamic version of a

storybook and then a static story, while the other half had the static storybook first and then

the dynamic story, ensuring that the order in which participants encounter the stories could

not affect their attention due to fatigue or environmental factors. The three storybooks were

evenly distributed over the three conditions by creating six separate groups of subjects, i.e.

Page 8: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 8

group 1 had the static version of the book Imitators (Na-apers!) (Veldkamp, 2006), the

dynamic version of Little Kangaroo (Kleine Kangoeroe) (van Genechten, 2005) and Bear is

in love with the butterfly (Beer Is Op Vlinder) (Haeringen, 2004) was the control condition,

while group 2 had the static version Bear is in love with the butterfly (Beer Is Op Vlinder)

(Haeringen, 2004), the dynamic version of Little Kangaroo (Kleine Kangoeroe) (van

Genechten, 2005) and Imitators (Na-apers!) as the control condition. In the present study

only the data of the groups that had Imitators (Na-apers!) in its static version was used.

Procedure

There were two sessions in total that took place inside the school. Dutch

undergraduate students collected the preschoolers individually from their classroom during

school hours and brought them back to class after the session. Preschoolers were awarded

with a “certificate” which they could fill with stickers in order to increase their motivation to

continue further with the experiment.

Standardized tests like Peg Tapping Task (Diamond & Taylor, 1996), Digit Span Test

(forward and backward parts) (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1983) and Head Toes Knees Shoulders

Task (Pontiz, et al., 2008) were used at the beginning of each session to measure children’s

skills of inhibitory control, working memory and attention. The results of these tests were

used as background measures to test if those skills have an effect on children’s reading

comprehension.

During the first session were posited in front of the screen of the eye-tracking device

where the two stories were played twice. During the second session participants listened to

the two stories once more.

When listening to the stories, children were seated approximately 60 cm from the

computer monitor, and were asked to stay still. After adjusting the best possible position for

the children, a calibration procedure took place. During this calibration participants followed

Page 9: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 9

five consecutive dots with their eyes that appeared in different positions on the Tobii screen.

All sessions were recorded with a digital camera in a fixed position.

Materials

Three picture storybooks were chosen for the experiment; Bear is in love with the

butterfly (Beer Is Op Vlinder) (Haeringen, 2004), Little Kangaroo (Kleine Kangoeroe) (van

Genechten, 2005) and Imitators (Na-apers!) (Veldkamp, 2006). These books were chosen

because they were all age-appropriate for the participants of this experiment, they had an

animated version of the story, making possible to compare the animated version with the

static one, and their illustrations had many elements that could be categorized in different

groups (non-humanoid, highlighted by the text, etc.) to answer the research questions of this

study. For this study only the book Imitators (Na-apers!) (Veldkamp, 2006) was used in its

static version. Three detailed pictures were chosen for analyses of the eye-tracking data. The

oral narrations of the stories were recorded with no sound or music effects that might be

distracting (Smeets et al., 2012). The pictures typically illustrate what the text describes but

they also illustrate details that are not mentioned in the text.

Measurement Instruments

Visual attention. While listening to the narration, children’s eye movements on the

illustrations were recorded with a Tobii eye-tracking device (Tobii TX120). With the eye-

tracking method it is possible to explore when the child first fixated on a particular part of the

illustrations, the duration of all those fixations and the elements that attracted the participants’

attention the most (Hyona, 2010). Tobii was chosen mostly because it is user friendly since

Tobii screen has built-in high-resolution cameras; no other equipment, like special glasses,

was needed (Tobii Technology, 2011). Thus, participants were not aware of the fact that their

Page 10: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 10

eye movements were recorded so they were spontaneous in their looking behavior. The Tobii

eye-tracking device requires a calibration process before the start of each recording (Tobii

Technology, 2011). A sampling rate of 120 Hz was used. The eye-tracking data was analyzed

using the Tobii Studio 3.1 software.

In this study the focus was on total fixation duration at the different visual elements.

Fixation means the point that eyes stand still on an element (Rayner, 1998). Previous findings

suggest that longer fixations indicate information processing and cognitive activities (Rayner,

1998). Average fixation times were calculated for the different types of elements over the

three sessions.

Areas of interest. Based on the research questions of this study areas of interest were

created in the Tobii Studio™ Analysis Software. The areas of interest were drawn in all the

three pictures that were used in this study.

The first distinction that was made between the elements in all the illustrations was if

they were humanoid or non-humanoid. For example in Figure 1, Mom Monkey is considered

a humanoid element, while the lamp a non-humanoid one. Secondly, a distinction was made

between the elements that were highlighted by the text and the ones that were not. For

example in Figure 3, the parents are mentioned in the text but none of the monkeys were

highlighted. Another research question regarded the combinations of the two kinds of

distinctions, such as elements that were both humanoid and highlighted by the text or

elements that were humanoid but not mentioned in the text. For example in picture 2, Mom

Monkey was highlighted by the text and was also considered a humanoid figure.

Most of the elements that were both humanoid and highlighted by the text or elements

that were humanoid but not mentioned in the text were often positioned at the center of the

pictures, while non-humanoid elements that were not mentioned in the text were more in the

Page 11: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 11

background of the illustration and tended to be smaller. This is a common tendency in

storybook illustrations to position essential elements of the story in the center of the picture.

Page 12: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 12

Figure1. Picture 1, scene from the static version of Imitators (Na-apers!) showing mother monkey and

Jaap’s mother having coffee. The areas of interest are marked with different colors. The accompanied

text was: “Then they shake coffee” (“Daarna schudden ze koffie”)

Page 13: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 13

Figure 2. Picture 2 scene from the static version of Imitators (Na-apers!) with the areas of interest

marked with different colors. The accompanied text was: “Jaaps’ mother walked on her hands”

(“Jaaps moeder aafte (liep) op haar handen.”)

Page 14: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 14

Figure 3. Picture 3 from the static version of Imitators (Na-apers!) showing Jaaps with his parents in

the zoo. The areas of interest are marked with different colors. The accompanied text was: “Jaap and his

parents were released” (“Jaap en zijn ouders liet hij vrij.”)

Page 15: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 15

Univariate Data Inspection

All variables were inspected for missing values, outliers and normal distribution.

Three children had poor quality data on one of the three pictures at the first session,

and for that reason their data from this session was not used in this study. The averages of the

total time that preschoolers fixated in each picture in the three different sessions were

calculated. There were no missing values on these average fixation duration variables.

The averages of the total time that preschoolers fixated on different visual elements in

each picture over the three different sessions were calculated. Normal distribution of these

variables was inspected with the help of histograms and boxplots. With boxplots extreme and

outlying scores were detected. Winsorizing was applied for the extremely low scores of three

children in total. In order for the distribution not to be influenced by these extreme outliers,

these scores were replaced with scores closer to the distribution but still lower than the lowest

value, to achieve normal distribution of the scores on the variables. After winsorizing normal

distribution was checked on the variables based on the skewness and kurtosis statistics. Table

1 shows the descriptive statistics of the different visual elements in each picture. Further on,

paired samples t-tests were used for the comparison of average mean time of children’s

looking behavior at the different types of elements in all three pictures.

Page 16: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 16

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics of the Average Time that Children Fixated on the Different Kinds of Elements

on the Three Target Pictures.

N Min. Max. Mean SD

Highlighted elements pic1 14 0.67 1.93 1.49 0.41

Highlighted elements pic2 14 0.50 1.74 1.20 0.31

Highlighted elements pic3 14 0.64 2.35 1.45 0.47

Not Highlighted elements pic1 14 0.51 1.39 0.82 0.27

Not Highlighted elements pic2 14 1.01 3.54 1.89 0.66

Not Highlighted elements pic3 14 0.09 0.77 0.44 0.26

Humanoid elements pic1 14 2.06 2.63 2.30 0.17

Humanoid elements pic2 14 1.04 2.72 1.99 0.40

Humanoid elements pic3 14 1.38 2.35 1.81 0.28

Non-Humanoid elements pic1 14 0.00 0.22 0.08 0.08

Non-Humanoid elements pic2 14 0.60 1.86 1.12 0.34

Non-Humanoid elements pic3 14 0.00 0.23 0.11 0.07

Not Highlighted, Humanoid elements pic1 14 0.40 1.30 0.74 0.27

Not Highlighted, Humanoid elements pic2 14 0.15 1.68 0.81 0.40

Not Highlighted, Humanoid elements pic3 14 0.00 0.70 0.32 0.26

Highlighted, Humanoid elements pic1 14 0.67 1.93 1.49 0.41

Highlighted, Humanoid elements pic2 14 0.50 1.74 1.20 0.31

Highlighted, Humanoid elements pic3 14 0.64 2.35 1.45 0.47

Not Highlighted, Non-Humanoid elements pic1 14 0.00 0.22 0.08 0.08

Not Highlighted, Non-Humanoid elements pic2 14 0.60 1.86 1.12 0.34

Not Highlighted, Non-Humanoid elements pic3 14 0.00 0.23 0.11 0.07

Page 17: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 17

Results

For each picture separate paired-sample t-tests were conducted to compare looking

behavior at the visual elements that were humanoid and non-humanoid. In picture 1, there

was a significant difference in fixation time on humanoid (M = 2.30, SD = .17) and non-

humanoid elements (M = .08, SD = .08); t(13) = 36.24, p < .001, d = 16.32. The same effect

was also present in picture 2, where there was a significant difference in visual attention on

humanoid (M = 1.99, SD = .40) and non-humanoid elements (M = 1.12, SD = .34); t(13) =

6.06, p < .001, d = 2.34. In picture 3 the same pattern was observed with a significant

difference in fixation time on humanoid (M =1.81, SD = .28) and non-humanoid elements (M

= .11, SD = .07); t(13) = 25.81, p < .001, d = 8.33. These results indicate that longer

fixations were made on all three pictures for the visual elements that were humanoid as

compared to non-humanoid ones.

For each picture separate paired-sample t-tests were conducted to compare looking

behavior at the visual elements that were highlighted by the text and the ones that were not.

See the descriptive statistics in Table 1. In picture 1, there was a significant difference in

looking behavior between the elements that were highlighted (M = 1.49, SD = .41) and the

ones that were not (M = .82, SD = .27); t(13) = 3.91, p = .002, d = 0.69. The same was true

for picture 3, there was also a significant difference in fixation time on highlighted (M = 1.45,

SD = .47) and not highlighted elements (M = .44, SD = .26); t(13) = 6.04, p < .001, d = 2.66.

These results suggest that longer fixations were made for the visual elements that were

highlighted by the text on picture 1 and 3. In picture 2, there was also a significant difference

in fixation between the elements that were highlighted (M = 1.20, SD = .31) and not (M =

1.89, SD = .66); t(13) = -2.93, p = .01, d = 1.34. However, the difference was in the opposite

direction in this picture. This suggests that children’s attention on the second picture was

more attracted by the visual elements that were not highlighted by the text as compared to the

visual elements that were highlighted.

Page 18: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 18

Lastly, for each picture separate paired-sample t-tests were conducted to compare

looking behavior at the humanoid visual elements that were highlighted by the text and the

ones that were not. In picture 1, there was a significant difference in fixation at highlighted

humanoid (M = 1.49, SD = .41) and not highlighted humanoid figures: (M = .74, SD = .27);

t(13) = 4.53, p = .001, d = 2.16. The same applied in picture 2; there was a significant

difference in visual attention to highlighted (M = 1.20, SD = .31) and not highlighted

humanoids (M = .81, SD = .40); t(13) = 2.28, p = .04, d = 1.09. Also in picture 3, there was a

significant difference in the scores for highlighted (M = 1.45, SD = .47) and not highlighted

humanoid figures (M = .32, SD = .26); t(13) = 6.42, p < .001, d = 2.98. These results suggest

that humanoid elements that were highlighted by the text attracted more attention than the

ones that were not highlighted.

Discussion

In the present study it was found that children’s visual attention when listening to a

picture storybook was most attracted to humanoid elements of the illustrations that were

highlighted in the oral narration. The present study converges with the body of empirical

work showing that children’s attention is focused on visual elements that are highlighted by

the text, suggesting that they integrate the oral text of the story with the illustrations (Evans &

Saint-Aubin, 2005; Verhallen & Bus, 2011).

For the first research question it was assumed that the humanoid visual elements

attract children’s attention more than non-humanoid figures, as previous findings indicate

(Buswell, 1935; van der Geest et al., 2002; Fletcher-Watson et al., 2008; Tatler et al., 2010;

Verhallen & Bus, 2011; Yarbus, 1967). As it was hypothesized, preschoolers showed in all

three pictures a strong preference for the humanoid elements of the illustrations reflected by

Page 19: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 19

the large differences in fixation time at these elements compared to the time they spent

looking at non-living objects.

For the second research question it was hypothesized that preschoolers would fixate

longer on the visual elements of the illustrations that were highlighted by the oral text of the

storybook compared to the ones that were not. This type of looking behavior was found for

the first and the third picture of the study, indicating that children integrated the oral text of

the story with the accompanying illustrations. This replicates the results of previous studies

(Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; Verhallen & Bus, 2011). Children focused more on the details

of the pictures that depict the story they were listening to as compared to other parts of the

illustrations that were irrelevant to the story, which implies that children follow the story line

on the illustration, and thus, integrate the visual and the verbal information. This finding also

confirms the dual coding theory (Paivio, 1986; Sadoski & Paivio, 2004; Sadoski, 2005)

proposing two separate cognitive structures for processing verbal and nonverbal information

that are interconnected.

On the other hand, in picture 2, results showed the opposite pattern: children’s

attention was attracted more by the visual elements that were not highlighted by the text. A

possible explanation for that could be the fact that there were many humanoid elements not

highlighted by the text on picture 2 that possibly worked as distractors.

Since both questions of whether elements are humanoid or non-living and whether

they are highlighted by the oral narration were found to have an effect on looking behavior,

children’s attention to humanoid elements that were highlighted and the humanoid figures

that were not highlighted were compared in order to test the effect of the narration while

controlling for the effects of humanoid and non-humanoid figures. Results indicated that in

all three pictures humanoid elements that were highlighted by the text had longer fixations

compared to the humanoids that were not highlighted by the text. Thus, this stricter

Page 20: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 20

comparison suggests that children integrated the oral text of the story and the illustrations that

were accompanying it, by focusing on the elements that were mentioned by the oral text. In

the case of picture 2, a plausible explanation for the finding that children focused more on the

visual elements that were not highlighted by the oral narration is that they were more

attracted to objects that were not highlighted by the text. On further inspection it was found

that children paid the most attention to the lamp that Jaap, the main character of the story,

was sitting on. Apparently children were more curious about the surprising element that a

child is sitting on a lamp, rather than to follow the story of this specific picture.

In summary, the findings of the present study are in line withthe dual coding theory

(Paivio, 1986; Sadoski & Paivio, 2004; Sadoski, 2005) since children’s comprehension was

dependent on both verbal (oral text) and the nonverbal information (images). These results

are in line with previous findings, implying that illustrations are helpful during storybook

reading because they can supply referential cues for the text and help preschoolers to

comprehend the story better (Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; Verhallen & Bus, 2011).

It has to be noted that the present study has some limitations. First of all, the size of

the sample was small, and as such the results can only be interpreted cautiously. It would be

interesting in future research to use a larger sample, taking into account other variables, such

as the gender of the child and the gender of the characters in the illustrations. Gender might

affect looking behavior since boys might look more often to male characters than female ones

and vice versa.

Secondly, the amount of information that eye tracking data alone can provide is

limited. It does not measure, for instance, whether the participants have comprehended the

story and how much they received from the text (Hyona, 2010). It is suggested to combine

eye-tracking data with comprehension and vocabulary tests in order to have a clear view of

what the eye movement behavior shows regarding learning (Hyona, 2010). With

Page 21: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 21

comprehension and vocabulary tests researchers would be able to make conclusions

regarding the level of understanding and word learning of the participants. Previous findings

suggest that there is a strong relationship between fixation behavior when listening to the

story and word learning since children are more likely to learn new words when they focus

more on the detail in the illustration that depicts the word (Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005).

In the present study, the conclusions were based on three pictures taken from the same

storybook. This fact can be considered as a limitation since three detailed pictures are neither

representative of all the pictures of this storybook nor of the illustrations in storybooks in

general.

Another limitation is the fact that the areas of interest in the pictures were not the

same size and had different positions as well; the highlighted humanoid elements tended to be

in the center of the picture, while non-humanoid elements that were not mentioned in the text

were more in the background of the illustration and tended to be smaller. As a consequence

the finding that preschoolers focused longer on humanoid elements that are highlighted in the

text as compared to non-humanoid elements that were not mentioned in the text might be due

to the central position of those elements in the illustration and their size (Buswell, 1935). In

picture 2, the focus of the preschoolers was mostly on the humanoid elements not mentioned

by the text, but at the same time they were in the center of the picture but also greater in size,

indicating that the position of the elements might be another important factor to be considered

when studying children’s attention to the illustrations.

The children were exposed to the picture storybook three times. Repeated exposure to

the same storybook increases story familiarity and consequently might change the attention

given to particular elements in the pictures by the children (Evans et al., 2008). It would be

interesting to test if there is any difference in the looking behavior between the different

sessions using the same storybook. A possible outcome would be that after the first session

Page 22: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 22

children could focus on objects in the background that are not highlighted in the narration,

since they have already inspected the humanoid and highlighted features of the illustration.

Future research could also explore whether children tend to look more at anthropomorphic

figures even if they are not text relevant as compared to nonliving elements that are

highlighted by the text. A possible outcome could be that they would look at both categories,

since humanoid visual elements attract children’s attention more than non-humanoid figures

in general, (Buswell, 1935; van der Geest et al., 2002; Fletcher-Watson et al., 2008; Tatler et

al., 2010; Verhallen & Bus, 2011; Yarbus, 1967) but they would spend more time looking at

the elements highlighted by the text compared to figures that are not text relevant, as was

found in the present study.

In summary, it was found that children’s attention to visual elements was increased

when they were highlighted by the text, suggesting that they integrated the oral text of the

story (Evans & Saint-Aubin, 2005; Verhallen & Bus, 2011). These results imply that

illustrations are helpful during storybook especially if they closely depict the oral text, in

order to facilitate the activation of both verbal and nonverbal cognitive systems of processing

information (Paivio, 1986; Sadoski & Paivio, 2004; Sadoski, 2005) Additionally, a general

tendency to focus on humanoid elements was found. The findings of the present study could

be used as possible guidelines to book publishers and illustrators of storybooks. Illustrators of

storybooks could take advantage of children’s preference for humanoid elements and the fact

that they tend to integrate illustrations with the oral text. By closely illustrating the most

important key concepts of the story with humanoid figures children are guided to focus on the

main points of the story in the illustrations and to connect them with the language of the story.

Page 23: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 23

References

Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (2001). Text talk: Capturing the benefits of read aloud

experiences for young children. The Reading Teacher, 55, 10-20.

Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for

success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of

literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65, 1-21.

doi:10.3102/00346543065001001

Buswell, G. T. (1935). How people look at pictures: A study of the psychology of perception

in art. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Chun, D. M., & Plaas, J. L. (1996). Facilitating reading comprehension with multimedia.

System, 24, 503-519. doi:10.1016/S0346-251X(96)00038-3

de Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2002). Quality of book-reading matters for emergent readers:

An experiment with the same book in a regular or electronic format. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 94, 145-155. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.94.1.145

Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control:

Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to ‘‘do as I say, not as I

do’’. Developmental Psychobiology, 29, 315–334. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-

2302(199605)29:4<315::AID-DEV2>3.0.CO;2-T

Evans, M. A., & Saint-Aubin J. (2013). Vocabulary acquisition without adult explanations in

repeated shared book reading: An eye movement study. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 105, 596–608. doi:10.1037/a0032465

Evans, M. A., & Saint-Aubin J. (2005). What children are looking at during shared storybook

reading. Psychological Science, 16, 913-920. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01636.x

Evans, M. A., Williamson K., & Pursoo T. (2008). Preschoolers' attention to print during

shared book reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 106-129.

Page 24: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 24

doi:10.1080/10888430701773884

Fang, Z. (1996). Illustrations, text, and the child reader. What are pictures in children’s

storybooks for? Reading Horizons, 37, 130-142.

Fletcher-Watson, S., Findlay, J. M., Leekam, S. R., & Benson, V. (2008). Rapid detection of

person information in a naturalistic scene. Perception, 37, 571-583.

doi:10.1068/p5705

Greenhoot, A. F., & Semb, P. A. (2008). Do illustrations enhance preschoolers’ memories for

stories? Age-related change in the picture facilitation effect. Journal of Experimental

Child Psychology, 99, 271–287. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2007.06.005

Guttmann, J., Levin, J. R., & Pressley, M. (1977). Pictures, partial pictures, and young

children's oral prose learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 473-480.

doi:10.1037/0022-0663.69.5.473

Hyönä, J. (2010). The use of eye movements in the study of multimedia learning. Learning

and Instruction, 20, 172-176. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.013

Justice, L. M., Pullen, P. C., & Pence, K. (2008). Influence of verbal and nonverbal

references to print on preschoolers’ visual attention to print during storybook

reading. Developmental Psychology, 44, 855-866. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.855

Justice, L. M., Skibbe, L., Canning, A., & Lankford, C. (2005). Pre-schoolers, print and

storybooks: An observational study using eye movement analysis. Journal of

Research in Reading, 28, 229–243. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2005.00267.x

Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1983). Kaufman assessment battery for children.

Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessments.

Levin, J. R., & Mayer, R. E. (1993). Understanding illustrations in text. In B. K. Britton, A.

Woodward, & M. Brinkley (Eds.), Learning from textbooks (pp. 95–113).

Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Page 25: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 25

Mayer, R. E., & Anderson, R. B. (1992). The instructive animation: helping students build

connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 84, 444-452. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.444

Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations. A dual coding approach. Oxford, UK: Oxford

University Press.

Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Jewkes, A. M., Connor, C. M., Farris, C. L., & Morrison,

F. J. (2008). Touch your toes! Developing a direct measure of behavioral

regulation in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 141–158.

doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.01.004.

Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of

research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372-422. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372

Sadoski, M. (2005). A dual coding view of vocabulary learning. Reading & Writing

Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 21, 221-238.

doi:10.1080/10573560590949359

Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2004). A dual coding theoretical model of reading. Theoretical

models and processes of reading (5th ed., pp. 1329-1362). Newark, DE:

International Reading Association.

Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J. A., Hudson, E., & Lawson, E. P. (1996). Knowledge of storybooks

as a predictor of young children's vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology,

88, 520-536. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.88.3.520

Sipe, L. R. (1998). How picture books work: A aemiotically framed theory of text-picture

relationships. Children’s Literature in Education, 29, 97–108.

doi:10.1023/A:1022459009182

Sipe, L. R. (2012). Revisiting the relationships between text and pictures. Children's

Literature in Education, 43, 4-21. doi:10.1007/s10583-011-9153-0

Page 26: Children’s Looking Behavior at Different Visual Elements ... · CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 5 highlighted by the narration, depicting and concretizing the oral

CHILDREN’S LOOKING BEHAVIOR AT VISUAL ELEMENTS 26

Smeets, D. J. H., van Dijken, M. J., & Bus, A. G. (2012). Using electronic storybooks to

support word learning in children with severe language impairments. Journal of

Learning Disabilities,47, 435-449. doi:10.1177/0022219412467069

Tatler, B. W, Wade, N. J., Kwan, H., Findlay J. M., & Velichkovsky, B. M. (2010). Yarbus,

eye movements and vision. i-Perception, 1, 7-27. doi:10.1068/i0382

Tobii Technology. (2011). User Manual: Tobii Technology Tobii T60 T120 Revision 4.

Danderyd, Sweden: Tobii Technology AB.

van der Geest J. N., Kemner C., Camfferman G., Verbaten M. N., & van Engeland H. (2002).

Looking at images with human figures: comparison between autistic and normal

children. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 32, 69-75. doi:0162-

3257/02/0400-0069/0

van Genechten, G. (2005). Kleine Kangoeroe [Little Kangaroo]. Clavis Publishing: Hasselt-

Amsterdam, Netherlands.

van Haeringen, A. (2004). Beer is op Vlinder [Bear is in love with the butterfly]. Leopold:

Amsterdam, Netherlands.

Veldkamp, T. (2006). Na-apers! [Imitators]. Lannoo: Netherlands.

Verhallen, M. J. A. J., & Bus A. G. (2011). Young second language learners' visual attention

to illustrations in storybooks. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 11, 480–500.

doi:10.1177/1468798411416785

Verhallen, M. J. A. J., Bus, A. G., & de Jong, M. T. (2006). The promise of multimedia

stories for kindergarten children at risk. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98,

410-419. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.410

Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye movements and vision. New York: Plenum Press.