Chapter 6

80
Chapter 6 Object Perception

description

Chapter 6. Object Perception. Recognition: Relates a current percept to something familiar. Identification: Recognition of a specific instance. Categorization: Placing a specific object into a category. Object recognition. 0.1 sec to identify many objects. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Transcript of Chapter 6

Page 1: Chapter 6

Chapter 6

Object Perception

Page 2: Chapter 6

Recognition: Relates a current percept to something familiar.

Identification: Recognition of a specific instance.

Categorization: Placing a specific object into a category.

Page 3: Chapter 6

Object recognition

0.1 sec to identify many objects. We typically don’t encounter things

tachistoscopically; but we instead scan. Fixations last typically about .25 sec.

Page 4: Chapter 6
Page 5: Chapter 6
Page 6: Chapter 6
Page 7: Chapter 6
Page 8: Chapter 6

Challenges in object recognition

Occlusion (desk, screen) Upside down Picture of a chair vs. chair Far chair in room vs. toy chair on desk

Page 9: Chapter 6
Page 10: Chapter 6

Two types of theories for object recognition Recognition by components (Biederman) View-based recognition (Bülthoff, Tarr, et

al.)

Page 11: Chapter 6
Page 12: Chapter 6

Geons:

Distinguishable from almost any perspective.

Recognizable even with occlusions.

Page 13: Chapter 6

Pros and cons of geon theory

Explains why it is hard to recognize objects from unusual angles.

No physiological evidence. Sometimes recognition is viewpoint

dependent. Doesn’t explain recognition of individuals.

Page 14: Chapter 6
Page 15: Chapter 6

View-based recognition

People have preferred viewing angles for familiar objects.

Less consensus on preferred viewing angles for novel objects.

Recognition is slower the greater the deviance from preferred viewing angle.

Page 16: Chapter 6
Page 17: Chapter 6

Impact of rotation on recognition

James Stone (1998) showed ameboid images.

Page 18: Chapter 6

Impact of rotation on recognition

James Stone (1998) showed ameboid images.

Guy Wallis (1998) five head shots in apparent rotation.

Page 19: Chapter 6

Learning to see

The young woman – old woman illusion James Elkins experiences with moth

hunting Face - woman illusion High contrast images

Page 20: Chapter 6
Page 21: Chapter 6

Perceptual learning

(p. 203) Panel A: Orientation discrimination Panel B: Vernier acuity (The degree to which a

pair of fine lines can be aligned to each other.) Panel C: Orientation of 3-line bars (followed by

mask) Panel D: Face recognition in the presence of

noise.

Page 22: Chapter 6

Perceptual learning

Chicken sexing Perception of high contrast images (p.

232)Pre-training: 55% for faces; 13% for others.Post-training: 93% for faces; 87% for others.

Page 23: Chapter 6

Inferotemporal cortex

Single cell recording reveals:Few neurons that prefer oriented lines.Neurons with large receptive fields.Neurons that are sensitive to “diagnostic”

features, such as eyes of a face.Neurons that are occlusion insensitive.Neurons that are object size invariant.

Page 24: Chapter 6

Dolan: Viewing gray-scale images facilitates interpreting high contrast versions of those images.

Tovee, Rolls, and Ramachandran, (1996). Monkeys shown black & white, then gray-scale, then black & white again. IT cells were more active the second time around.

Page 25: Chapter 6

Kobatake, Wang, & Tanaka, (1998).

Monkeys shown a target, then after a delay the target with three other objects.

Monkeys improved with practice to be able to perform with longer delays.

IT cells changed firing patterns.

Page 26: Chapter 6

Face Recognition

Much worse when contrast is reversed. Much worse when inverted. The top half of a face is harder to

recognize, when the face is hybridized. Anger is the most distinctive of the six basic

facial expressions (sadness, happiness, anger, fear, surprise, and disgust).

Page 27: Chapter 6
Page 28: Chapter 6
Page 29: Chapter 6
Page 30: Chapter 6
Page 31: Chapter 6
Page 32: Chapter 6
Page 33: Chapter 6
Page 34: Chapter 6
Page 35: Chapter 6

Prosopagnosia

Two types of strategies to cope:Configural: patients base their judgements on

the overall similarity of the faces’ configuration.

Featural processing: patients base their judgements on parts of the face, rather than the whole.

Page 36: Chapter 6

Greebles

http://mail.cog.brown.edu/~tarr/projects/greeble_poster.html

They have symmetric body parts, families, and genders.

Becoming a greeble expert.

Page 37: Chapter 6
Page 38: Chapter 6

Greebles

Expert recognition: As good with general categories as with specific categories.Bird and black-capped chickadeeSex, family, and individual identity

People can become greeble experts after thousands of trials.

Page 39: Chapter 6

Become a Greeble expert

The better a person is at greeble recognition, the more than fMRIs look like facial fMRIs.

The ‘face’ area appears to be deployed in the inspection of even novel greebles.

Page 40: Chapter 6

Attention and object recognition

Attention reduces variability in perception.Prinzmetal, et al., show that distractors

increase trial-to-trial variability in color assessments.

Attention changes an object’s appearance Attention can increase sensitivity of

appearances.

Page 41: Chapter 6

Inattentional Blindness

Page 42: Chapter 6

Spotlight versus object attention

Edgly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Page 43: Chapter 6
Page 44: Chapter 6
Page 45: Chapter 6
Page 46: Chapter 6
Page 47: Chapter 6
Page 48: Chapter 6
Page 49: Chapter 6
Page 50: Chapter 6
Page 51: Chapter 6
Page 52: Chapter 6
Page 53: Chapter 6
Page 54: Chapter 6

Moore, Yantis, & Vaughn, (1998), variation

Page 55: Chapter 6
Page 56: Chapter 6
Page 57: Chapter 6
Page 58: Chapter 6
Page 59: Chapter 6
Page 60: Chapter 6
Page 61: Chapter 6
Page 62: Chapter 6
Page 63: Chapter 6
Page 64: Chapter 6

Attention and object recognition

Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) suggest that attention is object-based.

The gorilla and girl with umbrella examples also suggest that vision is not a spotlight, but is object-based.

Page 66: Chapter 6

Imagery and Vision

Cheves Perky (1910). Confusability of imagination and vision

fMRI has shown that some of the same brain regions are active during both vision and imagining, including V1. (Kosslyn, et al., 1999)

Page 67: Chapter 6
Page 68: Chapter 6

Imagery and Vision

Cheves Perky (1910). Confusability of imagination and vision

fMRI has shown that some of the same brain regions are active during both vision and imagining, including V1. (Kosslyn, et al., 1999)

TMS to V1 shows that response times on this task are slowed by ~200msec. About the same for both imagery and vision.

Page 69: Chapter 6

What does visual imagery look like? Compare doctored pictures with mental

images. Rate them for similarity on a scale 1-7 when

simultaneously present. Rate them for similarity on a scale 1-7 when

successively present. Images look like vision, except with high

frequency detail removed.

Page 70: Chapter 6

Perceptual Aspects of Reading

Alexia = inability to read acquired late in life.Letter by letter alexia appears to be due to a

visual processing deficit.

The number of saccades and fixation times depends on reading ability and difficulty of material.

Page 71: Chapter 6

Basic facts on reading saccades

Saccades occur on average 0.25 sec. Range of 0.1-0.5 sec. Distance of 2 – 18 letters. 90% of saccades among English readers

are left to right. By making text fill one spot, it is possible

to increase reading rates 3-4 times.

Page 72: Chapter 6

Word superiority effect

Letters can be better recognized in the context of words, than in isolation.

Thus, it appears that words are processed holistically, rather than individually.

Page 73: Chapter 6
Page 74: Chapter 6

Reading through a window

One letter at a time: 90% accuracy on words.

7-9 letters (so entire words could be seen), words were read error-free, but more slowly than normal.

Four words at a time, reading was still slower than normal.

Page 75: Chapter 6
Page 76: Chapter 6

Contextual influences

Expectations shape reading. Unexpected words involve longer fixations.

Page 77: Chapter 6

Spaces in reading

Spaces are not necessary for reading. In fact, you can learn to read about as accurately and with comprehension with as without them.

Fixation points were largely unchanged with the removal of spaces.

Insertion of spaces in the wrong place lead to slowed processing.

Page 78: Chapter 6

Spaces and meaning

Booth, Epelboim, and Steinman (1996). Meaning versus interword spaces.

MeaningfulMeaningless

Normal spacing 100% <<100%

No spacing <100% <<100%

Page 79: Chapter 6

Temporary failures of processing What happens in the face of extreme

ambiguity? Bruner and Potter (1964), studied

perception of defocused images. If you start with more defocused images,

recognition is slowed.Hypothesis: People always conjecture

something and then persevere.

Page 80: Chapter 6

Age and recognition of defocused images Adults are 50% faster than children in

recognition. Adults shape their guesses in a

continuous way; children change their guesses randomly at each stage.