Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

download Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

of 10

Transcript of Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    1/10

    This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attachedcopy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial researchand education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

    and sharing with colleagues.

    Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling orlicensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party

    websites are prohibited.

    In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of thearticle (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website orinstitutional repository. Authors requiring further information

    regarding Elseviers archiving and manuscript policies areencouraged to visit:

    http://www.elsevier.com/copyright

    http://www.elsevier.com/copyrighthttp://www.elsevier.com/copyright
  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    2/10

    Author's personal copy

    DEVELOPMENTAL EMERGENCE OF REELIN DEFICITS IN THEPREFRONTAL CORTEX OF WISTAR RATS REARED IN SOCIALISOLATION

    A. W. CASSIDY, a S. K. MULVANY, a M. N. PANGALOS, b

    K. J. MURPHY a AND C. M. REGAN a *a The Applied Neurotherapeutics Research Group, School of Biomo-lecular and Biomedical Science, UCD Conway Institute, University College Dublin, Beleld, Dublin 4, Ireland b Discovery Neuroscience, Wyeth Research, Princeton, NJ 08543,USA

    AbstractAs the pathophysiological mechanism(s) of manyneuropsychiatric disorders relate to GABAergic interneuronstructure and function, we employed isolation rearing of Wistar rats as a model to correlate developmental emergenceof cognitive decits with the expression of reelin-producinginterneurons in the medial prefrontal cortex (PFC). Prepulseinhibition decits emerged at postnatal day 60 and persistedinto adulthood. Paralleling the emergence of these neurobe-havioural decits was an increase in reelin production andreelin-immunopositive cells in layer I of the PFC and this later became signicantly reduced at postnatal day 80. Cells ex-pressing reelin immunoreactivity in a horizontal orientationwere mainly located to the upper regions of layer I whereasthose with a vertical orientation, whose arbors extend intocortical layers II and III, were more numerous in the lower regions of layer I and became signicantly dysregulated dur-ing postnatal development. No behavioural decits or alteredreelin expression was observed at postnatal days 30 or 40.Developmental emergence of neurobehavioural and reelindecits in isolation reared animals is proposed to reectmaladaptive wiring within the medial prefrontal cortex duringa critical maturation period of this circuitry. 2010 IBRO.Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    Key words: prepulse inhibition, GABAergic interneurons, iso-lation rearing, layer I, immunouorescence, prelimbic cortex.

    Studies of postmortem brain tissue have provided signi-

    cant evidence that the GABAergic system is signicantlydisrupted in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders ( Lewiset al., 2005; Lisman et al., 2008 ). Such studies have showna reduction in protein markers of interneuron function inpost-mortem tissue obtained from the prefrontal cortex( Akbarian et al., 1995; Beasley and Reynolds, 1997; Sakaiet al., 2008 ), cingulate cortex ( Benes et al., 1991; Woo etal., 2004; Oblak et al., 2009 ), and temporal lobe ( Chanceet al., 2005 ). The reproducibility of these ndings supports

    the possibility that interneuron decits are a central featurein the underlying pathophysiology of these disorders(Benes and Berretta, 2001; Fountoulakis et al., 2008;Lewis and Gonzlez-Burgos, 2008 ).

    One subclass of GABAergic interneurons express andsecrete reelin ( Pesold et al., 1998, 1999 ), a large extracel-lular matrix glycoprotein ( 400 kDa) that has a wide arrayof functions in both the developing and adult cortex. Duringearly development reelin is synthesised by CajalRetziusinterneurons and plays a vital role in the lamination of cortical cell layers ( Tissir and Gofnet, 2003 ). In adulthoodreelin becomes more widely expressed by GABA interneu-rons, where it is believed to play a role in the renement of dendritic arbor and synapse formation ( Borrel et al., 1999;Costa et al., 2001; Dong et al., 2003; Boqoch and Linial,2008 ). Reelin contains eight repeats of 300350 aminoacids and, upon secretion into the extracellular space, iscleaved by metalloproteinases between repeats 23 and67, the central 36 repeats being required for activationof its receptor complex that is formed by the very low

    density lipoprotein receptor and apolipoprotein E receptor 2 (Lambert de Rouvroit et al., 1999; Trommsdorff et al.,1999; Ignatova et al., 2004; Jossin et al., 2004 ).

    The secretion of reelin into the extracellular spacesurrounding dendrites, dendritic spines and axon boutons(DArcangelo et al., 1995; Alcntara et al., 1998; Pappas etal., 2002; Tissir and Gofnet, 2003 ) is known to regulatesynapse structure and stability ( Weeber et al., 2002; Donget al., 2003 ). Evidence exists to implicate reelin decits inthe developmental emergence of a number of neuropsy-chiatric conditions including schizophrenia, bipolar disor-der (Torrey et al., 2005 ) and autism ( Fatemi et al., 2005; Ashley-Koch et al., 2007 ). For example in schizophrenia,reelin-mediated synapse plasticity appears to be compro-mised as both reelin mRNA and GAD67 mRNA expressionhave been found to be signicantly depressed in GABAergicinterneurons in the supercial layers of the prefrontalcortex ( Impagnatiello et al., 1998; Fatemi et al., 2000;Guidotti et al., 2000; Grayson et al., 2005; Torrey et al.,2005 ). These observations suggest altered reelin activitymay, in part, be responsible for the emergence of this andother psychotic states, however, postmortem tissue de-rived from patients at the end-stage of schizophrenia can-not provide an insight into the preceding developmentalmechanisms.

    As many psychiatric disorders emerge during late ad-olescence ( Paus et al., 2008 ), animal models that recapit-

    ulate their cardinal features in adulthood become a priorityin any attempt to understand the developmental mecha-

    *Corresponding author. Tel: 353-1-716-6775; fax: 353-1-716-6920.E-mail address: [email protected] (C. M. Regan). Abbreviations: ANOVA, analysis of variance; BSA, bovine serum al-bumin; EDTA, ethylenediamine tetra acetic acid; HEPES, 4-(2-hy-droxyethyl)-1-piperazineethanesulfonic acid; NGS, normal goat se-

    rum; PBS, phosphate-buffered saline; PFC, prelimbic region of pre-frontal cortex; PPI, prepulse inhibition; TBS-T, tris-buffered saline.

    Neuroscience 166 (2010) 377385

    0306-4522/10 $ - see front matter 2010 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.12.045

    377

  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    3/10

    Author's personal copy

    nisms that lead to the onset of psychosis. Several modelshave been extensively studied and include repetitive ad-ministrations of NMDA antagonists ( Braun et al., 2007 ),neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion ( Lipska et al., 1993;Tseng et al., 2008 ) and rearing in isolation from time of weaning ( Geyer et al., 1993; Fone and Porkess, 2008 ). Ineach model, the behavioural phenotype includes hyperlo-comotion and prepulse inhibition decits. Most of theseabnormalities emerge during adolescence and in manycases there is also evidence of a reduction in markers of GABAergic interneurons ( Lipska et al., 2003; Pillai-Nair etal., 2005; Penschuck et al., 2006; Endo et al., 2007 ).

    As reelin is implicated in the developmental structuring of the prefrontal cortex, a structure intimately involved in behav-ioural responses, we have correlated alteration of reelin pro-tein expression and reelin-secreting cells with emergence of behavioural decits in isolation reared animals.

    EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES

    Animal maintenance

    Experimentally naive male Wistar rats were employed in all stud-ies. The animals were purpose bred at the Biomedical Facility,University College Dublin, and maintained in standard laboratoryconditions until the time of experimental use. Animals were intro-duced to the experimental holding rooms 5 days prior to thecommencement of the study, housed in groups of 34 during thisperiod, and maintained at 2224 C on a standard 12 h light/darkcycle, with food and water available ad libitum . On each of the 2days preceding commencement of behavioural studies, the ani-mals were handled and weighed and assessed in an open-eldarena for locomotor activity, rearing and general behaviour over a5 min period. All observations were carried out in the quiet roomunder low-level, red light illumination between 8:00 and 12:00 h tominimise the inuence of circadian rhythms. Isolation-reared ani-mals (isolated animals) were housed individually in non-soft bot-tom cages (225 345 170 mm), from time of weaning (postnatalday 25) until completion of behavioural testing. The standard 12 hlight/dark cycle was maintained and food and water was providedad libitum . Noise and visual stimuli were kept to an absoluteminimum as previously described ( Geyer et al., 1993 ). All exper-imental procedures were approved by the Animal Research EthicsCommittee of University College Dublin, conformed to EU CouncilDirective 86609-EEC, and were carried out by individuals retain-ing the appropriate licence issued by the Irish Department of Health. Throughout the course of these studies, every attemptwas made to ensure that the number of animals and any physical

    distress was kept to an absolute minimum.

    Sensorimotor gating

    Pre-pulse inhibition served as an operational measure of senso-rimotor gating decits ( Swerdlow et al., 1994 ) and the protocolemployed was based on a procedure previously described byGeyer and colleagues (1993) . Each rat was restrained in anappropriately sized cylindrical holder, placed on a movement-sensitive platform and maintained in a soundproof chamber. Therat was allowed to habituate to a white noise background of 70 dBfor 5 min before receiving ve 20 ms startle trials of 120 dB,separated by randomised intervals of 1020 s. Immediately there-after, each rat received ve separate presentations with one of theprepulse stimuli of 72, 76, 80, or 84 dB and these were followed,100 ms later, by the 120 dB acoustic startle stimulus. Each trialwas separated by a time interval of 1020 s. The four prepulsestimuli were delivered in a randomised manner and included

    periods in which there was no prepulse or startle stimulus. Thesession terminated with an additional ve startle trials. Signalswere integrated by the software supplied by the manufacturers of equipment hardware (MED-Associates Inc., St. Albans, VT, USA).

    The effect of isolation rearing on pre-pulse inhibition was deter-mined in separate cohorts of animals ( n 78) on postnatal days30, 40, 60 and 80. The effects of isolation rearing on pre-pulseinhibition, as compared to that of the aged-matched cohorts(n 78) of social animals, were assessed by two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post hoc analysis using a Bonferroni postt -test. In all cases, P -values less than 0.05 were considered to besignicant.

    Immunoblot analysis of reelin expression

    Tissue preparation. Separate cohorts ( n 4) of naive animalsreared in isolation or social groups were killed on postnatal days 30,40, 60 and 80, the brain removed and the medial prefrontal cortexdissected and excised. Samples were immediately placed in cryo-tubes, snap frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored in a 80 C freezer until required. Immediately prior to use, the samples were homoge-nised at 4 C in 300 l of 10 mM HEPES, pH 7.4, containing 0.32 Msucrose, 2 mM EDTA, and 0.01% of a protease and phosphataseinhibitor cocktail (Sigma, UK). The homogenates were subsequentlycentrifuged (1000 rpm, 15 min) and the supernatant was removedand stored at 20 C until further use.

    SDS polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis and Western-blotting procedure. Protein concentrations were determined by theBCA assay, according to manufacturers instructions (Pierce,Rockford, IL, USA), and samples, of equal protein concentration,were boiled for 3 min in 70 mM TrisHCl, pH 6.8, containing 33mM NaCl, 1 mM EDTA, 2% (w/v) SDS, 0.01% (w/v) BromophenolBlue, 10% glycerol and 3% v/v dithiothreitol reducing agent. Thereduced and solubilised proteins were separated using pre-pre-pared 5% polyacrylamide gels and, subsequently, transferred tonitrocellulose membranes, according to manufacturers instruc-tions (Biorad, UK). Pre-stained molecular weight markers wereco-electrophoresed with the protein samples (Sigma, UK). Suc-cessful protein transfer was conrmed by staining the nitrocellu-lose sheet with Ponceau Red solution (Sigma, UK) prior to immu-noblotting and by Napthol Blue (Sigma, UK) following completionof immunoblotting. Reactive groups on the nitrocellulose sheetwere then inactivated using Tris buffered saline solution (TBS-T)blocking buffer (10 mM TrisHCl, pH 7.4, containing 150 mMNaCl, 0.05% (v/v) Tween-20, and with 5% (w/v) non-fat milkpowder) for 1 h at room temperature. The membrane was subse-quently incubated overnight (20 h) at 4 C in blocking buffer (5%v/v) containing a mouse monoclonal antibody to reelin (G10; Abcam, UK; 1:10,000 dilution). Following overnight incubation, thenitrocellulose membrane was washed three times in TBS-T pH 7.4

    before being incubated for 1 h in blocking buffer containing ahorse radish peroxidase-conjugated anti-mouse IgG monoclonalantibody (Novagen, UK; 1:20,000 dilution). After incubation, thenitrocellulose sheet was washed three times in washing buffer,exposed for 5 min to a chemiluminescent peroxidase substrate(Pierce, Rockford, IL, USA) washed and exposed to X-ray lm(Fuji, UK) in a dark room under red light illumination, until optimalresolution of the protein bands was achieved. Immunoblot analy-sis of Reelin with the G10 antibody revealed three specic proteinbands, the full length 400 kDa protein and two bands at 300 and180 kDa, the latter representing the proteolytic fragments gener-ated by metalloproteinase cleavage at repeats 23 and 67,respectively. The X-ray lms were scanned, converted into adigital format and the immunostained band density analysed usingImageJ software ( http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij/docs/index.html ). TheNaphthol Black-stained cellulose sheet was also scanned anddigitised and used to correct the immunostained bands for un-equal protein loading.

    A. W. Cassidy et al. / Neuroscience 166 (2010) 377385378

  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    4/10

    Author's personal copy

    Quantitative immunohistochemical analysis of reelinexpression in vivo

    Tissue preparation. Animals were terminally anaesthetised

    using 1 ml/kg sodium pentobarbital (Euthatal, Pzer AnimalHealth, UK) and their tissue was xed by transcardial perfusionwith a saline solution (0.9% w/v) for 2 min, followed by a 20 minperfusion with 0.12 M Srenson phosphate buffer (pH 7.2) con-taining 4% paraformaldehyde. The animals were then killed andtheir brains were removed and stored in 4% paraformaldehyde inSrensons buffer for a 24 h period. Following xation, the brainswere placed in a cryoprotective solution (40% sucrose [w/v] and10% glycerol [v/v] in dH 2 0) at 4 C for approximately 48 h. Thebrains were then coated in optimum cutting temperature com-pound (O.C.T.; Tissue-Tek, UK), to provide an even freezingrate, and lowered into a Cryoprep freezing apparatus (Algen Inc,UK) containing liquid CO 2 cooled n-hexane. The tissue was storedat 80 C until required.

    Cryosectioning and immunohistochemical protocols. Sections

    (16 m thick) of the prefrontal cortex were taken at level 3.2 mmrostral to bregma ( Paxinos and Watson, 1986 ) and placed in 0.32 Msucrose solution for 5 min. Free-oating sections were then washedtwice in 0.1 M (pH 7.4) PBS solution (phosphate buffered saline;Sigma, UK) solution for 10 min. Subsequently, the sections wereincubated in PBS containing 10% normal goat serum (NGS; Dako,DK) for 30 min, followed by incubation in a humidied chamber for 20 h with the G10 reelin monoclonal antibody diluted 1:1000 in PBScontaining 2% NGS and 2% bovine serum albumen (BSA; Sigma,UK). The sections were then washed twice in PBS for 10 min andfurther incubated for 3 h in the humidied chamber with uorescein-conjugated goat anti-mouse IgG antibody diluted 1:2000 in PBScontaining 2% BSA and 2% NGS. The sections were then washed inPBS and some sections were briey counterstained with PropidiumIodide (1 g/ml; Sigma, UK) for 5 s, collected on microscope slidesandmounted in Citiuor (AgarScientic), a uorescence-enhancingmedium. For qualitative purposes further sections were incubated for 20 min in a PBS solution containing Neuro-Trace, a uorescentNissl stain (1:100, Invitrogen), and counterstained with Hoechst33258 (1:1000, MolecularProbes, USA). Heat maps were created bypseudo-colouring photomicrographs of sections stained with the G10reelin antibody and an anti-mouse FITC-labeled secondaryantibody,areas of intense reelin immunopositive stain being represented byred.

    Quantitative evaluation of reelin-positive cells. A montageof three separate images that included the entire depth of theprelimbic cortex was created using a Leica DMLB uoresc-ence microscope. A counting frame (0.898 0.349 mm 2 ), outliningthe width of each layer, was overlaid on a montage of imagesobtained from the prelimbic cortex to facilitate counting of layer-specic cell number. Seven separate montages, derived fromserial sections obtained from each animal were used to estimatereelin immunopositive cells and this number was normalised tocells/mm 2 /unit area by dividing by the area of each layer. Thelayer areas employed were Layer I: 0.052 mm 2 ; Layer II: 0.017mm 2 ; Layer III: 0.043 mm 2 ; Layer V: 0.098 mm 2 ; and Layer VI:0.101 mm 2 . Cell counts were standardised to unit area of thegranule cell layer and expressed as mean SEM of cells/mm 2 .Statistical analysis employed the Students t -test and a signi-cance level of P 0.05 was employed in all cases.

    RESULTS

    Developmental emergence of behavioural decits inrats reared in isolation

    Analysis of open-eld behaviour in rats maintained in iso-lation from postnatal day 25 revealed signicant decits in

    the ability of adult animals (postnatal day 80) to habituateto a novel environment. Animals reared in social groupsexhibited the expected decrease in locomotor activity (ses-sion 1: 171.8 7.6; session II: 91.4 13.4; P 0.05) andvertical rearing (session 1: 22.5 1.4; session II: 11.6 1.4;rears/unit time; P 0.05) upon re-exposure to the open-eldparadigm. By contrast, animals reared in isolation failed tohabituate to the open-eld environment (Locomotor activity session 1: 182.9 13.1; session II: 171.9 15.9; P 0.05) andvertical rearing (Vertical rearingsession 1: 23.4 1.1; ses-sion II: 21.43 0.9; rears/unit time; P 0.05). This failure tohabituate in the open-eld paradigm is in agreement with allprevious studies that have employed isolation rearing tomodel features of neuropsychiatric conditions (for a reviewsee Fone and Porkess, 2008 ). However, this habituationdecit did not emerge in a developmental manner as bothrearing and locomotor activity over postnatal days 3060

    was found to be most erratic (data not shown). Animals reared in isolation also displayed impairments

    in sensorimotor processing, as assessed using the pre-pulse inhibition paradigm. These animals tended to exhibitan increased responsiveness, as judged by their basalstartle amplitude to a single 120 dB acoustic startle stim-ulus, and this was signicantly different to that observed inthe social control group at postnatal day 30 ( P 0.0019;unpaired two-tailed Students t -test) and postnatal day 60(P 0.0193; unpaired two-tailed Students t -test) ( Fig. 1 A).During behavioural testing the calibration of the move-ment-sensitive platform was maintained at a constant leveland the observed steady increase in basal startle ampli-tude was, therefore, directly correlated with the weight gainof cohorts over development. Exposure of animals rearedin isolation or maintained in social groups to separateprepulse stimuli of 72, 76, 80 and 84 dB resulted in anincreasing inhibition of the response to a subsequent star-tle stimulus of 120 dB ( Fig. 1B). Given the small prepulseincrements employed, the response curve was shallow butrobust at all developmental timepoints examined, with theexception of that obtained at postnatal day 40. Comparisonof the cohort reared in isolation to that of the social controlgroup revealed no signicant reductions in prepulse inhi-bition when tested at postnatal day 30 ( F [1,52] 0.083;P 0.7740; two-way ANOVA) and postnatal day 40(F [1,56] 1.515; P 0.2236), however, signicant decits

    became apparent at postnatal day 60 ( F [1,56] 15.38;P 0.0002) and these persisted into maturity at postnatalday 80 ( F [1,52] 17.35; P 0.0001). These effects on pre-pulse inhibition have been demonstrated to be indepen-dent of basal startle effects in all previous studies onisolation rearing ( Geyer et al., 1993; Domeney and Feldon,1998; Heidbreder et al., 2001 ).

    Disruption of reelin expression in rats rearedin isolation

    In order to relate the age-dependent emergence of cogni-tive decits to a cellular determinant of neural structuring,we further examined the inuence of isolation rearing on

    the expression of reelin in the prefrontal cortex. Immuno-blots, developed using the G10 monoclonal antibody to

    A. W. Cassidy et al. / Neuroscience 166 (2010) 377385 379

  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    5/10

    Author's personal copy

    reelin, reliably detected the 400 kDa full length protein andits expected proteolytic fragments of 300 and 180 kDa ( Fig.2 A). Strong immunoreactivity was detected in both the

    400, 300 and 180 kDa reelin bands at all developmentalages examined in both the prefrontal cortex obtained from

    animals reared in isolation and social groups. Semi-quan-titative analysis of these immunoblots revealed the full-length 400 kDa reelin protein in the prefrontal cortex to besignicantly modulated during the development of animalsreared in isolation, as compared to that observed in thesocial control group ( Fig. 2B). Reelin expression remainedunchanged over postnatal days 30 and 40 but exhibited a60% increase at postnatal day 60 followed by a 40%decrease at postnatal day 80 in the isolation reared ani-mals, both modulations being signicant relative to thesocial control group ( P 0.0419 and P 0.0003, respec-tively; two-tailed unpaired Students t -test).

    Fig. 2. Inuence of isolation rearing on reelin expression in the pre-frontal cortex (PFC) of Wistar rats at increasing age. Immunoblotsillustrating the major protein products of the reelin protein are shown inPanel A and their semi-quantitative densitometric analysis is shown inPanel B. The values are expressed as the mean SEM and signicantdifferences (two-tailed unpaired Students t -test) between the isolationreared (IR; lled circles) and social cohorts (SC; open circles) areindicated in the gure. Signicant differences by t -test are indicatedwith an asterisk ( P 0.05).

    Fig. 1. Inuence of isolation rearing on pre-pulse inhibition in Wistar rats of increasing age. Basal startle amplitude (Panel A) values areexpressed as the mean SEM and those signicantly different(P 0.05; unpaired two-tailed Students t -test) between the isolationreared group ( n 78) and the social control group ( n 78) areindicated with an asterisk. The effect of separate prepulse stimuli onstartle inhibition to a subsequent pulse of 120 dB is shown in Panel B.The values are expressed as the mean SEM and signicant differ-ences (two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test) between theisolation reared (lled columns and circles) and social cohorts (opencolumns and circles) are indicated in the gure.

    A. W. Cassidy et al. / Neuroscience 166 (2010) 377385380

  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    6/10

    Author's personal copy

    The above isolation rearing-induced modulations inreelin expression may relate to change in the rate of pro-tein synthesis or to aberrations in the developmental ex-pression of GABA interneurons, the main source of reelin.Immunohistochemistry was, therefore, employed to exam-ine the expression of reelin-positive cells in cohorts of animals reared in isolation and in social groups. Reelin-immunopositive cells were found to be predominantly lo-cated to layers I and II of the prefrontal cortex ( Fig. 3 A)where reelin was found to be strongly expressed in thecytoplasmic compartment ( Fig. 3B). Quantitative analysisof the reelin immunopositive cells in layer I of the prefrontalcortex revealed negligible change in the density of thesecells during development of animals reared in socialgroups ( Fig. 3C). By contrast, isolation rearing induced asignicant dysregulation in the density of immunpositive

    cells (Fig. 3C), in a manner that was reminiscent of thatobserved for reelin protein expression ( Fig. 2B). No differ-

    ence in the density of reelin immunopositive cells wasapparent at postnatal day 40, however, their number showed a signicant increase of 40% at postnatal day 60and 50% decrease at postnatal day 80 ( P 0.0276 andP 0.0196, respectively; two-tailed unpaired Students t -test). This effect was restricted to layer I of the prefrontalcortex as no signicant change in reelin immunopositivecell frequency in layers II-VI was induced by isolationrearing ( Table 1 ).

    Closer inspection of the reelin immunopositive cells inlayer I of the prefrontal cortex allowed the identication of two sub-populations of cells based on the alignment of their immunoreactivity pattern and their position withinlayer I. Cells expressing reelin immunoreactivity in a hori-zontal orientation were more numerous in the upper re-gions of layer I, as evidenced by the few counterstained

    propidium iodide nuclei, suggesting these to be the bipolar cells (Fig. 4 A, B) (Bacon et al., 1996; Gabbott et al., 1997 ).

    I

    II

    III

    V

    VI

    Deeplayers

    Upper layers

    piaA

    20 m

    B

    *

    *

    R e e

    l i n p o s

    i t i v e c e

    l l s

    / m m

    2

    60

    80

    100

    120

    140

    160

    180

    200

    Postnatal day40 60 80

    C

    Layer I

    Fig. 3. Inuence of isolation rearing on the density of reelin-expressing cells in the prefrontal cortex of Wistar rats at increasing age. The distributionof immunopositive cells in the layers of the prefrontal cortex is illustrated in Panel A and the extra-nuclear location of the immunoreactivity is shownin Panel B (red demonstrating highest expression). Quantitation of cell density in layer I of the prefrontal cortex is shown in Panel C and values areexpressed as the mean SEM and signicant differences between the isolation reared (lled circles) and social cohorts (open circles) are shown withan asterisk ( P 0.05, two-tailed unpaired Students t -test). For interpretation of the references to color in this gure legend, the reader is referred tothe Web version of this article.

    A. W. Cassidy et al. / Neuroscience 166 (2010) 377385 381

  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    7/10

    Author's personal copy

    Those with reelin immunoreactivity exhibiting a verticalorientation were more numerous in the lower regions of layer I, where a greater number of counterstained nucleiwere evident ( Fig. 4C, D), suggesting these to be thevertical cells whose arbors extend into cortical layers II andIII (Fig. 4E, F) (Gabbott et al., 1997 ). Following isolationrearing, both horizontal and vertical cell populations weresignicantly decreased at postnatal day 80 ( P 0.0100 andP 0.0415, respectively, two-tailed unpaired Students t -test), however, the signicant increase in overall reelin-positive cell number at postnatal day 60 ( Fig. 3) was ob-served only in the vertical cell population ( P 0.0348, two-tailed unpaired Students t -test) ( Fig. 4G, H).

    DISCUSSIONDevelopmental emergence of isolationrearing-induced decits in prepulse inhibition

    Rearing Wistar rats in isolation from time of weaning re-sulted in hyperlocomotion and signicant decits in pre-pulse inhibition, as has been previously reported in similar studies ( Geyer et al., 1993; Fone and Porkess, 2008 ) andargued to be hallmarks of the behavioural decits associ-ated with neuropsychiatric conditions ( Swerdlow et al.,1994 ). The inability of rats reared in isolation to habituateto the open-eld environment demonstrates disrupted in-tegration of past and current experience, which is believed

    to be a core decit contributing to acute psychosis ( Gray,1998; Gray et al., 1999 ). Moreover, hyperlocomotion anddecits in sensorimotor processing observed in animalsreared in isolation has been related to increased subcorti-cal dopamine transmission ( Swerdlow et al., 2001 ). Im-paired prepulse inhibition is thought to reect the cognitivefragmentation associated with such conditions ( Braff andGeyer, 1990; Geyer et al., 1993 ), and, in close alignmentwith the human conditions, these decits emerge duringadolescence in isolation reared rats ( Lipska et al., 1995;Bakshi and Geyer, 1999 ). In conclusion, the spectrum of behavioural abnormalities observed in rats reared in iso-lation further validate this model as one that reasonably

    recapitulates some of the major correlates of neuropsychi-atric disorders.

    Correlation of reelin expression with prepulseinhibition decits

    The possible association of impaired neuroplastic mecha-nisms with the developmental emergence of cognitive def-icits in animals reared in isolation was provided by ananalysis of reelin expression. In the prefrontal cortex, thematching increase in reelin protein expression and fre-quency of reelin immunopositive cells in layer I suggestedthe emergence of prepulse inhibition decits at postnatalday 60 to be accompanied by a signicant increase in theproduction of this extracellular matrix protein. Within layer I,which consists almost entirely of GABAergic interneurons,the classic horizontal interneurons extend their wide dendriticarbour throughout this layer ( Hestrin and Armstrong, 1996;

    Gabbott et al., 1997 ). By contrast, the vertical GABAergicinterneurons within layer I project descending axons intodeeper cortical lamina of layers II and III ( Gabbott et al., 1997; Aguil et al., 1999 ). Interestingly, the timing of the increasein reelin expression coincides with the proliferation of amygdalar and ventral hippocampal afferents on tothese GABAergic interneurons and pyramidal neuronsof layers II and III during late adolescence in rodents,primates and humans ( Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997;Gogtay et al., 2004; Cunningham et al., 2002, 2008 ). Asthe role of this secreted matrix protein in later postnatalperiods relates mainly to dendritic remodelling and syn-aptogenesis, the marked change in reelin expression ob-served at postnatal day 60 in isolation reared animals sug-gests the emergence of prepulse inhibition decits maybeassociated with enhanced, possibly excessive synapse re-modelling (Jay and Witter, 1991 ; Bacon et al., 1996 ; Borrel etal., 1999; Costa et al., 2001; Dong et al., 2003; Cunninghamet al., 2002, 2008; Boqoch and Linial, 2008 ).

    The persistence of decits in prepulse inhibition andthe decrease in reelin expression at postnatal day 80further suggests that the reelin-associated changes maybe initially compensatory in nature but fail to correct theisolation-induced dysregulation of the developing neuralcircuits, the low reelin expression ultimately resulting insynapse loss. This concept is consistent with post-mor-tem studies on tissue derived from schizophrenic pa-

    tients that have demonstrated reduced neuropil and syn-apse number in layer III ( Selemon and Goldman-Rakic,

    Table 1. Expression of reelin immunopositive cells in individual layers of the prelimbic cortex in isolation-reared and social control groups

    Layer I Layer II Layer III Layer V Layer VI

    Postnatal day 40Social control 148.4 12.18 94.54 33.15 84.72 15.49 94.02 19.46 65.77 13.65Isolation reared 151.8 14.85 92.44 22.76 90.53 7.58 95.12 14.29 64.36 4.64

    Postnatal day 60Social control 138.7 7.31 119.7 5.29 69.77 9.00 70.34 6.85 63.65 8.65Isolation reared 179.4 12.00* 102.2 2.43 82.78 6.17 70.70 4.93 61.23 4.85

    Postnatal day 80Social control 136.7 8.11 98.74 16.94 68.94 10.45 72.89 6.35 55.16 9.83Isolation reared 87.91 13.13* 60.92 12.55 47.34 8.72 56.85 12.89 48.80 15.00

    Data represents the mean SEM (n 34) of reelin immunopositive cells/mm 2 in each layer of the prelimbic cortex. Values signicantly different fromthe social control group are indicated with an asterisk (*)( P 0.05; two-tailed unpaired Students t -test).

    A. W. Cassidy et al. / Neuroscience 166 (2010) 377385382

  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    8/10

    Author's personal copy

    1999 ) and the substantial losses of GABAergic interneu-ron markers in layers IIII of the medial prefrontal cortex(Glantz and Lewis, 1997; Day-Wilson et al., 2006;

    Bloomeld et al., 2008; Woo et al., 1998; Pierri et al.,1999; Volk et al., 2002 ).

    CONCLUSION

    In conclusion, our observations on the expression of reelin-

    positive cells accompanying the developmental emer-gence of behavioural decits during isolation rearing may

    Merged Reelin 20 m

    Merged Reelin 20 m

    Reelin-expressinghorizontal cells

    Reelin-expressingvertical cells

    A B

    C D

    Layer I

    Layer II

    Reelin-expressing

    vertical cell

    F

    Postnatal day40 60 80

    *20

    40

    60

    80

    100

    R e e

    l i n p o s

    i t i v e c e

    l l s

    / m m

    2

    120

    GReelin-expressing

    horizontal cells

    *

    *

    20

    40

    60

    80

    100

    R e e

    l i n p o s

    i t i v e c e

    l l s / m

    m 2

    120

    Postnatal day40 60 80

    HReelin-expressing

    vertical cells

    Fig. 4. Inuence of isolation rearing on the density of two separate reelin-expressing cell populations in layer I of the prefrontal cortex of Wistar ratsat increasing age. Cells expressing reelin immunoreactivity in a horizontal and vertical manner are shown in Panels AD and the position of the verticalcells in relation to layer II is shown in Panels E and F. The reelin-expressing cells in Panels A, C and F are counter-stained with NeuroTraceuorescent Nissl stain and Hoechst 33258. Panel E is a phase contrast image. Quantitation of two separate reelin-expressing cell populations in layer I of the prefrontal cortex is shown in Panels G and H and values are expressed as the mean SEM and signicant differences between the isolationreared (lled circles) and social cohorts (open circles) are shown with an asterisk ( P 0.05, two-tailed unpaired Students t -test). For interpretation of the references to color in this gure legend, the reader is referred to the Web version of this article.

    A. W. Cassidy et al. / Neuroscience 166 (2010) 377385 383

  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    9/10

    Author's personal copy

    provide a basis for the structural decits observed in neu-ropsychiatric conditions, however, as yet, the physiologicaltrigger that leads to reelin over-expression at postnatal day60 remains to be established in this animal model.

    AcknowledgmentsThe Applied Neurotherapeutics ResearchGroup is a Strategic Research Cluster funded jointly by ScienceFoundation Ireland (07/IN.1/B1322) and Wyeth Discovery.

    REFERENCES Aguil A, Schwartz TH, Kumar VS, Peterlin ZA, Tsiola A, Soriano E,

    Yuste R (1999) Involvement of Cajal-Retzius neurons in sponta-neous correlated activity of embryonic and postnatal layer 1 fromwild-type and reeler mice. J Neurosci 19:1085610868.

    Akbarian S, Kim JJ, Potkin SG, Hagman JO, Taffazzoli A, Bunney WEJr, Jones EG (1995) Gene expression for glutamic acid decarbox-ylase is reduced without loss of neurons in prefrontal cortex of

    schizophrenics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52:258266. Alcntara S, Ruiz M, DArcangelo G, Ezan F, de Lecea L, Curran T,

    Sotelo C, Soriano E (1998) Regional and cellular patterns of reelinmRNA expression in the forebrain of the developing and adultmouse. J Neurosci 18:77797799.

    Ashley-Koch AE, Jaworski J, Ma de Q, Mei H, Ritchie MD, Skaar DA,Robert Delong G, Worley G, Abramson RK, Wright HH, CuccaroML, Gilbert JR, Martin ER, Pericak-Vance MA (2007) Investigationof potential gene-gene interactions between APOE and RELNcontributing to autism risk. Psychiatr Genet 17:221226.

    Bacon SJ, Headlam AJ, Gabbott PL, Smith AD (1996) Amygdala inputto medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in the rat: a light and electronmicroscope study. Brain Res 720:211219.

    Bakshi VP, Geyer MA (1999) Ontogeny of isolation rearing-induceddecits in sensorimotor gating in rats. Physiol Behav 67:385392.

    Beasley CL, Reynolds GP (1997) Parvalbumin-immunoreactive neu-rones are reduced in the prefrontal cortex of schizophrenia.Schizophr Res 24:349355.

    Benes FM, McSparren J, Bird ED, SanGiovanni JP, Vincent SL (1991)Decits in small interneurons in prefrontal and cingulated corticesof schizophrenic and schizoaffective patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry48:9961001.

    Benes FM, Berretta S (2001) GABAergic interneurons: implications for understanding schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropsycho-pharmacology 25:127.

    Bloomeld C, French SJ, Jones DN, Reavill C, Southam E, Cilia J,Totterdell S (2008) Chandelier cartridges in the prefrontal cortexare reduced in isolation reared rats. Synapse 62:628631.

    Boqoch Y, Linial M (2008) Coordinated expression of cytoskeletonregulating genes in the accelerated neurite outgrowth of P19 em-bryonic carcinoma cells. Exp Cell Res 314:677690.

    Borrell V, Del Rio JA, Alcantara S, Derer M, Martinez A, DArcangeloG, Nakajima K, Mikoshiba K, Derer P, Curran T, Soriano E (1999)Reelin regulates the development and synaptogenesis of the layer-specic entorhino-hippocampal connections. J Neurosci 19:1345 1358.

    Braff D, Geyer MA (1990) Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia: humanand animal model studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47:181188.

    Braun I, Genius J, Grunze H, Bender A, Mller H-J, Rujescu D (2007) Alterations in hippocampal and prefrontal GABAergic interneuronsin an animal model of psychosis induced by NMDA receptor an-tagonism. Schizophr Res 97:254263.

    Chance SA, Walker M, Crow TJ (2005) Reduced density of calbindin-immunoreactive interneurons in the planum temporale in schizo-phrenia. Brain Res 1046:3237.

    Costa E, Davis J, Grayson DR, Guidotti A, Pappas GD, Pesold C(2001) Dendritic spine hypoplasticity and downregulation of reelinand GABAergic tone in schizophrenia vulnerability. Neurobiol Dis8:723742.

    Cunningham MG, Bhattacharyya S, Benes FM (2002) Increasing in-teractions of amygdalar afferents with GABAergic interneuronsbetween birth and adulthood. J Comp Neurol 453:116130.

    Cunningham MG, Bhattacharyya S, Benes FM (2008) Increasing in-

    teraction of amygdalar afferents with GABAergic interneurons be-tween birth and adulthood. Cereb Cortex 18:15291535.

    DArcangelo G, Miao GG, Chen SC, Soares HD, Morgan JI, Curran T(1995) A protein related to extracellular matrix proteins deleted inthe mouse mutant reeler. Nature 374:719723.

    Day-Wilson KM, Jones DN, Southam E, Cilia J, Totterdell S (2006)Medial prefrontal cortex volume loss in rats with isolation rearing-induced decits in prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. Neuro-science 141:11131121.

    Domeney A, Feldon J (1998) The disruption of prepulse inhibition bysocial isolation in the Wistar rat: how robust is the effect? Phar-macol Biochem Behav 59:883890.

    Dong E, Caruncho H, Liu WS, Smalheiser NR, Grayson DR, Costa E,Guidotti A (2003) A reelin-integrin receptor interaction regulatesarc mRNA translation in synaptoneurosomes. Proc Natl Acad Sci

    U S A 100:54795484.Endo K, Hori T, Abe S, Asada T (2007) Alterations in GABA(A)receptor expression in neonatal ventral hippocampal lesioned rats:comparison of prepubertal and postpubertal periods. Synapse61:357366.

    Fatemi SH, Earle JA, McMenomy T (2000) Reduction in reelin immu-noreactivity in hippocampus of subjects with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depression. Mol Psychiatry 5:654663.

    Fatemi SH, Snow AV, Stary JM, Araghi-Niknam M, Reutiman TJ, LeeS, Brooks AI, Pearce DA (2005) Reelin signaling is impaired inautism. Biol Psychiatry 57:777787.

    Fone KCF, Porkess MV (2008) Behavioural and neurochemical effects of post-weaning social isolation in rodentsrelevance to developmentalneuropsychiatric disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:10871102.

    Fountoulakis KN, Giannakopoulos P, Kvari E, Bouras C (2008) As-sessing the role of the cingulate cortex in bipolar disorder: neuro-pathological, structural and functional imaging data. Brain Res Rev59:921.

    Gabbott PLA, Dickie BGM, Vaid R, Headlam AJN, Bacon SJ (1997)Local-circuit neurons in the medial prefrontal cortex (areas 25, 32and 24b) in the rat: morphology and quantitative distribution.J Comp Neurol 377:465499.

    Geyer MA, Krebs-Thomson K, Braff DL, Swerdlow NR (1993) Phar-macological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating decits in schizophrenia: a decade in review. Biol Psychiatry34:361372.

    Glantz LA, Lewis DA (1997) Reduction of synaptophysin immunore-activity in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with schizophrenia.Regionaland diagnostic specicity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:660669.

    Gogtay N, Giedd NJ, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC,Nugent TF 3rd, Herman DH, Clasen LS, Toga AW, Rapoport JL,Thompson PM (2004) Dynamic mapping of human cortical devel-opment during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl AcadSci U S A 101:81748179.

    Gray JA (1998) Integrating schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 24:249266.Gray JA, Kumari V, Lawrence N, Young AJM (1999) Functions of the

    dopaminergic innervation of the nucleus accumbens. Psychobiol-ogy 27:225235.

    Grayson DR, Jia X, Chen Y, Sharma RP, Mitchell CP, Guidotti A,Costa E (2005) Reelin promoter hypermethylation in schizophre-nia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:93419346.

    Guidotti A, Auta J, Davis JM, Di-Giorgi-Gerevini V, Dwivedi Y, GraysonDR, Impagnatiello F, Pandey G, Pesold C, Sharma R, Uzunov D,Costa E (2000) Decrease in reelin and glutamic acid decarboxylase67(GAD67) expression in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: apostmortem brain study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57:10611069.

    Heidbreder CA, Weiss IC, Domeney AM, Pryce C, Homberg J, HedouG, Feldon J, Moran MC, Nelson P (2001) Behavioral, neurochem-

    A. W. Cassidy et al. / Neuroscience 166 (2010) 377385384

  • 7/27/2019 Cassidy Et Al., 2010_ Reelin PFC

    10/10

    Author's personal copy

    ical and endocrinological characterization of the early socialisolation syndrome. Neuroscience 100:749768.

    Hestrin S, Armstrong WE (1996) Morphology and physiology of corti-cal neurons in layer I. J Neurosci 16:52905300.

    Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS (1997) Regional differences in syn-aptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol 387:167 178.

    Ignatova N, Sindic CJ, Gofnet AM (2004) Characterization of the variousforms of the reelin protein in the cerebrospinal uid of normal subjectsand in neurological disease. Neurobiol Dis 15:326330.

    Impagnatiello F, Guidotti AR, Pesold C, Dwivedi Y, Caruncho H, PisuMG, Uzunov DP, Smalheiser NR, Davis JM, Pandey GN, PappasGD, Tueting P, Sharma RP, Costa E (1998) A decrease of reelinexpression as a putative vulnerability factor in schizophrenia. ProcNatl Acad Sci U S A 95:1571815723.

    Jay TM, Witter MP (1991) Distribution of hippocampal CA1 and sub-icular efferents in the prefrontal cortex of the rat studied by meansof anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin.J Comp Neurol 313:574586.

    Jossin Y, Ignatova N, Hiesberger T, Herz J, Lambert de Rouvroit C,Gofnet AM (2004) The central fragment of reelin, generated byproteolytic processing in vivo , is critical to its function during corti-cal plate development. J Neurosci 24:514521.

    Lambert de Rouvroit C, de Bergeyck V, Cortvrindt C, Bar I, EeckhoutY, Gofnet AM (1999) Reelin, the extracellular matrix protein de-cient in reeler mutant mice, is processed by a metalloproteinase.Exp Neurol 156:214217.

    Lewis DA, Gonzlez-Burgos G (2008) Neuroplasticity of neocorticalcircuits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:141165.

    Lewis DA, Hashimoto T, Volk DW (2005) Cortical inhibitory neuronsand schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:312324.

    Lipska BK, Jaskiw GE, Weinberger DR (1993) Postpubertal emer-gence of hyperresponsiveness to stress and to amphetamine after neonatal excitotoxic hippocampal damage: a potential animalmodel of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 9:6775.

    Lipska BK, Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Jaskiw GE, Braff DL, Wein-berger DR (1995) Neonatal excitotoxic hippocampal damage inrats causes post-pubertal changes in prepulse inhibition of star-tle and its disruption by apomorphine. Psychopharmacology122:3543.

    Lipska BK, Lerman DN, Khaing ZZ, Weickert CS, Weinberger DR(2003) Gene expression in dopamine and GABA systems in ananimal model of schizophrenia: effects of antipsychotic drugs. Eur J Neurosci 18:391402.

    Lisman JE, Coyle JT, Green RW, Javitt DC, Benes FM, Heckers S,Grace AG (2008) Circuit-based framework for understanding neu-rotransmitter and risk gene interactions in schizophrenia. TrendsNeurosci 31:234242.

    Oblak A, Gibbs TT, Blatt GJ (2009) Decreased GABA A receptors andbenzodiazepine binding sites in the anterior cingulate cortex inautism. Autism Res 2:205219.

    Pappas GD, Kriho V, Pesold C (2002) Reelin in the extracellular matrixand dendritic spines of the cortex and hippocampus: a comparisonbetween wild type and heterozygous reeler mice by immu-noelectron microscopy. J Neurocytol 30:413425.

    Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN (2008) Why do many neuropsychiatricdisorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci 9:947957.

    Paxinos G, Watson C (1986) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates,2nd ed. New York: Academic Press.

    Penschuck S, Flagstad P, Didriksen M, Leist M, Michael-Titus AT(2006) Decrease in parvalbumin-expressing neurons in the hip-pocampus and increased phencyclidine-induced locomotor activity

    in the rat methylazoxymethanol (MAM) model of schizophrenia.Eur J Neurosci 23:279284.

    Pesold C, Impagnatiello F, Pisu MG, Uzunov DP, Costa E, Guidotti A,Caruncho HJ (1998) Reelin is preferentially expressed in neurons

    synthesizing -aminobutyric acid in cortex and hippocampus of adult rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:32213226.

    Pesold C, Liu WS, Guidotti A, Costa E, Caruncho HJ (1999) Corticalbitufted, horizontal, and Martinotti cells preferentially express andsecrete reelin in perineuronal nets, postsynaptically modulatinggene expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:32173222.

    Pierri JN, Chaudry AS, Woo TU, Lewis DA (1999) Alterations inchandelier neuron axon terminals in the prefrontal cortex of schizo-phrenic subjects. Am J Psychiatry 156:17091719.

    Pillai-Nair N, Panicker AK, Rodriguiz RM, Gilmore KL, DemyanenkoGP, Huang JZ, Wetzel WC, Maness PF (2005) Neural cell adhe-sion molecule-secreting transgenic mice display abnormalities inGABAergic interneurons and alterations in behaviour. J Neurosci25:46594671.

    Sakai T, Oshima A, Nozaki Y, Ida I, Haga C, Akiyama H, Nakazato Y,

    Mikuni M (2008) Changes in the density of calcium-binding-protein-immunoreactive GABAergic interneurons in prefrontal cortex of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropathology 28:143150.

    Selemon LD, Goldman-Rakic P (1999) The reduced neuropil hypoth-esis: a circuit based model of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry47:681683.

    Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Taaid N, Geyer MA (1994) Assessing thevalidity of an animal model of decient sensorimotor gating inschizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:139154.

    Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL (2001) Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: current knowledge andfuture challenges. Psychopharmacology 156:194215.

    Tissir F, Gofnet AM (2003) Reelin and brain development. Nat RevNeurosci 4:496505.

    Torrey EF, Barci BM, Webster MJ, Bartko JJ, Meador-Woodruff JH,Knable MB (2005) Neurochemical markers for schizophrenia, bi-polar disorder, and major depression in post-mortem brains. BiolPsychiatry 57:252260.

    Trommsdorff M, Gotthardt M, Heisberger T, Shelton J, Stockinger W,Nimpf J, Hammer RE, Richardson JA, Herz J (1999) Reeler/dis-abled-like disruption of neuronal migration in knock out mice lack-ing the VLDL receptor and ApoE receptor 2. Cell 97:689701.

    Tseng KY, Lewis BL, Hashimoto T, Sesack SR, Kloc M, Lewis DA,ODonnell P (2008) A neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion causesfunctional decits in adult prefrontal cortical interneurons. J Neu-rosci 28:1269112699.

    Volk DW, Pierri JN, Fritschy JM, Auh S, Sampson AR, Lewis DA(2002) Reciprocal alterations in pre- and postsynaptic inhibitorymarkers at chandelier cell inputs to pyramidal neurons in schizo-phrenia. Cereb Cortex 12:10631070.

    Weeber EJ, Beffert U, Jones C, Christian JM, Forster E, Sweatt JD,

    Herz J (2002) Reelin and ApoE receptors cooperate to enhancehippocampal synaptic plasticity and learning. J Biol Chem 277:3994439952.

    Woo TU, Walsh JP, Benes FM (2004) Density of glutamic acid decar-boxylase 67 messenger RNA-containing neurons that express theN-methyl- D-aspartate receptor sub-unit NR2A in the anterior cin-gulate cortex in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Arch GenPsychiatry 61:649 657.

    Woo TU, Whitehead RE, Melchitzky DS, Lewis DA (1998) A subclassof prefrontal gamma-aminobutyric acid axon terminals are selec-tively altered in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A95:53415346.

    (Accepted 16 December 2009)(Available online 24 December 2009)

    A. W. Cassidy et al. / Neuroscience 166 (2010) 377385 385