Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

download Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

of 12

Transcript of Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    1/12

    Childhood Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury:Findings from a National Cohort Study

    Richard Rowe,1 PHD, Barbara Maughan,1 PHD, and Robert Goodman,2 PhD1

    MRC Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Center, Institute of Psychiatry,Kings College London, UK, and 2Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

    Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College London, UK

    Objective We set out to examine the relationship between unintentional injury and

    common child psychiatric disorders, including both internalizing and externalizing diagnoses.

    Methods The 1999 British Child and Adolescent Mental Health Survey provided a nationally

    representative sample of over 10,000 children aged 515 years. Measures included assessment of

    diagnoses of psychiatric disorder from theDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,

    fourth edition, unintentional injury, and a range of potentially related psychosocial

    factors. Results Children with psychiatric disorders had higher rates of unintentional

    injury. After accounting for psychosocial risk factors and comorbid psychopathology,

    oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) was independently associated with burns and poisoning.

    Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) was related to fractures, and depression and

    anxiety also showed independent links to some injury types. Conclusions ODD and

    ADHD, rather than conduct disorder, appear to be the externalizing disorders associated with

    unintentional injury. We discuss possible models of the relationships between internalizing

    disorders and unintentional injury.

    Key words accident; psychopathology; children; adolescents.

    Unintentional injury is a major cause of childhood

    suffering and mortality. For children aged 514 years in

    the United Kingdom, 29% of deaths in boys (41.7 deaths

    per million) and 19% of deaths in girls (21.4 deaths per

    million) were caused by injuries in 1999 (National Statis-

    tics, 2001). Epidemiological studies of the relationship

    between child psychopathology and unintentional injury

    have been conducted using several large-scale datasets,

    with particular attention paid to associations with anti-

    social behavior and hyperactivity. There is much less evi-

    dence regarding possible links with other common types

    of psychopathology, such as anxiety and depression.

    There is some agreement that antisocial behavior isrelated to unintentional injury (Bijur, Golding, Haslum,

    & Kurzon, 1988; Bijur, Stewart-Brown, & Butler, 1986;

    Davidson, Taylor, Sandberg, & Thorley, 1992; Langley,

    McGee, Silva, & Williams, 1983). Although the Diag-

    nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth

    edition (DSM-IV) (APA, 1994), mentions increased

    accident liability as a characteristic of conduct disorder

    (CD), a case-control study has shown that unintentional

    injury is also increased in children with oppositional

    defiant disorder (ODD) (Schwebel, Speltz, Jones &

    Bardina, 2002). The DSM-IV also mentions physical

    injury as an associated feature of attention deficit hyper-

    activity disorder (ADHD), but the empirical evidence

    regarding hyperactivity and related behaviors has been

    mixed; a relationship has been reported in some studies

    (Bijur et al., 1986; Langley et al., 1983) but not others.

    Davidson et al. (1992) found no evidence of a simplerelationship between hyperactivity and later uninten-

    tional injury in boys, while Bijur et al. (1988) found that

    there was a simple relationship between overactivity and

    later unintentional injury, but that this was not indepen-

    All correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dr Richard Rowe, MRC Social, Ge netic,and Developmental Psychiatry Research Center, Box P046, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park,Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK. E-mail: [email protected].

    Journal of Pediatric Psychology29(2) pp. 119130, 2004DOI: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsh015

    Journal of Pediatric Psychologyvol.29 no. 2 Q Society of Pediatric Psychology2004; all rights reserved

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    2/12

    dent of the relationship between unintentional injury

    and antisocial behavior. In general, the association has

    been reported to be stronger in cross-sectional studies

    than in prospective designs. Davidson et al. (1992) noted

    that this may suggest that cross-sectional findings have

    been influenced by biases in retrospective injury

    reporting. A recent study has suggested that ADHDmay be related to unintentional injury in a design free

    from reporting biases. Analysis of population health

    records from British Columbia have shown that children

    who had been prescribed methylphenidate were at

    increased risk of sustaining a variety of injuries (Brehaut,

    Miller, Raina, & McGrail, 2003).

    Apart from psychopathology, a number of other

    demographic, family, and environmental risk factors for

    unintentional injury have been demonstrated. For ex-

    ample, unintentional injury has consistently been found

    to be more common in boys than girls. Other risk factors

    identified for unintentional injury include nontraditionalfamilystructureandparentalhistoryofteenagepregnancy

    (OConnor, Davies, Dunn, & Golding, 2000). Because

    such risk factors are also well-documented correlates of

    psychopathology (e.g., Rutter, Giller, & Hagell, 1998), it

    is important to take these confoundsinto account. Bijur et

    al. (1986) addressed this issue, including in their analyses

    a range of family and environmental correlates. They

    found that the relationships between unintentional injury

    and psychopathology were reduced after control for

    environmental confounds but that they remained sub-

    stantial. Viewed from the alternative perspective, it is also

    possible that demographic and psychosocial associations

    with unintentional injury are mediated by psychopathol-

    ogy. For example, the preponderance of antisocial

    behavior in boys may contribute to the gender difference

    in rate of injury.

    This study examines the cross-sectional relation-

    ships between childhood injury and psychiatric disorder

    in a large-scale nationally representative sample of British

    children aged 515 years as collected in the 1999 British

    Child and Adolescent Mental Health Survey (BCAMHS-

    99). Psychiatric disorder was assessed using the Devel-

    opment and Well-Being Assessment interview (DAWBA)

    (Goodman, Ford, Richards, Gatward, & Meltzer, 2000),

    which generates clinically assigned DSM-IV diagnoses.A range of psychosocial risk factors was also assessed dur-

    ing the interview.

    MethodsPopulation

    Full details of the survey design and measures are avail-

    able elsewhere (Meltzer, Gatward, Goodman, & Ford,

    2000). Briefly, the Child Benefit Register was used as

    a sampling frame to identify children aged 515 years in

    England, Scotland, and Wales. The Register should pro-

    vide a relatively complete basis for sampling the child

    population because the Child Benefit program is avail-

    able to all families with children in the UK. This frame

    was stratified by Regional Health Authority, and withinthat by sociodemographic groupings. Information from

    up to three informants was collected on 10,438 children.

    Ethical approval for the BCAMHS-99 was obtained from

    the appropriate multicenter research ethics committee in

    the United Kingdom. Informed consent was obtained

    from both child and parent. Signed parental consent was

    also required before the teacher was invited to provide

    information on the index child.

    Measures

    Demographic data (including ethnicity, family type, and

    parental employment status/socioeconomic group) were

    collected in the parent interview. Psychiatric disorder

    was assessed using the DAWBA (Goodman et al., 2000),

    which consists of a parental interview (available for

    99.7% of cooperating families), a child interview for

    children aged 11 years and over (available for 95.3% of

    age-appropriate children), and a teacher questionnaire

    (available for 80.3% of children). Data on symptoms and

    related psychosocial impairment were probed through

    a structured, computer-assisted interview. In addition,

    interviewers recorded participants verbatim descrip-

    tions of symptomatic behaviors in response to open-ended questions. Preliminary DSM-IV diagnoses were

    made by computer algorithm on the basis of symptom

    and impairment data, using evidence from each available

    informant. Experienced clinicians then reviewed a com-

    puter-generated case summary including both the

    symptom and impairment data and the verbatim tran-

    scripts and either accepted or amended the computer-

    generated diagnoses. A reliability study (see Heyman et

    al., 2001) in which two clinicians rated disorder in

    500 children showed that the chance-corrected reliability

    for the presence of any diagnosis was .86 (95% CI 5

    .78 to .95).The DAWBA assessed all the information required

    to make DSM-IV diagnoses of a range of psychiatric

    disorders, including duration criteria and measures of

    functional impairment. In our analyses, we diagnosed

    ODD independently of CD, rather than applying the

    DSM-IV rule excluding ODD diagnoses for children with

    CD. Information on unintentional injury was also

    collected during the parental interview. Parents were

    120 Rowe, Maughan, and Goodman

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    3/12

    asked whether their child had ever suffered a head injury

    with loss of consciousness, a broken bone other than

    head injury (hereafter referred to as fractures), a burn

    requiring hospital admission, or a poisoning requiring

    hospital admission. Where injuries were reported, the

    parent was asked to categorize the timing of the incident

    as either less than 1 month ago, at least 1 month ago butless than 6 months ago, at least 6 months ago but less

    than a year ago, or more than a year ago. Severe injuries

    of this kind were relatively uncommon, so we analyzed

    binary variables indicating whether each type of injury

    had taken place at any prior time unless otherwise stated.

    A range of child and family/environmental factors,

    which are known to correlate with psychiatric disorder

    (Meltzer et al., 2000), were also collected during the

    assessment procedures. Many of these factors are also

    potential correlates of unintentional injury. In order to

    account for these factors in our analyses, we formed

    a number of binary indicators of risk. The socioeco-

    nomic characteristics of the neighborhood were assessed

    using the ACORN [A Classification Of Residential

    Neighbourhoods] scheme (see Meltzer et al., 2000).

    We formed a binary variable to index whether or not

    children lived in a striving neighborhood (the most

    disadvantaged of the broad categorizations made by

    ACORN), which identified 25% of the sample. Ethnic

    origin was coded as a binary variable to indicate majority

    (white, 91%) or minority status (black, Indian, Pakistani,

    or other, 9%), reflecting the ethnic mix of the child

    population of Great Britain at the time of the study.

    Family structure was categorized as traditional (66%),

    single parent(22%) orstepparent(11%). Binary variables

    were coded to indicate large family (four or more

    children; 11%), low income (household income less than

    200 [approx $300] per week; 24%), and low social class

    (22%). Using the Standard Occupation Classification

    (OPCS, 1991), low social class (22%) was coded if the

    highest classification of the occupation of both parents

    was social class IV (partly skilled) or V (unskilled) or if

    neither parent had ever worked. Family functioning was

    assessed using the General Functioning Scale of the

    McMaster Family Assessment Device (Epstein, Baldwin,& Bishop, 1983). A binary variable was coded to index

    the most unhealthy functioning 19% of families. A binary

    variable was also formed to identify parents who used

    harsh physical discipline as indexed by the use of an

    implement with which to hit their child (2%).

    A number of measures of the characteristics of the

    interviewed parent (usually the mother) were also

    assessed. Current symptoms of anxiety and depression

    were assessed using the 12-item version of Goldberg and

    Williams (1988) General Health Questionnaire (GHQ)

    which has been the subject of a number of validation

    studies. For example, it was shown to have good

    sensitivity (76.3%) and specificity (83.4%) in identifying

    psychiatric diagnoses in a validation study conducted

    across 15 clinical settings located around the world(Goldberg et al., 1997). We formed a binary variable to

    identify the most symptomatic 25% of parents on the

    GHQ which corresponded to the positive endorsement

    of three items. Education level was also assessed and we

    coded a binary variable to identify parents who had not

    passed any public examinations usually taken on leaving

    school at age 16 (26%). The parents age at the index

    childs birth was also recorded and we coded a binary

    variable to index teenage parenthood (6%).

    All children were invited to participate in an

    assessment of intellectual ability. Of particular interest

    here was global cognitive ability, which was measured

    using the British Picture Vocabulary Scale (Dunn, Dunn,

    Whetton, & Burley, 1997), and reading ability, which

    was measured using the Word Reading Scale of the

    British Ability Scale, second edition (BASII) (Elliot,

    Smith, & McCulloch, 1996), on an age-standardized

    scale. For both measures we formed binary variables to

    identify children who performed in the lowest quartile of

    the distribution. IQ data were available for 9,884

    children (94.7%), and reading data were available for

    9,337 (89.4%).

    Data Analyses

    The survey models of STATA 7 (StataCorp, 2001) were

    used for all analyses. These models allowed for the use of

    weights to account for the unequal probability of postal

    sector selection and to correct for nonresponse bias

    associated with region, age, and gender (see Meltzer et

    al., 2000). The models also accounted for the clustering

    of observations taken from the same postal sector and for

    stratification by Regional Health Authority and socio-

    economic group.

    We analyzed each type of injury separately, and the

    first step in our strategy was to identify significantunivariate predictors. We employed logistic regression

    models for this purpose, which calculate odds ratios

    (ORs) as a measure of association. With a binary

    predictor, an OR may be interpreted as the difference

    between the odds of having suffered the particular injury

    by people who have and have not endorsed the predictor

    variable. For example, if the OR for ADHD predicting

    ever having had a burn is 2.3, this shows that the odds of

    Psychopathology and Injury 121

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    4/12

    having ever been burned are 2.3-fold greater for children

    with ADHD than for children who do not have ADHD.

    Ninety-five percent CIs are also provided with all ORs,

    and these show the range of values which are consistent

    with the data. Where the CIs do not include 1, effects are

    significant at the p , .05 level, and we used this

    conventional significance level to guide variable selec-

    tion throughout. Within the psychiatric and psychoso-

    cial domains, we built joint predictor models from

    significant univariate predictors. Significant predictors

    in these joint models are shown to have effects that are

    independent of the other predictors in the model.

    Nonsignificant effects in these joint models imply that

    the significant univariate relationships were observed

    only because of relationships with other predictorsincluded in the model. In the case of the psychiatric

    models, such interrelation of predictors is usually re-

    ferred to as comorbidity. Significant predictors in these

    psychiatric and psychosocial models were then com-

    bined into a final model of each injury type. We

    confirmed that the psychiatric correlates of unintentional

    injury identified in this way did not differ from models in

    which all predictors were entered simultaneously (de-

    tails available on request). In view of the wide de-

    velopmental range included in the study, we checked

    whether the correlates included in the final models

    significantly differed in subgroups of children aged 11years and younger and 12 years and older.

    Data on all variables of interest in this paper were

    available for 8,337 (80%) of the 10,438 children in-

    cluded in the study. Although data on psychiatric

    diagnoses were available for all observations and missing

    data on unintentional injury were rare (1.2 to 1.3%),

    information on a number of the risk measures was

    unavailable for a slightly larger proportion of cases. In

    addition to child IQ and reading levels (noted above),

    other variables with limited availability included income

    level (6.4%) and teenage parenthood (3.9%). Uninten-tional injury was reported at similar levels in those with

    complete and incomplete observations, but those with

    incomplete data did show elevated rates of CD (1.3% vs.

    2.1%; OR 5 1.7; 95% CI 5 1.22.4;p 5 .004), anxiety

    (3.5% vs. 4.8%; OR 5 1.4; 95% CI 5 1.11.7,p 5 .003),

    and ADHD (2.1% vs. 2.8%; OR 5 1.4; 95% CI 5 1.0

    1.8; p 5 .04). Incomplete observations were omitted

    from all analyses. This approach should not lead to

    substantial bias of the results, although the power to

    detect relationships between unintentional injury and

    psychopathology may be reduced.

    ResultsInjury Prevalence and Age/Sex Trends

    Twenty percent of girls and 24% of boys had suffered at

    least one unintentional injury during their lifetime.

    Fractures were most common (15.8%), followed by head

    injury (4.3%), burns (2.3%), and poisoning (2.1%). Age

    trends in unintentional injury at any time prior to

    interview are shown in Figures 1 (boys) and 2 (girls).

    As shown in Table I, boys were at greater risk for all

    types of injury. Across gender, rate of injury increasedwith age, although not significantly so regarding poison-

    ing. The overall age trend was also nonsignificant for

    burns, but there was a hint of interaction between age

    and sex (p 5 .053) in that the age trend was flat for boys

    (OR [for a 5-year increase in age] 5 0.9; 95% CI 5 0.7

    1.2; p 5 .6) but increased significantly in girls (OR 5

    1.5; 95% CI 5 1.02.1; p 5 .04). The increase in frac-

    tures across age was significantly steeper for boys than

    Figure 1 The rate of ever having suffered each injury across age in boys.Figure 2 The rate of ever having suffered each injury across age in girls.

    122 Rowe, Maughan, and Goodman

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    5/12

    for girls, and the age trends in poisoning and head injury

    were similar across gender.

    Burns

    Table II shows that burns were more common among

    children with all psychiatric diagnoses than in thosewithout disorder but that these contrasts reached signif-

    icance for only ADHD, ODD, and anxiety. Only ODD

    remained significant when these three disorders were

    modeled jointly. A number of psychosocial factors pre-

    dicted burns in simple models, but the multiple pre-

    dictor modeling indicated that only poor reading ability

    and stepfamily status were independently significant.

    In the final model, only ODD and poor reading abil-

    ity retained significant links with increased risk for

    burns.

    PoisoningAs shown in Table III, poisoning was significantly more

    common in children with all types of psychiatric

    disorder. The effects of only ODD and anxiety remained

    significant when all disorders were considered jointly,

    however. Of the psychosocial risk factors, poor child

    reading, large family size, and high parental GHQ score

    had effects that were independent from the other psy-

    chosocial measures. In the final model, ODD, anxiety,

    poor reading, and large family size had significant

    independent effects.

    Head Injury

    Rates of head injury were elevated in all types of psy-

    chiatric disorder except depression (Table IV), but only

    anxiety remained significant in the multiple psychiatric

    predictor model; ADHD was of borderline significance

    (p 5 .055). Of the psychosocial factors, high parental

    GHQ score and poor child reading were independently

    significant risk factors, while rates of head injury were

    lower in children from ethnic minorities. We explored

    this unexpected finding in more detail and found that

    white children had similar rates of head injury (4.4%) as

    Pakistani children (6.5%; p 5.3) but higher rates than

    blacks (1.7%; p 5 .08) and Indians (0.8%; p 5 .06).

    Anxiety remained significant in the final model, as didthe psychosocial predictors. Additionally, we found

    some evidence of interaction between age and high

    parental GHQ score (p 5 .057). There was a strong

    association with head injury in children aged 11 and

    younger (OR 5 1.9; 95% CI 5 1.42.5; p , .001) but

    not in children aged 12 and over (OR 5 1.3; 95% CI 5

    0.91.8;p 5 .2).

    Fractures

    Of the psychiatric disorders, only ADHD and depression

    were significantly related to fractures, and these relation-ships were independent of each other. Ethnic minority

    status was once again a significant protective factor;

    white children (16.4%) had a similar rate of fractures to

    children of Pakistani origin (11.2%; p 5 .2) but signif-

    icantly higher rates than blacks (8.8%; p 5 .01) and

    Indians (5.7%;p 5 .002). None of the other psychosocial

    risk factors was significant. ADHD, depression, and

    ethnic minority status remained significant predictors in

    the final model (see Table V).

    The higher frequency of fractures relative to other

    injury types meant that fractures during the most recent

    12 months (3.0%) could be analyzed. We investigatedwhether recall bias was involved in the relationship

    identified between ADHD and fracture. We found that

    ADHD was related to more recent fractures (OR 5 1.9;

    95% CI 5 0.93.7;p 5 .07) in a very similar way to less

    recent fractures (OR 5 1.6; 95% CI 5 1.12.5; p 5

    .017); the comparison between these coefficients did not

    approach significance (OR 5 1.1; 95% CI 5 0.52.5;

    p 5 .8).

    Table I. Age and Gender Effects in Unintentional Injury at Any Time Prior to Interview

    Gendera Age 3 Gender

    Injury Type Rate in Boys Rate in Girls OR (95% CI) ORb (95% CI) Boysc Ageb Trend Girlsc Ageb Trend

    Burn 2.8 1.8 1.5 (1.12.0)** 0.6 (0.41.0) 1.1 (.91.4)

    Poisoning 2.5 1.8 1.4 (1.01.9)* 1.0 (0.61.6) 1.2 (.91.5)

    Head injury 5.3 3.3 1.6 (1.32.0)*** 1.1 (0.81.6) 1.6 (1.41.9)***

    Fracture 17.2 14.5 1.2 (1.11.4)**

    1.5 (1.21.8)***

    2.7 (2.33.1)***

    1.8 (1.62.1)***

    OR5 odds ratio; coefficients in boldface indicate p , .05.a Gender tested in single predictor model.b OR given for a 5-year increase in age.c Single age coefficient given when Age 3Gender interaction was not significant.

    *p , .05; **p , .01; ***p , .001.

    Psychopathology and Injury 123

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    6/12

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    7/12

    the study measured. Unusually for a study of this scale,

    we were additionally able to assess the relationships

    between anxiety/depression and injury during child-

    hood, a topic which has received much less attention in

    the existing literature.

    Despite the many strengths of this dataset for

    studying the relationship between injury and psychopa-

    thology, at least two weaknesses should be noted. First,

    unintentional injuries were retrospectively reported. It is

    possible that parents forgot injuries, particularly if theyhad taken place in the less recent past, and that other

    retrospective reporting biases (Wazana, 1997) were in-

    volved. More serious injuries are more memorable,

    however (Harel et al., 1994; Peterson, Ewigman, &

    Kivlahan, 1993), so it is likely that the severe nature of

    the injuries studied here has minimized any biases

    introduced in this way. Second, although we were able to

    include a wider range of psychosocial correlates than has

    typically been included in studies of unintentional

    injury, one feature that may be especially salientthe

    quality of parents supervision of the childs activities

    was not directly assessed. It is therefore possible that

    inadequate parental supervision may contribute to some

    of the associations between psychopathology and injury

    reported in this study.

    Results for the four types of injury studied here

    showed an interesting pattern of similarities and differ-

    ences. As expected, all four were more common in boysthan girls, but only head injuries and fractures showed

    the strong age trends that might be expected in measures

    of injury across the lifetime. Poisoning showed no signif-

    icant increase across age, while there was some evidence

    that the frequency of burns increased with age for girls

    but not boys. This suggests that many of the burns and

    poisonings reported here took place very early in

    childhood, before the study entry age of 5 years. While

    Table III. Prediction of Poisoning

    % Poisoned If

    Predictor Absent

    % Poisoned If

    Predictor Present

    Joint ORa

    Predictor Simple ORa Psychiatric Model Psychosocial Model Final Model

    Psychiatric disorder

    ADHD 2.1 5.1 2.3 (1.14.8)* 1.2 (0.52.6)

    Conduct disorder 2.1 6.6 2.9 (1.36.6)** 1.0 (0.42.3)

    ODD 1.9 9.0 4.8 (2.88.0)*** 3.9 (2.17.0)*** 3.4 (2.05.8)***

    Anxiety 2.0 6.1 3.2 (1.95.4)*** 2.3 (1.34.1)*** 2.2 (1.33.7)**

    Depression 2.1 7.2 3.5 (1.39.2)* 1.5 (0.45.4)

    Intellectual functioning

    Low child IQ 1.9 2.8 1.5 (1.02.0)* 1.0 (0.71.4)

    Poor child reading 1.7 3.3 1.9 (1.42.7)*** 1.7 (1.22.4)** 1.6 (1.12.3)*

    Socioeconomic status

    Striving neighborhood 2.1 2.4 1.1 (0.81.6)

    Low income 1.9 2.9 1.6 (1.12.3)* 1.2 (.81.9)

    Low social class 2.0 2.6 1.3 (0.91.9)

    Family characteristics

    Ethnic minority 2.2 1.4 0.6 (0.31.1) Single parent family 2.0 2.8 1.4 (1.02.0)* 1.2 (.81.8)

    Stepparent family 2.0 3.1 1.6 (1.02.4)* 1.5 (1.02.3)

    Large family 2.0 3.6 1.9 (1.32.8)** 1.6 (1.12.5)* 1.6 (1.02.4)*

    Unhealthy family functioning 2.0 2.7 1.3 (0.91.9)

    Harsh physical discipline 2.1 3.1 1.4 (0.53.8)

    Parent characteristics

    Limited educational qualifications 2.0 2.5 1.2 (0.91.7)

    High GHQ score 2.0 2.6 1.3 (1.01.8)

    Teen parenthood 2.1 3.0 1.5 (0.92.4)

    OR5 odds ratio; ADHD 5 attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD 5 oppositional defiant disorder; GHQ 5 General Health Questionnaire.

    Coefficients in boldface indicate p , .05.a Adjusted for age and gender.

    *p , .05; **p , .01; ***p , .001.

    Psychopathology and Injury 125

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    8/12

    recall biases cannot be ruled out as contributing to what

    might seem a surprising result, it is possible that the

    requirement for hospital admission in the definition of

    these injuries is salient here. Burns and poisonings of

    a similar nature may continue to befall children at older

    ages, but hospital admission may more rarely be required

    when they happen at later stages of development. Within

    the sample there was very little evidence that the

    correlates of injury involvement differed for older andyounger children. The only hint we found was that high

    parental GHQ score was more strongly associated with

    head injury in younger children (11 years and younger)

    than older children, although this effect fell below the

    conventional level of significance. The four types of

    injury also showed interesting variations in the pattern

    of associated psychiatric disorder and psychosocial risk

    factors, as discussed below.

    Previous studies have consistently reported a strong

    relationship between unintentional injury and antisocial

    behavior in childhood (Bijur et al., 1986; Langley et al.,

    1983; Schwebel et al., 2002). This was confirmed in our

    analyses, and we found that ODD was more strongly

    related than CD. CD showed simple relationships with

    poisoning and head injury, but these were not in-

    dependent of the other psychiatric disorders measured.

    By contrast, ODD was independently related to bothpoisoning and burns. As discussed above, it is likely that

    many of the burns and poisonings studied here took

    place prior to age 5. The DAWBA addresses symptom-

    atology in the present and recent past, but given the

    typically early onset of many ODD symptoms (Angold &

    Costello, 1996), it is plausible that substantial ODD

    behaviors were evident in many of the diagnosed

    children at much younger ages. One possible mechanism

    Table IV. Prediction of Head Injury

    % Injured If

    Predictor Absent

    % Injured If

    Predictor Present

    Joint ORa

    Predictor Simple ORa Psychiatric Model Psychosocial Model Final Model

    Psychiatric disorder

    ADHD 4.2 9.4 2.1 (1.23.5)** 1.8 (1.03.3)

    Conduct disorder 4.2 11.6 2.2 (1.24.2)* 1.8 (0.83.8)

    ODD 4.2 7.6 1.7 (1.02.8)* 1.1 (0.52.1)

    Anxiety 4.1 7.9 1.9 (1.22.9)** 1.8 (1.22.8)** 1.7 (1.12.6)*

    Depression 4.2 8.4 1.6 (0.73.8)

    Intellectual functioning

    Low child IQ 4.4 3.9 0.9 (0.71.1)

    Poor child reading 4.0 5.1 1.3 (1.01.7)* 1.2 (1.01.6)

    Socioeconomic status

    Striving neighborhood 4.1 4.8 1.2 (0.91.5)

    Low income 4.2 4.7 1.2 (0.91.5)

    Low social class 4.3 4.3 1.0 (0.81.3)

    Family characteristics

    Ethnic minority 4.4 2.4 0.5 (0.30.9)*

    0.5 (0.30.8)**

    0.5 (0.30.9)*

    Single parent family 4.1 4.9 1.2 (0.91.6)

    Stepparent family 4.2 4.6 1.1 (0.31.5)

    Large family 4.2 4.6 1.2 (0.81.6)

    Unhealthy family functioning 4.0 5.7 1.4 (1.11.8)** 1.3 (1.01.7)

    Harsh physical discipline 4.3 1.6 0.3 (0.11.4)

    Parent characteristics

    Limited educational qualifications 4.1 4.9 1.2 (0.91.5)

    High GHQ score 3.7 6.0 1.6 (1.32.1)*** 1.5 (1.22.0)** 1.6 (1.22.0)***

    Teen parenthood 4.3 4.5 1.1 (0.71.6)

    OR5 odds ratio; ADHD 5 attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD5 oppositional defiant disorder; GHQ 5 General Health Questionnaire.

    Coefficients in boldface indicatep , .05.a Adjusted for age and gender.

    *p , .05; **p , .01; ***p , .001.

    Bold coefficients indicatep , .05.

    126 Rowe, Maughan, and Goodman

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    9/12

    for the relationship with unintentional injury may be

    through the problematic parent-child relationships that

    characterize oppositionality. For example, children with

    ODD may not follow the safety instructions of supervis-

    ing adults, putting them at greater risk for injury. This

    finding is consistent with the growing evidence that

    ODD may have important negative consequences that

    are independent of its relationships with CD (Greeneet al., 2002; Maughan et al., 2004; Pickles et al., 2001). It

    is possible that the relationship between CD and

    unintentional injury noted in the DSM-IV might be at

    least partly due to the substantial comorbidity of CD and

    ODD.

    The picture regarding ADHD in the previous

    literature was less clear. We found that although ADHD

    had simple relationships with all types of injury

    measured in this study, this effect was independent of

    other psychiatric disorders only for fractures. An

    independent relationship with head injury fell just

    short of significance. As noted in the introduction, it

    has been suggested that biases in retrospective assess-

    ment of injuries may spuriously inflate the relationship

    between hyperactivity and unintentional injury in

    cross-sectional studies (Davidson et al., 1992). Injurieswere reported retrospectively in this study, as discussed

    above. However, substantial bias due to differential

    rates of forgetting by parents of children with and

    without ADHD seems unlikely here. ADHD was related

    similarly to fractures reported to have occurred during

    the last 12 months and the less recent past. Given the

    recovery time required for fractures, many of the

    children injured within the last 12 months would still

    Table V. Prediction of Fractures

    % Injured If

    Predictor Absent

    % Injured If

    Predictor Present

    Joint ORa

    Predictor Simple ORa Psychiatric Model Final Model

    Psychiatric disorder

    ADHD 15.7 24.7 1.7 (1.22.4)** 1.7 (1.22.4)** 1.6 (1.22.3)**

    Conduct disorder 15.8 18.5 0.8 (0.51.3)

    ODD 15.8 18.6 1.1 (0.81.6)

    Anxiety 15.8 16.1 0.9 (0.71.3)

    Depression 15.7 35.3 1.9 (1.13.3)* 1.9 (1.13.3)* 1.9 (1.13.3)*

    Intellectual functioning

    Low child IQ 15.3 17.6 1.1 (1.01.3)

    Poor child reading 15.7 16.3 1.1 (0.91.2)

    Socioeconomic status

    Striving neighborhood 15.7 16.3 1.1 (0.91.2)

    Low income 15.8 16.1 1.1 (1.01.3)

    Low social class 15.7 16.6 1.1 (1.01.3)

    Family characteristics

    Ethnic minority 16.4 8.8 0.5 (0.40.6)***

    0.5 (0.40.6)***

    Single parent family 15.7 16.5 1.1 (0.91.2)

    Stepparent family 15.6 18.3 1.2 (1.01.4)

    Large family 16.1 13.9 0.9 (0.81.1)

    Unhealthy family functioning 15.6 16.8 1.0 (0.91.2)

    Harsh physical discipline 15.9 14.1 0.8 (0.51.4)

    Parent characteristics

    Limited educational qualifications 15.3 17.6 1.1 (0.91.2)

    High GHQ score 15.3 17.4 1.1 (1.01.3)

    Teen parenthood 15.8 16.9 1.1 (0.91.4)

    OR5 odds ratio; ADHD 5 attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD5 oppositional defiant disorder; GHQ 5 General Health Questionnaire.

    Coefficients in boldface indicate p , .05.a Adjusted for age and gender.

    *p , .05; **p , .01; ***p , .001.

    Bold coefficients indicatep , .05.

    Psychopathology and Injury 127

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    10/12

    have been suffering related difficulties or would only

    recently have returned to full health at the time of the

    interview, making it unlikely that recall of these recent

    fractures would have been compromised in any

    important ways.

    Associations between unintentional injury and in-

    ternalizing disorder have received little attention in

    previous studies. We found that depression was in-

    dependently related to fractures, while anxiety was

    independently related to poisoning and was the only

    independent psychiatric correlate of head injury. In

    multiple predictor analyses, we confirmed that these

    relationships were not products of the comorbidity

    between the internalizing and externalizing disorders

    and the relationships between externalizing disorders

    and unintentional injury. Given the cross-sectional

    nature of the dataset, it is impossible to determine the

    order of occurrence of the injury and the onset of

    emotional difficulties. As with externalizing disorders, it

    is possible that anxiety and depression put children at

    increased risk for some types of unintentional injury.One possible mechanism here may be that internalizing

    disorders disrupt attentional control during potentially

    hazardous activities, making an accident more likely.

    First, general anxious and depressive cognitions may be

    distracting. Second, anxiety about performing the

    activity itself may cause interference. Alternatively,

    injury could contribute to risk for psychopathology,

    and this pathway may be particularly plausible regarding

    internalizing disorders. A range of adverse life events

    have been documented as risk factors for anxiety and

    depression in children (Goodyer, 1994; Goodyer,

    Cooper, Vize, & Ashby, 1993; Silberg et al., 1999),and it is possible that severe accidents contribute to risk

    in a similar way. An alternative mechanism for the

    relationship between head injury and anxiety may be

    that brain damage suffered as a component of the injury

    has led to the disorder (Brown, Chadwick, Shaffer,

    Rutter, & Traub, 1981). Our findings suggest that these

    differing possibilities require further examination in

    longitudinal studies.

    Of the psychosocial risk factors studied, poor

    reading skill emerged as an independent correlate of

    both burns and poisoning. One possible interpretation of

    this association is that reading problems contribute in

    a direct way to unintentional injury, by making it

    difficult, for example, for children to read safety

    instructions. Because most burns and poisonings appearto have been experienced prior to age 5, however, this

    seems an unlikely explanation in the majority of

    instances. Instead, two other accounts seem more

    plausible. First, reading difficulties are known to be

    associated with increased rates of inattention and

    overactivity (Hinshaw, 1992), which, even if not severe

    enough to meet criteria for ADHD, might nonetheless

    put poor readers at increased risk for unintentional

    injuries. Second, reading problems may act as a marker

    for other family difficulties, including stressors that

    reduce the effectiveness of parents capacity to supervise

    young children; the association between large family size

    and risk for poisoning may also point in a similar

    direction.

    One unexpected finding was the lower rate of

    fractures and head injury within some ethnic minorities.

    Recent findings from the Health Survey for England

    (Nazroo, Becher, Kelly, & McMunn, 2001) have also,

    however, documented differences between minority

    groups in parental report of recent major accidents,

    and more strikingly in relation to minor accidents. As

    these authors note, this may suggest underreporting in

    some groups; alternatively, other social or family

    correlates may be implicated. Once again, additional

    research is clearly needed to examine these important

    issues more closely.

    Finally, we found robust gender differences in

    unintentional injury. Consistent with many other

    studies, boys were at greater risk for all the injuries

    studied here. We had anticipated that some of this effect

    might be mediated by gender differences in the

    correlates of unintentional injury, and externalizing

    disorders, which are more common in boys than in girls

    (Rutter et al., 1998), seemed to be the primary

    candidates here. Our analyses provided little supportfor this hypothesis, however, as the increased rates of

    injury in boys compared with girls were little changed

    when the important correlates of unintentional injury

    were accounted for. This suggests that other factors

    might mediate the sex differences; one possibility is that

    temperamental characteristics such as activity levels and

    impulsivity are important here (Plumert & Schwebel,

    1997), indexing a broader spectrum of risk than that

    Table VI. Sex Differences in Unintentional Injury after Accounting

    for Independently Significant Predictors of Unintentional Injury

    Injury Type

    Sex Odds Ratio (95% CI)

    from Final Model

    Burns 1.4 (1.11.9)*

    Poisoning 1.3 (0.91.7)

    Head injury 1.7 (1.32.1)***

    Fractures 1.2 (1.11.4)**

    *p , .05; **p , .01; ***p , .001.

    Coefficients in boldface indicatep , .05.

    128 Rowe, Maughan, and Goodman

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    11/12

    identified by diagnostic groups alone. Other overlapping

    possibilities include biological differences and differ-

    ences in the socialization of boys and girls that make

    them more or less vulnerable to unintentional injuries.

    For example, gender role stereotypes for boys may

    include more injury-prone activities such as contact

    sports and rough-and-tumble play. Understanding theroots of these gender differences in unintentional injury

    must remain a challenge for further research.

    These analyses provide further evidence that child

    behavior is related to unintentional injury. Modification

    of behavior therefore offers a potential strategy to reduce

    the incidence of unintentional injury during childhood.

    Should it prove replicable, the pattern of results found

    here suggests that young children with ODD and

    children of all ages with ADHD may particularly benefit

    from such interventions in terms of reducing their injury

    liabilities. The strong associations between ODD and

    certain types of injury provide additional motivations forclinicians to focus on treatment of oppositional symp-

    tomatology, whether or not the disorder presents with

    other comorbid externalizing disorders. If, as discussed

    above, the parent/child relationship is of particular

    importance in mediating this contingency, then inter-

    ventions targeting improvements in parenting (Scott,

    Spender, Doolan, Jacobs, & Aspland, 2001) may be of

    particular benefit for reducing injury risk. Longitudinal

    studies will be required to identify whether the in-

    ternalizing disorders increase risk for injury rather than

    being consequences of it. If anxiety and depression are

    found to increase risk, however, then effective treatment

    of these disorders may provide an additional benefit in

    terms of reducing unintentional injury.

    As well as targeting psychiatric symptomatology

    directly, programs aimed at specifically reducing injury

    in these high-risk groups may also be useful. One

    important issue here is the level at which to target such

    interventions (Garbarino, 1988). Children suffering

    from disruptive disorders may be unreceptive to such

    interventions given the nature of their symptoms.

    Therefore programs targeting the primary caregiver,

    perhaps aiming to reduce the presence of household

    hazards in the childs environment, may be more

    appropriate here. Children with internalizing disordersmay be more receptive to safety instructions and could

    therefore also be targeted for injury prevention educa-

    tion, perhaps as part of a familywide intervention. The

    relationships between injury and psychopathology in

    childhood mean that health care professionals should be

    on the lookout for treatable psychiatric disorder among

    children who present with injury in emergency room

    and primary care settings.

    Received December 2, 2002; revisions received April 2,

    2003; accepted April 30, 2003

    ReferencesAPA [American Psychiatric Association] (1994).

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders,

    4th edition. Washington DC: Author.

    Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (1996). Toward

    establishing an empirical basis for the diagnosis of

    oppositional defiant disorder. Journal of the

    American Academy of Child and Adolescent

    Psychiatry, 35,12051212.

    Bijur, P. E., Golding, J., Haslum, M., & Kurzon, M.

    (1988). Behavioral predictors of injury in school-age

    children.American Journal of Diseases of Children,

    142,13071312.

    Bijur, P. E., Stewart-Brown, S., & Butler, N. (1986).

    Child behavior and accidental injury in 11,966

    preschool children.American Journal of Diseases of

    Children, 140,487492.

    Brehaut, J. C., Miller, A., Raina, P., & McGrail, K. M.

    (2003). Childhood behavior disorders and injuries

    among children and youth: A population-based

    study. Pediatrics, 111,262269.

    Brown, G., Chadwick, O., Shaffer, D., Rutter, M., &

    Traub, M. (1981). A prospective study of children

    with head injuries: III. Psychiatric sequelae.Psychological Medicine, 11,6378.

    Davidson, L. L., Taylor, E. A., Sandberg, S. T., &

    Thorley, G. (1992). Hyperactivity in school-age

    boys and subsequent risk of injury. Pediatrics, 90,

    697702.

    Dunn, L., Dunn, L., Whetton, C., & Burley, I. (1997).

    The British Picture Vocabulary Scale second edition

    testbook.Windsor: NFER.

    Elliot, C., Smith, P., & McCulloch, K. (1996). British

    Ability Scalessecond edition: Administration and

    scoring manual.Windsor: NFER.

    Epstein, N. B., Baldwin, L. M., & Bishop, D. S. (1983).The McMaster Family Assessment Device.Journal of

    Marital and Family Therapy, 9,171180.

    Garbarino, J. (1988). Preventing childhood injury:

    Developmental and child health issues. American

    Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 58,2545.

    Goldberg, D. P., Gater, R., Sartorious, N., Uston, T. B.,

    Picinelli, M., Gureje, O., et al. (1997). The validity

    of two versions of the GHQ in the WHO study of

    Psychopathology and Injury 129

  • 8/12/2019 Childhood 2004 Psychiatric Disorder and Unintentional Injury

    12/12