Bilingual education for a monolingual test

19
ORIGINAL PAPER A bilingual education for a monolingual test? The pressure to prepare for TAKS and its influence on choices for language of instruction in Texas elementary bilingual classrooms Deborah Palmer Anissa Wicktor Lynch Received: 1 September 2007 / Accepted: 9 June 2008 / Published online: 19 July 2008 Ó Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2008 Abstract A tension exists for teachers in Texas bilingual third and fifth grade classrooms between state and local bilingual education policy, which encourages them to transition students gradually from Spanish into English instruction while providing bilingual support; and state and federal accountability policy, which requires them to choose a single language for each child’s high-stakes test. Inter- view data from teachers in six Texas elementary schools suggest that the high-stakes Texas Assessment of Knowledge and Skills (TAKS), a test offered in both English and Spanish in 3rd–6th grades and used for school and district rankings at both state and federal levels, drives teachers’ decisions with regards to language of instruction for their students. We argue that children who test in Spanish will be taught in Spanish, with little attention to the transition process until the testing pressures are lifted; children who test in English will be taught in English, with little attention to the support in their primary language that may determine their ability to succeed on a test in their second language. Keywords Bilingual education Á High-stakes testing Á No child left behind Á Teacher sense-making Á Transition Á Testing accommodations Á English language learners Introduction There is a growing body of knowledge exploring the ways in which schools and communities negotiate externally imposed policy (Darling-Hammond and Millman D. Palmer (&) Á A. W. Lynch University of Texas at Austin, 1 University Station, Mailcode: D5700, Austin, TX 78712, USA e-mail: [email protected] A. W. Lynch e-mail: [email protected] 123 Lang Policy (2008) 7:217–235 DOI 10.1007/s10993-008-9100-0

description

 

Transcript of Bilingual education for a monolingual test

Page 1: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

ORI GIN AL PA PER

A bilingual education for a monolingual test?The pressure to prepare for TAKS and its influenceon choices for language of instruction in Texaselementary bilingual classrooms

Deborah Palmer Æ Anissa Wicktor Lynch

Received: 1 September 2007 / Accepted: 9 June 2008 / Published online: 19 July 2008

� Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2008

Abstract A tension exists for teachers in Texas bilingual third and fifth grade

classrooms between state and local bilingual education policy, which encourages

them to transition students gradually from Spanish into English instruction while

providing bilingual support; and state and federal accountability policy, which

requires them to choose a single language for each child’s high-stakes test. Inter-

view data from teachers in six Texas elementary schools suggest that the high-stakes

Texas Assessment of Knowledge and Skills (TAKS), a test offered in both English

and Spanish in 3rd–6th grades and used for school and district rankings at both state

and federal levels, drives teachers’ decisions with regards to language of instruction

for their students. We argue that children who test in Spanish will be taught in

Spanish, with little attention to the transition process until the testing pressures are

lifted; children who test in English will be taught in English, with little attention to

the support in their primary language that may determine their ability to succeed on

a test in their second language.

Keywords Bilingual education � High-stakes testing � No child left behind �Teacher sense-making � Transition � Testing accommodations � English language

learners

Introduction

There is a growing body of knowledge exploring the ways in which schools and

communities negotiate externally imposed policy (Darling-Hammond and Millman

D. Palmer (&) � A. W. Lynch

University of Texas at Austin, 1 University Station, Mailcode: D5700, Austin, TX 78712, USA

e-mail: [email protected]

A. W. Lynch

e-mail: [email protected]

123

Lang Policy (2008) 7:217–235

DOI 10.1007/s10993-008-9100-0

Page 2: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

1990; Tyack and Cuban 1995; Clotfelter and Ladd 1996; Spillane 2004).

Scholarship increasingly demonstrates that schools, far from transparently convey-

ing reforms, serve as ‘‘buffered institutions’’ interpreting and remaking reforms

within their particular contexts. Further, professionals within the institution, both

teachers and principals, play an important role in resisting, adapting or appropriating

elements of reform (Alamillo and Viramontes 2000; Coburn 2001; Stritikus and

Garcia 2000). Particularly the work of teachers, which entails the actual delivery of

instruction to children, is what Cochran-Smith termed ‘‘unforgivably complex’’

(Cochran-Smith 2003). As a result, attempts to change educational practice that do

not directly address teachers’ work, do not always have the intended outcomes.

Attempts to understand the impact of certain reforms are only as valuable as the

conceptions of teaching and learning upon which they are based.

Given the increasing recognition of the importance and the complexity of the role

of teachers in enacting education reform initiatives, it is not surprising to note that

there has also been significant work to understand the decision-making process of

teachers. One school of thought known as sense-making theory, which emerges out of

the field of organizational science, takes a close look at individuals’ processes as they

go about making sense of implausible occurrences in their institutional/political

contexts (Weick 1995; Westrum 1982; Porac et al. 1989). In their struggle to resolve

contradictions in the environment around them, individuals will manipulate messages

and pressures, sometimes combining divergent aspects, sometimes adapting them

to suit their needs, sometimes ignoring them altogether. Individuals will go to

extraordinary lengths to resolve contradiction, particularly when they face contra-

diction in places such as a workplace where they have relatively little power. Studies

of teachers’ sense-making of externally imposed school reform have focused both on

individual sense-making as described above (Jennings 1996; Spillane and Jennings

1997) and a more collaborative process of group sense-making (Spillane 1999;

Coburn 2001).

This article documents the results of our attempt to further explore the role of

teachers, particularly elementary school teachers, in enacting policy reforms in a

specific and highly contradictory context, serving bilingual children in a large urban

school district in central Texas. Similar to school districts throughout the US, this

district of approximately 82,000 students struggles to serve a large and growing

population of English language learners (ELLs); approximately 20,000, or 25%, of

the district’s students are ELLs, of which 93% speak Spanish as their first language.

State and district policy call for Transitional Bilingual Education to serve

Spanish-speaking ELLs in the elementary grades, in which children are taught basic

subjects in their primary language during their first few years of schooling as they

learn English, and are gradually transitioned into all-English instruction. Transi-

tional bilingual programs are characterized by the use of both students’ primary

language and English to varying degrees for instruction. It is common in such

programs for students to be instructed in early literacy in their primary language.

The goals of transitional bilingual programs are for ELLs to acquire English without

falling behind academically, to become proficient in both their native language and

English, and to exit into English mainstream instruction as soon as they are

proficient enough in English (Crawford 2004).

218 D. Palmer, A. W. Lynch

123

Page 3: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

While enforcement statewide is lax, support is relatively broad for bilingual

programs; in the district under study, schools vary in their implementation of

bilingual education, but all schools with significant populations of ELLs have at

least cursory programs. Teachers are encouraged by district personnel to use

primary language instruction with young children and to gradually transition

children to English instruction by the upper elementary grades. In most district

schools, third grade is considered the ‘‘transition year,’’ in which successful learners

make the move from Spanish into English instruction. This is complicated by the

fact that some immigrant children enter US schools in later grades, but the district

does not offer primary language support after fifth grade; thus, fifth grade is viewed

as the final cut-off in terms of transition.

Simultaneously, Texas third and fifth grade teachers must contend with another

major policy imposition: the Texas Assessment of Knowledge and Skills (TAKS),

which is administered annually to students in third through eighth grades. In the

elementary grades 3–5, students all take the TAKS reading and math tests. In fourth

grade they also take the writing test, and in fifth grade they take the science test.

TAKS tests carry high stakes at the federal, state, and local levels. They are used to

rank and assign ratings to schools statewide, a system that carries consequences for

poor performance including eventual state takeover. Further, these tests are used to

determine schools’ Adequate Yearly Progress (AYP)1 for federal No Child LeftBehind (NCLB) mandates (No Child Left Behind 2000), which also include

consequences for failure to perform and improve. In third and fifth grades, certain

TAKS tests (e.g. reading in third grade, reading and math in fifth) are also used to

determine whether children will be permitted to advance to the next grade, with

grade retention looming as a threat over children who fail to achieve passing scores.

While all TAKS tests are available in Spanish for grades three through six, teachers

must decide well in advance in which language students will test, with all of the

consequences this entails. Because of the well-documented negative consequences

of grade retention, including dramatically increased high school drop-out rates and

reduced educational attainment (Roderick 1994; Rumberger 1995), the outcomes

resulting from teachers’ decisions, both for instruction and for testing, are vitally

important to the life-chances of ELLs.

A clear tension, if not outright contradiction, emerges for teachers, particularly in

third and fifth grades, when making decisions for their students about language for

instruction and for testing. While bilingual programs encourage children to

transition gradually into English during these grades, testing mandates require

children to test entirely in one language only—and many pressures exist to make

that language English. We were interested in what factors teachers considered when

they made and carried out these difficult language decisions for their students:

which aspects of the various policy impositions did they pay closest attention to, and

1 Adequate Yearly Progress (AYP) is an individual state’s measure of progress toward the goal of 100%

of students achieving to state academic standards in at least reading/language arts and math. It sets the

minimum level of proficiency that the state, its school districts, and schools must achieve each year on

annual tests and related academic indicators. Parents whose children are attending Title I (low-income)

schools that do not make AYP over a period of years are given options to transfer their child to another

school or obtain free tutoring (supplemental educational services).

Bilingual education for a monolingual test 219

123

Page 4: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

which did they adapt, combine, manipulate or ignore? Our main research question,

and the one that will be addressed in this article, is: how did teachers decide in

which language to instruct their students?

Since all of the schools in this study served mainly low socioeconomic

communities (see school profiles below for more details), it is important to note that

there is some evidence that the overall socioeconomic level of a school impacts the

level of opportunity and freedom of the teachers to make appropriate pedagogical

choices for their students. In other words, in schools with wealthier student bodies,

teachers are given more freedom to do what they believe is right for children, while

teachers of poor children must follow institutional mandates more strictly

(Goldstein 2007; Orfield and Lee 2005). In any case, teaching at the elementary

level is always a complex endeavor: ‘‘Contradiction, tension, inconsistency, and

uncertainty, while difficult to manage, are a non-negotiable part of teaching young

children’’ (Goldstein 2007, p. 52).

Literature review

English language learners, like all students throughout the United States, are

expected to excel on single-measure high-stakes tests such as the TAKS.

Intensifying pressure is placed on schools and students to show ‘‘Adequate Yearly

Progress’’ in order to comply with federal mandates (No Child Left Behind 2000) or

face serious monetary and educational consequences. In many cases, school

curricula and teachers’ choices of pedagogy have been altered in an attempt to

prepare students for the tests (Au 2007). ‘‘Fairness aside, high-stakes testing has

radically altered the kind of instruction that is offered to the point that teaching to

the test has become a prominent part of the nation’s educational landscape’’ (Kohn

2000, p. 20).

ELLs face a particular challenge: to perform in a language that they by definition

do not master. Students who are limited in English are unlikely to be able to

demonstrate their content knowledge on a test written in English; both linguistic

structures and vocabulary are often laborious (Abedi 2004). In fact, ELLs in general

score lower than non-ELL students on tests, but the so-called ‘‘achievement gap’’ is

widest in content areas with high language demands, such as reading (Abedi 2002).

In essence, it is clear that tests administered to ELLs in English lack validity and

reliability because they inadvertently test ELLs’ English language knowledge. NoChild Left Behind (2000) requires that states assess ELLs in a ‘‘valid and reliable

manner’’ and specifies that ELLs be provided ‘‘reasonable accommodations’’ in

order to effectively demonstrate their knowledge. The purpose of all accommoda-

tions is to improve the performance of ELLs by helping them overcome any

language barriers that might exist, either by modifying materials so that the student

has a better understanding of them, or by altering the administration of the test to

give the student more time or access to tools that might help bridge language gaps.

Yet some research is demonstrating that conflicting state and federal policies in

essence nullify many if not all of the testing accommodations designed to help ELLs

perform on tests (Wright 2005).

220 D. Palmer, A. W. Lynch

123

Page 5: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

The federal government mandates that states must provide accommodations to

ELLs in the form of ‘‘testing students in their native language’’ to the ‘‘extent

practicable,’’ yet no method for enforcement of this measure was included to ensure

that ELLs receive any testing accommodations (No Child Left Behind 2000).

Accommodations designed to aid ELLs have been shown to give a more accurate

measure of the type of content knowledge ELLs possess (Linn and Gronlund 2000;

Rivera and Stansfield 1998). ELLs benefit from versions of tests that have been

modified to reduce their linguistic complexity, and are able to score slightly higher

than on an unmodified version of the test (Abedi and Lord 2001; Cummins et al.

1998; De Corte et al. 1985; Hudson 1983; Riley et al. 1983). Allowing more time

for tests is another promising accommodation since research has shown that ELLs

read more slowly (Mestre 1998). Spanish language versions of many achievement

tests are available to measure the content areas for ELLs who are Spanish speakers.

Perhaps because they are more expensive and labor-intensive to carry out, language-

based accommodations such as translation of a test into a students’ native language,

a bilingual version of the test, or a modified English version are less commonly used

accommodations (Abedi et al. 2004).

As noted above, Texas provides translated Spanish language versions of the

TAKS as an accommodation for ELLs in grades three through six (Rivera and

Collum 2006). Translated tests are not without controversy since it is difficult to

translate a test into a version that takes into account the multiple dialects spoken by

students, thus limiting the appropriateness of the translated test (Olsen and

Goldstein 1997). Test translation is time consuming, technically difficult and

expensive. In addition, there is always a risk that the content and construct of the

two tests will differ (Kopriva 2000). In fact, a review prepared by the National

Assessment Governing Board addressing the accommodations available to ELL

students in the Voluntary National Test, identified validity problems with native-

language testing as a result of the issues involved with the translation of construct

equivalence (American Institutes for Research 1999).

Schools have the possibility of avoiding the issues of translated tests by

developing Spanish-language achievement tests that are not translations of English-

language tests. Spanish-language versions offer norm-referenced and criterion-

referenced score reports that test various subjects such as reading/language arts,

mathematics, listening and English in most cases (Linn and Gronlund 2000).

Accommodations on tests have only proven to be effective if they match the

types of accommodations regularly used by classroom teachers during instruction,

some research suggests (Rivera and Stansfield 1998). Native language accommo-

dations are useful when students have received native-language instruction in the

content areas being tested. Likewise ELLs who have learned subject-area-specific

academic vocabulary in English because they received instruction in English should

be tested in English even when a test in their native language exists (Abedi et al.

2004).

A noteworthy consequence of high-stakes testing for ELLs is that teachers tend to

‘‘teach to the test,’’ or be pressured to alter their instruction to improve test scores in

lieu of providing instruction designed to meet the linguistic and cultural demands of

their student population (Alamillo et al. 2005; Wright 2002). The result is a

Bilingual education for a monolingual test 221

123

Page 6: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

narrowing of the curriculum in which non-tested subjects are eliminated or

deemphasized (Wright and Choi 2005). States and districts even go so far as to

mandate changes to the curriculum intended to improve student performance on

tests by adopting new materials and producing comprehensive pacing guides. These

guides ensure that teachers stay on track and expose students to the materials that

will be on the test before it is administered. Some of these pacing guides squeeze in

such an enormous amount of information that teachers teach more material in less

time than ever before, essentially eliminating the possibility of modifying

instruction to meet students’ needs.

Wright and Choi’s (2005) survey of experienced third-grade ELL teachers in

Arizona found that the majority of teachers did not feel that high-stakes testing had

improved the quality of teaching and learning in their classroom, nor did these

teachers feel that the tests had helped them to become more effective teachers of

ELLs. Rather, teachers felt that the testing pressures had resulted in a reduction in

the use of effective instructional practices for ELLs and an increase in less effective

practices. Teachers felt extremely pressured to prepare ELL students for high-stakes

tests in English in spite of their lack of faith in the appropriateness and validity of

the tests. Au’s (2007) qualitative metasynthesis of studies on the impact of high-

stakes testing on curriculum found that while testing very much drives curricular

choices, it appears that the structure of the tests themselves has a great deal to do

with the ways in which tests will impact curriculum. A multiple-choice test such as

the TAKS, which tests a specific body of knowledge, appears to narrow the

curriculum and fragment the presentation of subject area knowledge, leading

teachers to increase the use of teacher centered pedagogies. All of these changes

will negatively impact ELLs, who thrive on student-centered, thematic/holistic rich

curriculum (Echevarria et al. 2006). Thus, the impact of high-stakes tests on

curriculum could have life-long consequences for ELLs in Texas, resulting from a

less rich curriculum and a decrease in the English language skills necessary for

academic achievement.

Curriculum, pedagogic, or programmatic changes as schools confront the reality

of student performance on a standardized test is of extreme importance. The impact

of tests on teaching and learning, known as ‘‘washback’’ (Cheng et al. 2004; Wall

1997), could theoretically be positive; assessment can and should drive instruction

in schooling. However, in this case, we have cause for concern. Perhaps the most

worrisome aspect of this rapid change in curricula for ELLs is the dismantling of

bilingual programs. While several recent comprehensive studies pointed to the

efficacy of primary language instruction for ELLs (Rolstad et al. 2005; Genesee

et al. 2006; Francis et al. 2006; Slavin and Cheung 2005), findings in California, a

state that mandates English-only high-stakes testing beginning in second grade,

indicated that schools largely modified their bilingual programs to cut down on the

amount of time spent in a language other than English, or simply did away with their

bilingual programs altogether (Alamillo et al. 2005) under the weight of high-stakes

testing pressures.

Research does not support the practice of swapping out programs with native

language components to programs in which English is the language of instruction.

Greene (1997) determined that the use of at least some native language instruction

222 D. Palmer, A. W. Lynch

123

Page 7: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

was more likely to help the average ELL student on standardized tests in English

than the use of only English for instruction. Rolstad et al. (2005) found the practice

of banning or discouraging the use of native language instruction was not justified,

and that the rapid transition to English that federal policies tend to favor is ill

advised. According to a well-established research base, programs that provide

English-only instruction do not accelerate the learning of English and may in fact

place students in such programs at risk of falling further behind their English

speaking peers (Krashen and McField 2005; Ramirez 1992).

In the high-stakes environment in Texas, where transitional bilingual education is

currently mandated, a translated test in Spanish is the primary accommodation made

for ELLs. One might at first assume that, while curriculum may narrow and teachers

may ‘‘teach to the test,’’ at least the bilingual programs are safe from erosion. Yet,

evidence from this study suggests that even with efforts to accommodate testing

with primary language supports, the tendency of high stakes to distort teachers’

instructional choices and impact their professional judgment nonetheless erodes

bilingual programs. This corroborates Menken’s (2008) findings in New York that

in spite of test translations, the pressure to succeed in high-stakes testing imposes

monolingual policy in bilingual classrooms.

Methods: data collection

This study aimed to explore teachers’ sense-making in response to a series of imposed

alterations of their practices and programs. We conducted ethnographic interviews

with the third and fifth grade bilingual teachers at six schools. Ethnographic interviews

were open-ended, allowing us to understand the ways that teachers of second language

learners constructed their understandings of bilingual education, literacy, and the

various policies that district, state, and national entities had imposed to direct their

interpretations of these constructs. The goal of open-ended interviews is to understand

participants’ framing of the events and realities under study, and for this reason to the

extent possible we allowed participants to direct the interview topics (Mischler 1991;

Rubin and Rubin 1995). However, certain guiding questions helped shape our

interviews and start participants off (see Appendix I).

Originally, the interview questions were designed to explore areas of congruence

and tension between local policy initiatives and state and national initiatives, and

how these various mandates came together in the classroom to influence teachers’

decision making. Local initiatives included a new policy of early-exit transitional

bilingual education for English learners in the elementary grades that the school

district was calling ‘‘RISE,’’ or Rigorous Instruction in Spanish and English, and a

district-wide push for the use of what the University of Pittsburgh’s ‘‘Principles of

Learning’’ called ‘‘Accountable Talk’’ (Michaels et al. 2002). Testing and

accountability measures, in particular the reading TAKS, were the main focus at

the state and national level. Between study design and the time the interviews were

conducted, district leadership experienced a shift and the ‘‘RISE’’ program was

under scrutiny. Rumor had it throughout the district that the program would be

disappearing as quickly as it had arrived, and thus our participants spoke more about

Bilingual education for a monolingual test 223

123

Page 8: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

what they actually did in the classroom and how they conceived of their bilingual

education model than they did about the impact ‘‘RISE’’ was having on their

decisions. This turned out to be a valuable turn of events, as it was more revealing in

terms of teacher sense-making.

Six schools were selected (a purposive sample) based on the level of support and

understanding the principal provided for bilingual education. In other words, two of

the schools had principals who were highly knowledgeable and supportive of

bilingual education. Two of the schools had principals who were relatively neutral

or noncommittal in the area of bilingual education, and two had principals who were

unsupportive of bilingual education and/or lacked knowledge of how best to serve

bilingual students. School administrators influence the school climate as well as

teacher morale and professionalism. Interviews with the principals provided insight

on the ways school leaders’ personal philosophies affected how teachers were able

to use information to make decisions. Schools were selected with the assistance of

the district’s director of bilingual education, and a well-respected bilingual principal

in the district, who were both also interviewed as part of the project. Because all

principals were consulted—and interviewed—before their teachers were contacted,

our method of selection is a variation on the ‘‘snowball’’ method, in which each

participant is asked to recommend others to participate (Denzin and Lincoln 1998).

See Table 1 (below) for demographic details on selected schools.

For our teacher interviews, we chose to focus on grades three and five because of

the confluence in these grades, explained above, of mandates emerging both from

bilingual education policy and assessment/accountability demands. We hypothe-

sized that teachers in these grades would have strong views and much to contribute

to a conversation about local, state, and national policies and the ways these have

impacted their teaching.

Thus, during the spring of 2006, interviews were conducted one-on-one with the

district director of bilingual education, with the six principals, and with a total of 16

teachers. Of the 16 teachers interviewed for this study, nine were third grade

teachers, one was a forth grade teacher, and six were fifth grade teachers. There was

never more than one-fifth grade teacher at any single school. Average length of

interviews was approximately 45 min. Interviews were digitally audio-recorded and

transcribed.

Table 1 Participating schools%

LEP

Economically

disadvantaged

# Tchr

interviews

School 1 58.3 92.2 4

School 2 35.7 93.5 1

School 3 42.3 90 3

School 4 49.8 94.4 3

School 5 54.0 94.4 3

School 6 44.3 91.5 2

District 23.6 60.3 16

State 15.8 55.6

224 D. Palmer, A. W. Lynch

123

Page 9: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

Methods: data analysis

Data analysis followed the methods of traditional ethnographic research (Bogdan

and Biklen 1998; Lincoln and Guba 1981). After all interviews were transcribed, we

conducted a thorough (hand) coding of the data. Interestingly, there was little

difference in tone or topic between educators in schools where leaders were highly

supportive and informed about bilingual education, and educators in schools without

such strong supporters of bilingual programs. However, one immediate finding was

that principals’ and teachers’ interviews were dramatically different, regardless of

the school in which they worked. Although the themes to emerge from principals’

interviews were similar to those emerging in teacher interviews, principals were of

course more focused on school-wide issues and issues of district support than on

issues of classroom practice and teacher decision-making. Furthermore, principals

tended to offer an ideal perspective rather than a realistic, ‘‘messy’’ (Goldstein

2007) perspective on classroom practice—they described what they believed should

be happening in the classrooms rather than what was actually happening. Principals

echoed teachers’ concerns with the difficulty of choosing a language for instruction

during the complex transition process; they also echoed what we discovered to be a

major commonality among teachers, i.e. the powerful influence of the TAKS test in

the decision-making process. Even a very strong school-based language policy

seemed to provide insufficient protection against the power of tests in shaping

language policy. Overall, the principal interviews did not offer considerable insight

into teachers’ decision-making process, which was the research question driving this

analysis. For this reason, we decided to focus on teacher interviews for this article.

The following analysis is based upon our coding of teacher interviews. Using

Microsoft Word, we sorted and categorized teacher comments from each interview

into files according to emerging themes; in the process, as always occurs, the themes

altered and expanded to fit the full data set.

The following themes emerged as salient, leading to our findings reported below.

The TAKS impacted teacher instruction in terms of both curriculum and pedagogy.

The TAKS drove the language of instruction and further complicated teachers’ highly

complex and multi-dimensional processes of helping children transition from Spanish

to English instruction. And, teachers’ views of the higher status of English and the

lower status of Spanish (and their view that the Spanish test was more difficult due to

translation issues) led them to move children into English testing as soon as they

could. These three dynamics worked together in such a way that the TAKS test,

despite its translation into Spanish, appeared to undermine bilingual programs and

thereby short-circuit the education of ELLs in Texas elementary classrooms.

Limitations

In order to triangulate our findings, we had intended to include classroom

observations in the study, thus offering evidence of what teachers were doing, not

just what they said they were doing. We had planned to spend time in the

classrooms of six of our teacher participants; however, due in large part to testing

Bilingual education for a monolingual test 225

123

Page 10: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

pressures, the interview process took longer than expected, and access to classrooms

for observation was limited. For instance, one principal who we contacted in early

January was willing to allow her teachers to be interviewed, but not until after they

had completed the first round of TAKS testing in late February. Principals were

unwilling to allow us access to observe in classrooms at all during the months of

March and April, and a few teachers explicitly told us that during those months we

would not see ‘‘normal’’ classroom operations anyway, due to the impact of testing.

Our efforts to track down teachers and observe the following Fall were thwarted due

to a number of staffing changes, and having by that point done our preliminary

coding we realized that the teachers would by then have different students and have

made different decisions than those they described anyway.

Without the perspective that the observations would have helped us to garner,

our findings in this study can only have a limited impact. We can, at this point,

only discuss the participants’ impressions and assertions; we cannot talk about

their actual classroom practices. Further, while an effort was made to seek out as

wide a range of perspectives as possible with a limited sample size, this study is

limited by the small number of participants (Principals: 6; Teachers: 16) as well

as by the fact that each was only interviewed on one occasion. Nonetheless, we

hope that the questions we raise and the issues we address will help to guide

further research in this area. We are confident that what we have identified is

worthy of further study, and we would be remiss to hold off on sharing our

findings despite their limitations. Furthermore, the fact that this study echoes

findings elsewhere in the United States (Menken 2008) implies that there is

reason to believe that we have unearthed a powerful and possibly dangerous

impact of high-stakes testing.

Findings

Three interrelated themes emerged from the research, relating to the narrowing of

curriculum and pedagogy, to choices about language of instruction and the process

of transition, and to the impact of the higher status of English. In all three, teachers

as responsible and caring professionals appear to go through a complex process of

sense-making in their efforts to resolve often contradictory demands. Teaching is

indeed an endeavor of ‘‘unforgivable complexity’’ (Cochran-Smith 2003), and high-

stakes testing appears to have only added to the complexity for teachers’ processes

of decision-making.

First, as has been demonstrated in other research (Au 2007; Wright and Choi

2005), almost all the teachers in this study identified various ways in which the TAKS

has directly and indirectly influenced their curricular and instructional choices. While

many of the teachers expressed an understanding of the need for accountability in

schools, they were critical of the pressure they felt to modify their teaching in ways

that resulted in instruction narrowly focused on preparing students to pass the TAKS.

As one teacher stated, ‘‘That TAKS test has entirely changed the way I teach. I teach

with the goal of getting the kids to pass the test rather than to get them to be successful

in reading, writing and math in general.’’ Another teacher told us, ‘‘I think [the TAKS]

226 D. Palmer, A. W. Lynch

123

Page 11: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

has definitely changed the way I teach. That’s our focus, that’s our job for the year.’’

In fact, all but one of the sixteen teachers we interviewed stated that the TAKS has

changed the way that they teach; and we discovered when we attempted to follow up

in the Fall that that one, a fifth grade teacher, was not asked to return to his school the

following year because, according to his principal, he was not adequately preparing

children for the TAKS. The high-stakes nature of the TAKS has left bilingual teachers

with few choices other than to narrow the curriculum and teach to the test. The vast

majority of teachers felt that the changes in their teaching caused by the TAKS have

been negative or not beneficial to their students.

Second, teachers’ choices about language of instruction for their transitional

bilingual classrooms were impacted by the TAKS test. Teachers described the

difficult decision-making process they go through in their classrooms subject-by-

subject and child-by-child, regarding the language in which to teach. During the

transition years, state and district policies encourage teachers to move children

gradually into English instruction. Yet accountability policy requires teachers to

choose a single language for each child’s high-stakes test. Perhaps not

surprisingly, it appears that the language of instruction teachers choose for each

child is largely determined by the language in which the child will test during that

school year.

Meanwhile, in order to best prepare children, choice of a language for testing

occurs early in the school year in most schools, balancing many factors, and is

described by many of our participants as agonizing. Teachers describe getting input

from parents, children’s former teachers, and children themselves, looking to

various assessments, observations, work samples, and classroom performance to

help determine in what language they will test. Teachers express a profound sense

of responsibility in making this decision for their students. A third grade teacher

said, ‘‘For my kids who take the reading test in English, I mean that’s what

determines the course of their year.’’

Once this decision is made, it influences every other decision teachers make

about instruction for that child. It is not uncommon for teachers to have students in

the same classroom who will be testing in different languages. To meet this

challenge, teachers routinely group students by language for instruction in the

classroom, teaching English speakers in small group lessons in English, and Spanish

speakers in small group lessons in Spanish. When teachers have to teach the whole

group at the same time, they must determine a way to incorporate both languages.

They will often use concurrent translation, i.e. they will repeat all instructions in

both languages, although this has been shown to be a less effective means of

teaching language (Legarreta 1979). Teachers will justify this by pointing out that

their priority is for students to understand math and reading instruction in their

dominant language; second language instruction will come later (after the test).

Another third grade teacher described her approach to teaching math: ‘‘Math, we do

a lot of oral English but you know most of their books and stuff is in Spanish, and

because of the test. Because if they test in Spanish, they have to know the word in

Spanish, so...’’ Although wanting to move the children gradually to English

instruction in mathematics, this teacher maintains Spanish texts in order to continue

to prepare them to test in Spanish only; she is elegantly addressing a complicated

Bilingual education for a monolingual test 227

123

Page 12: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

balance of contradictory pressures. In an example of the kind of complexity we

often heard voiced, a teacher explained the roles of English and Spanish in her

classroom in the following way:

Math, I throw in [Spanish], it depends on if I’ve already introduced it. Then

I’ll bring it in English, otherwise it goes in Spanish. I have to tell you those

three subjects [math, science, social studies] I hit English pretty strong, in

reading I don’t, and you know why that is... Because of the TAKS.

Thus, the teacher instructs children in their native language when she fears they will

not understand her instruction in English, but otherwise ‘‘hits English pretty strong’’

to give the children practice for future years and move them into English. But for

reading instruction, in which her group will be tested in Spanish, she does not even

make an effort to encourage English. She is quite candid that her choice is driven by

the test, rather than by her professional judgment about what would be best to help

her students learn content and language.

In describing the ways they determine language of instruction for students,

most participants described a larger frame: their overall goal to guide their

students in the transition process from Spanish to English instruction. Still, this

process was impacted by the need to prepare children for the tests; some teachers

described the challenge they face reconciling what they know to be poor practice

in terms of transitioning bilingual students—such as overnight switches from

all-Spanish to all-English instruction, or simultaneous translation throughout their

instruction—with what they see as a necessity in order to prepare children to

test monolingually and to receive instruction in future grades without primary

language supports.

In third grade, the process was highly complex. Each of our third grade informants

described a unique and, to a certain extent, individualized approach to developing the

ideal program for each bilingual child in his/her classroom. There does not appear to

be any single process that teachers follow, and teachers do not appear to display any

sense of rigidity in terms of the language needs of children. They offer children

‘‘Spanish support’’ where they see it is needed, and they encourage children to ‘‘try in

English’’ when they feel they are ready. A third grade teacher in a classroom with

children of varying levels of English at various points in their transition process

described how he decides which language to use for instruction:

I have whole group and small group. Whole group is always going to be done

in English, small group in Spanish. But at the same time, they also, whenever

there is a new [word], they have the vocabulary in English and Spanish in front

of them [because I provide the texts or worksheets in both languages].

At times, factors from outside the classroom influence the language in which a

specific subject is taught. In the words of one teacher:

Here my Newcomer [support] teacher helps me. What he does is more science

and social studies because it’s only two hours he can come in... I do all my

instruction in English and then they go with [the Newcomer teacher] for

science and social studies. They get the concepts in Spanish. What I do is I

228 D. Palmer, A. W. Lynch

123

Page 13: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

pull small groups after the whole group lesson. I’ll reiterate the content in

Spanish just in case they didn’t get it.

Teachers are constantly making multifaceted decisions regarding language of

instruction, balancing the need to address the TAKS with the language and

curricular needs of children.

Without denying the complexity of teachers’ decision-making with regards to

transition in third grade, we found that many decisions are largely determined by the

TAKS testing schedule. In Texas, the TAKS is administered for the first time in

February. Students who do not meet the minimum score for promotion in third and

fifth grade take the TAKS for a second time in April. If a third test is necessary, it is

administered during the summer after intense summer school test preparation

instruction, often referred to by teachers and students as ‘TAKS camp’. Other

indicators of individual student readiness to transition to English can be forced out

of the decision-making process by the TAKS’ fixed schedule. A third grade teacher

commented on how and when she decided to transition her students to English

instruction:

Like I said after the TAKS test, that’s when I started introducing more of the

English. But after the reading TAKS test is the math TAKS test, which is also

in Spanish. You know that’s why it’s kind of hard...doing the transition

because I know that they’re going to be taking the TAKS test in Spanish. I

want to make sure they have the concept in Spanish, and not confuse them

with English.

Here is another example of a third grade teacher bending her students’ transition

schedule to the TAKS:

I haven’t done a lot of direct teaching in English as far as direct decoding in

English to those who are working in Spanish yet, because I was waiting until

after the TAKS test, because I wanted to focus on the Spanish.

Expressing the fear shared by many of our participants regarding the pressure to

choose a successful language for testing each child, another third grader teacher

asserted, ‘‘If the student is reading fourth grade in Spanish, and third or second in

English, I’m not going to chance it, we’re going with the fourth grade Spanish [and

the child will test in Spanish].’’ Clearly, teachers struggle to reconcile the absolute

nature of the administration schedule of the monolingual test, with the gradual

nature of third grade transition.

We found that in most of the fifth grade classrooms, the process for choosing a

language of instruction appeared less complex; it was almost entirely determined by

the TAKS testing schedule and the fact that sixth grade classrooms district-wide did

not offer primary language support to students. Teachers by and large chose to

transition students rapidly as soon as the TAKS calendar allowed rather than to base

their decision on any signs of student readiness. This resulted in teachers choosing

at times to delay a student’s transition until after success on the Spanish TAKS, and

at times to accelerate transition in preparation for the English TAKS. As one-fifth

grade teacher stated, ‘‘The borderline kids, that’s the tricky one, we tend to usually

Bilingual education for a monolingual test 229

123

Page 14: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

keep them in Spanish just to be on the safe side. After TAKS then we transition

them over on into English.’’ Another fifth grade teacher shared a summary of her

transition schedule for her students:

Now once my children who took TAKS in Spanish and passed the first

administration, then it’s intense English for them. Everything they do from

that point on is in English. Writing is in English. Books from the library get

chosen in English. Everything is done in English for them. I say, ‘Okay, once

you pass the TAKS Spanish, shoooot [sic], English only.’

Teachers were highly critical of these constraints; they seemed simultaneously

aware and unaware that the testing schedule and the middle schools’ lack of services

were leading them to undermine their bilingual programs. Yet, defiant in their

commitment to help their ELL students navigate an extremely challenging set of

obstacles, they stood by their practices.

As these data show, language of instruction is profoundly influenced by the

language teachers and schools choose for their children’s high-stakes testing. A

child testing in Spanish will receive the majority of instruction in Spanish until they

test successfully, allowing them little time and support for the challenging transition

into English instruction. Meanwhile, a child testing in English will receive that same

instruction in English with little of the primary language support they need to help

them succeed.

The district office’s bilingual education department commonly advises schools to

‘‘test students in the language they are taught in,’’ the teachers tend to frame this

process in the opposite direction: they assert that they teach children in the language

in which they will ultimately test. The difference here may be subtle, but we believe

it is very significant. Whereas the district’s message puts instruction primary and

assessment secondary and dependent upon instruction, the teachers translate this in

the opposite direction. Most teachers’ processes for determining language of

instruction, as they described it, began with a determination of the language in

which the student would take the TAKS.

A third theme that emerged in the data, one that is reinforced throughout the

Southwest United States and perhaps underlies the logic of a transitional bilingual

program, was participants’ prevalent view that English, with its higher status in

society, was to be preferred in the classroom. Children were labeled as ‘‘successful’’

and seen as smarter when they performed in English, while students who passed the

Spanish TAKS were merely labeled as ready for the ‘‘challenge’’ of English.

Students’ entire academic careers throughout the transition years of elementary

school were seen as a progression from leaning on the ‘‘crutch’’ of Spanish (in the

words of one fifth grade teacher) to demonstrating they could ‘‘make it’’ in English.

While this should not surprise anyone who has spent time working with teachers on

the topic of transition, it is extremely important to the dynamic of teacher sense-

making we are describing. Teachers who believe that success is determined only by

achievement in English will push children to test in English whether they are ready

or not. When keeping children in Spanish ‘‘to be safe,’’ they will inevitably convey

their impressions to children: that Spanish is only a temporary stepping stone to the

‘‘real’’ academic language of English, and that hard-working, fast-learning children

230 D. Palmer, A. W. Lynch

123

Page 15: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

will no longer ‘‘need’’ to lean on Spanish. This too has a profoundly negative impact

on the success of bilingual programs.

Discussion/Conclusion

Primary language instruction has been demonstrated to be the most efficient way to

help ELLs both learn English and succeed academically in US schools (Rolstad

et al. 2005; Genesee et al. 2006; Slavin and Cheung 2005; Francis et al. 2006).

However, no form of instruction appears immune to the distorting effects of high-

stakes accountability. These interviews conducted with elementary bilingual

teachers in Texas demonstrate that the TAKS profoundly impacts teachers’

decisions about curricular topics, instructional practices, and the process of

transition from Spanish into English. Even when Spanish is expected to be used as

the language of instruction for native Spanish speakers, and even when a Spanish

translation is offered for the high-stakes tests, the mere presence of a high-stakes

test appears to undermine the purposes of the bilingual program.

It is clear that a primary language assessment is the most helpful form of test

accommodation for ELLs who have received primary language instruction (Rivera

and Collum 2006; Abedi et al. 2004). It has even been argued that this is the only

form of test accommodation that has the possibility of allowing us to accurately

assess ELLs’ content area knowledge (Wright 2005). This research is in no way

meant to imply that the presence of a Spanish language version of the TAKS is the

problem; on the contrary, without the Spanish TAKS, children would have even less

access to the supports they need to succeed. The problem lies rather in the high-

stakes, single-measure accountability system that pervades Texas elementary

classrooms. Single-measure high-stakes accountability has been shown to lead to a

range of perverse effects, no matter which language is used for testing.

No single assessment can effectively replace a teacher’s professional judgment

for instructional decisions about a bilingual child. Teachers are the professionals

who have the most direct contact with students and the most intimate knowledge of

their students’ abilities and needs. Teachers in this study attempted to resist policies

they felt were harmful to their students, but they had little actual power to do so.

Many felt they had no choice other than to narrow the curriculum in an effort to

boost test scores and to comply with the policy changes that have been made. Thus,

as in New York (Menken 2008), Texas’ elementary bilingual teachers have been

conscripted into the role of policy agents enacting a de facto language policy

created by the imposition of high-stakes standardized testing.

It is central to effective bilingual instruction that classroom teachers know how to

make decisions about the language(s) they should use when instructing children;

however, it is also crucial that teachers have the latitude to make these decisions in

the best interests of children. The ever-present power of the TAKS to impact

teachers’ decisions about language of instruction appears to have a degrading effect

on bilingual programs in Texas. This may have negative implications not only for

Texas’ bilingual learners, but also for ELLs subjected to high-stakes testing

throughout the US.

Bilingual education for a monolingual test 231

123

Page 16: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

We recommend opening up our definitions of accountability in K-12 education

to multiple and diverse measures of student achievement. Current accountability

systems too often look only at the single measure results of high-stakes

standardized tests such as the TAKS; we would like to see accountability that

takes into account the complexity of the teaching and learning process, and the

unending complexity of schools, drawing from a much wider array of measures

including (but not limited to) attendance; graduation rates; parent and student

surveys; classroom-based performance measures such as grades and other forms of

teacher assessment; and various measures of teacher professionalism, expertise,

and rates of certification. We would also like to see systems with the flexibility to

allow local district and school decision-making in terms of which measures will

be used. This will ensure that teachers have the assessment data they need in order

to best instruct ELLs, while preventing any single test from undermining their

choices.

Further, we call for the restoration of the goals of bilingualism and biliteracy as

major priorities of federal education policy in the United States. Because broader

language policy is often implicitly determined through the language policies in our

schools (Crawford 2004), restoring bilingualism and biliteracy as explicit goals of our

education systems could impact both education outcomes and language policy

nationwide. Not only will this allow ELLs in our K-12 system to succeed in greater

numbers, but in an increasingly interconnected world, we can help multilingual

children view all of their languages and cultures as resources that will enrich their lives

and our society.

Appendix I

Guiding interview questions for teachers

General:

1. Do you believe that bilingual education is an effective way to teach English

Language Learners? Why/why not? (What do you see as its strengths/weaknesses?)

2. In what ways does your school support or not support bilingual education?

3. In which language do you instruct reading? Does it vary by student? How do

you decide which language to use?

4. Do you have any strategies that support reading instruction in both languages

(English and Spanish)?

5. Schedule: How do you fit primary and second language instruction throughout

the day? How is this determined?

6. Tell me specifically about the components of your reading instruction. Do you

do, for example, shared reading, read aloud, guided reading? What else? What

materials do you use most?

7. How do you teach students to read in English?

8. How do you decide when your second language learners are ready to transfer to

an all English classroom?

232 D. Palmer, A. W. Lynch

123

Page 17: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

Policy-specific:

9. Tell me about RISE. What is your understanding of what it is, what it’s for, how

it works/doesn’t work, who came up with it, etc.?

10. What has RISE meant for you and your students? E.g. has RISE changed the

way you teach? Has it had positive/negative effects on student learning?

11. Tell me about TAKS reading test. What is your understanding of what it is,

what it’s for, how it works/doesn’t work, who came up with it, etc.?

12. What has TAKS reading meant for you and your students? E.g. has TAKS

changed the way you teach? Has it had positive/negative effects on student

learning?

13. How do you determine what language your students take TAKS reading test in?

14. Tell me about Accountable Talk (AT). What is your understanding of what it

is, what it’s for, how it works/doesn’t work, who came up with it, etc.

15. What has Accountable Talk meant for you and your students? E.g. Has

Accountable Talk changed the way you teach? Has it had positive/negative

effects on student learning?

16. How do you use accountable talk within your reading block?

17. Do you use AT in English, Spanish, or both?

References

Abedi, J. (2002). Standardized achievement tests and English language learners: Psychometrics issues.

Educational Assessment, 8(3), 231–257.

Abedi, J. (2004). The no child left behind act and English language learners: Assessment and

accountability issues. Educational Researcher, 33(1), 4–14.

Abedi, J., & Lord, C. (2001). The language factor in mathematics tests. Applied Measurement inEducation, 14(3), 219–234.

Abedi, J., Hofstetter, C., & Lord, C. (2004). Assessment accommodations for English language learners:

Implications for policy-based empirical research. Review of Educational Research, 74(1), 1–28.

Alamillo, L., & Viramontes, C. (2000). Reflections from the classroom: Teachers perspectives on the

implementation of proposition 227. Bilingual Research Journal, 24(1–2), 155–168.

Alamillo, L., Palmer, D., Viramontes, C., & Garcia, E. (2005). In A. Valenzuela (Ed.), Leaving childrenbehind: How Texas-style accountability fails Latino youth (pp. 201–224). Albany: State University

of New York Press.

American Institutes for Research. (1999). Voluntary national tests in reading and math: Backgroundpaper reviewing laws and regulations, current practice, and research relevant to inclusion andaccommodations for students with limited English proficiency. Palo Alto, CA: Author.

Au, W. (2007). High-stakes testing and curricular control: A qualitative metasynthesis. EducationalResearcher, 36(5), 258–267.

Bogdan, R., & Biklen, S. (1998). Qualitative research for education: An introduction to theory andmethods. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon; Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Cheng, L., Watanabe, Y., & Curtis, A. (Eds.). (2004). Washback in language testing: Researchy contextsand methods. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Clotfelter, C., & Ladd, H. (1996). Recognizing and rewarding success in public schools. In H. Ladd (Ed.),

Holding schools accountable: Performance-based reform in education (pp. 23–64). Washington, DC:

The Brookings Institution.

Coburn, C. (2001). Collective sensemaking about reading: How teachers mediate reading policy in their

professional communities. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 23(2), 145–170.

Cochran-Smith, M. (2003). The unforgivable complexity of teaching: Avoiding simplicity in an age of

accountability. Journal of Teacher Education, 54(1), 3–5.

Bilingual education for a monolingual test 233

123

Page 18: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

Crawford, J. (2004). Educating English learners: Language diversity in the classroom (5th ed).

Los Angeles, CA: Bilingual Educational Services, Inc.

Cummins, D., Kintsch, W., Reusser, K., & Weismer, R. (1998). The role of understanding in solving

word problems. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 405–438.

Darling-Hammond, L., & Millman, J. (Eds.). (1990). The new handbook of teacher evaluation: Assessingelementary and secondary school teachers. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., & DeWin, L. (1985). Influences of rewording verbal problems on children’s

problem representations and solutions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 27(4), 460–470.

Denzin, N., & Lincoln, Y. (Eds.). (1998). Collecting and interpreting qualitative materials. Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Echevarria, J., Short, D., & Powers, K. (2006). School reform and standards-based education: A model for

English-language learners. Journal of Educational Research, 99(4), 195–210.

Francis, D., Leseaux, N., & August, D. (2006). Language of instruction. In D. August & T. Shanahan

(Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners (pp. 365–413). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates.

Genesee, R., et al. (2006). Educating English language learners: A synthesis of research evidence.

New York: Cambridge University Press.

Goldstein, L. (2007). Beyond the DAP versus standards dilemma: Examining the unforgiving complexity

of kindergarten teaching in the United States. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 39–54.

Greene, J. P. (1997). A meta-analysis of the Rosell and Baker review. Bilingual Research Journal, 21(2–3),

103–123. http://ifl.lrdc.pitt.edu/ifl/index.php?section=about.

Hudson, T. (1983). Correspondences and numerical differences between disjoint sets. Child Development,54, 84–90.

Jennings, N. (1996). Interpreting policy in real classrooms: Case studies of state reform and teacherpractice. New York: Teachers College Press.

Kohn, A. (2000). Burn at the high stakes. Journal of Education, 51(4), 315–327.

Kopriva, R. (2000). Ensuring accuracy in testing for English language learners. Washington, DC:

Council of Chief State School Officers.

Krashen, S., & McField, G. (2005). What works? Reviewing the latest evidence on bilingual education.

Language Learner, 1(2), 7–10, 34.

Legarreta, D. (1979). The effects of program models on language acquisition by Spanish-speaking

children. TESOL Quarterly, 8, 521–534.

Lincoln, Y., & Guba, E. (1981). Effective evaluation. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers.

Linn, R., & Gronlund, N. (2000). Measurement and assessment in teaching (8th ed.). Upper Saddle River,

NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Menken, K. (2008). English learners left behind: Standardized testing as language policy. Clevedon,

England: Multilingual Matters.

Mestre, J. P. (1998). The role of language comprehension in mathematics, problem solving. In R. R. Cocking

& J. P. Mestre (Eds.), Linguistic and cultural influences on learning mathematics (pp. 200–220).

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Michaels, S., O’Connor, M. C., Hall, M. W., & Resnick, L. (2002). Accountable talk: Classroomconversation that works, CD-ROM Set. On principles of learning. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburg.

Mischler, E. G. (1991). Research interviewing: Context and narrative. Cambridge: Harvard.

No Child Left Behind Act. Public Law 107–110.

Olsen, J. F., & Goldstein, A. A. (1997). The inclusion of students with disabilities and limited Englishproficiency students in large-scale assessments: A summary of recent progress. (NCES Publicationno. 97–482). Washington, DC: National Center for Educational Statistics.

Orfield, G., & Lee, C. (2005). Why segregation matters: Poverty and educational inequality. Cambridge,

MA: Harvard Civil Rights Project.

Porac, J. F., Thomas, H., & Baden-Fuller, C. (1989). Competitive groups as cognitive communities: The

case of Scottish knotwear manufacturers. Journal of Management Studies, 26, 397–416.

Ramirez, D. (1992). Executive summary of the longitudinal study of structured English immersion

strategy, early-exit and late-exit transitional bilingual education programs for language minority

children. Bilingual Research Journal, 16, 1–62.

Riley, M. S., Greeno, J. G., & Heller, J. I. (1983). Development of children’s problem-solving ability in

arithmetic. In H. P. Ginsburg (Ed.), The development of mathematical thinking (pp. 153–196).

New York: Academic Press.

234 D. Palmer, A. W. Lynch

123

Page 19: Bilingual education for a monolingual test

Rivera, C., & Collum, E. (Eds.). (2006). State assessment policy and practice for English languagelearners: A national perspective. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Rivera, C., & Stansfield, C. W. (1998). Leveling the playing field for English learners: Increasing

participation in state, local assessments through accommodations. In R. Brandt (Ed.), Assessing studentlearning: New rules, new realities (pp. 65–92). Arlington, VA: Educational Research Service.

Roderick, M. (1994). Grade retention and school dropout: Investigating the association. AmericanEducational Research Journal, 31(4), 729–759.

Rolstad, K., Mahoney, K., & Glass, G. (2005). The big picture: A meta-analysis of program effectiveness

research on English language learners. Educational Policy 19(4), 572–594.

Rubin, H., & Rubin, I. (1995). Qualitative interviewing. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Rumberger, R. W. (1995). Dropping out of middle school: A multilevel analysis of students and schools.

American Educational Research Journal, 32(3), 583–625.

Slavin, R., & Cheung, A. (2005). A synthesis of research on language of reading instruction for English

language learners. Review of Educational Research, 75(2), 247–284.

Spillane, J. (1999). External reform initiatives and teachers’ efforts to reconstruct their practice: The

mediating role of teacher’s zone of enactment. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 31(2), 143–175.

Spillane, J. (2004). Standard deviation: How schools misunderstand educational policy. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press.

Spillane, J., & Jennings, N. (1997). Aligned instructional policy and ambitious pedagogy: Exploring

instructional reform from the classroom perspective. Teachers College Record, 98, 439–481.

Stritikus, T., & Garcia, E. (2000). Education of limited English proficient students in California schools:An assessment of the influence of proposition 227 on selected teachers and classrooms. Paper

presented at the American Education Research Association annual meeting, San Francisco, CA.

Tyack, D., & Cuban, L. (1995). Tinkering toward utopia: A century of public school reform. Cambridge,

MA: Harvard University Press.

Wall, D. (1997). Impact and washback in language testing. In C. Clapham & D. Corson (Eds.), Languagetesting and assessment, Encyclopedia of language and education (Vol. 7, pp. 291–302). Dordrecht,

Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Pulbishers.

Weick, K. (1995). Sensemaking in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.

Westrum, R. (1982). Social intelligence about hidden events. Knowledge, 3(3), 381–400.

Wright, W. (2002). The effects of high stakes testing on an inner-city elementary school: The curriculum,

the teachers, and the English language learners. Current Issues in Education, 5(5). Retrieved January

16, 2007 from: http://cie.ed.asu.edu/volume5/number5/.

Wright, W. (2005). English language learners left behind in Arizona: The nullification of accommo-

dations in the intersection of federal and state policies. Bilingual Research Journal, 29.

Wright, W. & Choi, D. (2005). Voices from the classroom: A statewide survey of experienced third-grade

English language learner teachers on the impact of language and high-stakes testing policies in

Arizona. Language Policy Research Unit.

Author Biographies

Deborah Palmer is an Assistant Professor in Bilingual/Bicultural Education in the Department of

Curriculum and Instruction at the University of Texas at Austin. She holds a B.A. in Anthropology from

Stanford University, and an M.A. and a Ph.D. from the University of California, Berkeley in Language,

Literacy and Culture in Education. She is a former bilingual elementary school teacher. She conducts

qualitative research using ethnography and discourse analysis in linguistically diverse settings. Her interests

include bilingual education policy and politics in the United States, two-way bilingual immersion education,

and the complexities of preparing teachers to manage race and class diverse bilingual classrooms.

Anissa Wicktor Lynch is a fourth year doctoral student at the University of Texas at Austin in bilingual/

bicultural education. She holds a B.A. in Spanish and Latin American Iberian Studies from the University

of Wisconsin-Madison and an M.A. in Elementary Education from the University of Alabama. She

completed her teaching certification coursework at Prescott College and worked for seven years as a

bilingual elementary school teacher and adult ESL teacher. Her current research focuses on caring in

bilingual classrooms. This is her first publication.

Bilingual education for a monolingual test 235

123