Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter...

11
Chapter 4 Day Six • Establishing Causation

Transcript of Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter...

Page 1: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Chapter 4 Day Six

• Establishing Causation

Page 2: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Establishing Causation

Page 3: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Beware the post-hoc fallacy

“Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.”

To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming that an observed correlation is due to causation, you must put any statement of relationship through sharp inspection.

Causation can not be established “after the fact.” It can only be established through well-designed experiments. {see Ch 5}

Page 4: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Explaining Association

Strong Associations can generally be explained by one of three relationships.

Confounding: x may cause y, but y may instead be caused by a confounding variable z

Common Response: x and y are reacting to a lurking variable z

Causation:x causes y

Page 5: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Causation

Causation is not easily established.

The best evidence for causation comes from experiments that change x while holding all other factors fixed.

Even when direct causation is present, it is rarely a complete explanation of an association between two variables.

Even well established causal relations may not generalize to other settings.

Page 6: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Common Response“Beware the Lurking Variable”

The observed association between two variables may be due to a third variable.

Both x and y may be changing in response to changes in z.

Consider the fact that students who are smart and who have learned a lot tend to have both high SAT scores and high collge grades. The positive correlation is explained by this common response to students’ ability and knowledge.

Page 7: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Confounding

Two variables are confounded when their effects on a response variable cannot be distinguished from each other.

Confounding prevents us from drawing conclusions about causation.

We can help reduce the chances of confounding by designing a well-controlled experiment.

Page 8: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Confounding Example• Mothers with high BMI have a strong correlation with

daughters with high BMI. Gene inheritance no doubt explains part of the association between BMI of daughters and their mothers, but can we use r or r squared to say how much inheritance contributes to the daugthers’ BMI’s? No!

• Mothers who are overweight also set an example of little exercise, poor eating habits, and lots of television so their daughters pick up these habits to some extent, so the influence of heredity is mixed up with influences from the girls’ environment.

• The mixing of influences is what we call confounding.

Page 9: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Examples4.41: There is a high positive correlation: nations with many TV sets have higher life expectancies. Could we lengthen the life of people in Rwanda by shipping them TVs?

4.42: People who use artificial sweeteners in place of sugar tend to be heavier than people who use sugar. Does artificial sweetener use cause weight gain?

4.43: Women who work in the production of computer chips have abnormally high numbers of miscarriages. The union claimed chemicals cause the miscarriages. Another explanation may be the fact these workers spend a lot of time on their feet.

Page 10: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

cont.4.44: People with two cars tend to live longer than people who own only one car. Owning three cars is even better, and so on. What might explain the association?

4.45: Children who watch many hours of TV get lower grades on average than those who watch less TV. Why does this fact not show that watching TV causes low grades?

Page 11: Chapter 4 Day Six Establishing Causation. Beware the post-hoc fallacy “Post hoc, ergo propter hoc.” To avoid falling for the post-hoc fallacy, assuming.

Cont.

4.46: Data show that married men (and men who are divorced or widowed) earn more than men who have never been married. If you want to make more money, should you get married?

4.47: High school students who take the SAT, enroll in an SAT coaching course, and take the SAT again raise their mathematics score from an average of 521 to 561. Can this increase be attributed entirely to taking the course?