Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer...

185
Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010
  • date post

    21-Dec-2015
  • Category

    Documents

  • view

    213
  • download

    0

Transcript of Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer...

Page 1: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Basic Experimental Design

Larry V. HedgesNorthwestern University

Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010

Page 2: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Institute ScheduleMonday Tuesday Wednesday Thursday Friday

26-Jul 27-Jul 28-Jul 29-Jul 30-Jul

8:00-10:00 8:00-10:00 8:00-10:00 8:00-10:00 8:00-10:00

Basic Design I Sample/power I Growth Modeling Power Lab I Specify models

Hedges Bloom Hedges Spybrook Lipsey

10:30-12:30 10:30-12:30 10:30-12:30 10:30-12:30 10:30-12:30

Basic Design II Sample/Power II Analysis Lab I Power Lab II Describe outcomes

Hedges Bloom Hedges Spybrook Lipsy

    Konstantopoulos    

Lunch 12:30-1:30 Lunch 12:30-1:30 Lunch 12:30-1:30 Lunch 12:30-1:30 Lunch 12:30-1:30

1:30-3:30 1:30-3:30 1:30-3:30 1:30-3:30 1:30-3:30

Basic Design III Sampling/External Analysis Lab II Mediation Models Model Cause

Hedges Validity Hedges Beretvas Cordray

  Bloom Konstantopoulos    

4:00-5:30 4:00-5:30 4:00-5:30 4:00-5:30 4:00-5:30

Introduce Group Project Group Project Group Project Group Project

Group Projects Meeting Meeting Meeting Meeting

Cordray Cordray + Others Cordray + Others Cordray+ Others Others

Dinner 6:00 Dinner 6:00 Dinner at Carmen's Dinner 6:00 Dinner at Stained Glass

Page 3: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Institute ScheduleMonday Tuesday Wednesday Thursday

2-Aug 3-Aug 4-Aug 5-Aug

8:00-10:00 8:00-10:00 8:00-10:00 8:00-10:00

Missing Data I Moderator Analysis Finalize Group Group 3 Presents

Graham Konstantopoulos Projects (faculty feedback)

10:30-12:30 10:30-12:30 10:30-12:30 10:30-12:30

Missing Data II Alternate Designs I Finalize Group Group 4 Presents

Graham Lipsey Projects (faculty feedback)

Lunch 12:30-1:30 Lunch 12:30-1:30 Lunch 12:30-1:30 Lunch 12:30-1:30

1:30-3:30 1:30-3:30 1:30-3:30 1:30-3:30

Analyzing Fidelity Alternate Designs II Group 1 Presents Group 5 presents

Cordray Lipsey (faculty feedback) (faculty feedback)

4:00-5:30 4:00-5:30 4:00-5:30 4:00-5:30

Group Project Group Project Group 2 Presents Course Evaluation

Meeting Meeting

Cordray + Others Cordray + Others (faculty feedback) Debrief

Dinner at Mt Everest Dinner 6:00 Dinner 6:00 Dinner & Graduation 

Page 4: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

What is Experimental Design?

Experimental design includes both• Strategies for organizing data collection• Data analysis procedures matched to those data

collection strategies

Classical treatments of design stress analysis procedures based on the analysis of variance (ANOVA)

Other analysis procedure such as those based on hierarchical linear models or analysis of aggregates (e.g., class or school means) are also appropriate

Page 5: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Why Do We Need Experimental Design?

Because of variability

We wouldn’t need a science of experimental design if

• If all units (students, teachers, & schools) were identical

and

• If all units responded identically to treatments

We need experimental design to control variability so that treatment effects can be identified

Page 6: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

A Little History

The idea of controlling variability through design has a long history

In 1747 Sir James Lind’s studies of scurvy

Their cases were as similar as I could have them. They all in general had putrid gums, spots and lassitude, with weakness of their knees. They lay together on one place … and had one diet common to all (Lind, 1753, p. 149)

Lind then assigned six different treatments to groups of patients

Page 7: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

A Little History

The idea of random assignment was not obvious and took time to catch on

In 1648 von Helmont carried out one randomization in a trial of bloodletting for fevers

In 1904 Karl Pearson suggested matching and alternation in typhoid trials

Amberson, et al. (1931) carried out a trial with one randomization

In 1937 Sir Bradford Hill advocated alternation of patients in trials rather than randomization

Diehl, et al. (1938) carried out a trial that is sometimes referred to as randomized, but it actually used alternation

Page 8: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

A Little History

The first modern randomized clinical trial in medicine is usually considered to be the trial of streptomycin for treating tuberculosis

It was conducted by the British Medical Research Council in 1946 and reported in 1948

Page 9: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

A Little History

Experiments have been used longer in the behavioral sciences (e.g., psychophysics: Pierce and Jastrow, 1885)

Experiments conducted in laboratory settings were widely used in educational psychology (e.g., McCall, 1923)

Thorndike (early 1900’s)

Lindquist (1953)

Gage field experiments on teaching (1978 – 1984)

Page 10: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

A Little History

Studies in crop variation I – VI (1921 – 1929)

In 1919 a statistician named Fisher was hired at Rothamsted agricultural station

They had a lot of observational data on crop yields and hoped a statistician could analyze it to find effects of various treatments

All he had to do was sort out the effects of confounding variables

Page 11: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Studies in Crop Variation I (1921)

Fisher does regression analyses—lots of them—to study (and get rid of) the effects of confounders

• soil fertility gradients• drainage differences• effects of rainfall• effects of temperature and weather, etc.

Fisher does qualitative work to sort out anomalies

Conclusion

The effects of confounders are typically larger than those of the systematic effects we want to study

Page 12: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Studies in Crop Variation II (1923)

Fisher invents

• Basic principles of experimental design

• Control of variation by randomization

• Analysis of variance

Page 13: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Studies in Crop Variation IV and VI

Studies in Crop variation IV (1927)

Fisher invents analysis of covariance to combine statistical control and control by randomization

Studies in crop variation VI (1929)

Fisher refines the theory of experimental design, introducing most other key concepts known today

Page 14: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Our Hero in 1929

Page 15: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Principles of Experimental Design

Experimental design controls background variability so that systematic effects of treatments can be observed

Three basic principles

1. Control by matching

2. Control by randomization

3. Control by statistical adjustment

Their importance is in that order

Page 16: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Control by Matching

Known sources of variation may be eliminated by matching

Eliminating genetic variationCompare animals from the same litter of mice

Eliminating district or school effectsCompare students within districts or schools

However matching is limited• matching is only possible on observable characteristics• perfect matching is not always possible• matching inherently limits generalizability by removing (possibly

desired) variation

Page 17: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Control by Matching

Matching ensures that groups compared are alike on specific known and observable characteristics (in principle, everything we have thought of)

Wouldn’t it be great if there were a method of making groups alike on not only everything we have thought of, but everything we didn’t think of too?

There is such a method

Page 18: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Control by Randomization

Matching controls for the effects of variation due to specific observable characteristics

Randomization controls for the effects all (observable or non-observable, known or unknown) characteristics

Randomization makes groups equivalent (on average) on all variables (known and unknown, observable or not)

Randomization also gives us a way to assess whether differences after treatment are larger than would be expected due to chance.

Page 19: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Control by Randomization

Random assignment is not assignment with no particular rule. It is a purposeful process

Assignment is made at random. This does not mean that the experimenter writes down the names of the varieties in any order that occurs to him, but that he carries out a physical experimental process of randomization, using means which shall ensure that each variety will have an equal chance of being tested on any particular plot of ground (Fisher, 1935, p. 51)

Page 20: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Control by Randomization

Random assignment of schools or classrooms is not assignment with no particular rule. It is a purposeful process

Assignment of schools to treatments is made at random. This does not mean that the experimenter assigns schools to treatments in any order that occurs to her, but that she carries out a physical experimental process of randomization, using means which shall ensure that each treatment will have an equal chance of being tested in any particular school (Hedges, 2007)

Page 21: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Control by Statistical Adjustment

Control by statistical adjustment is a form of pseudo-matching

It uses statistical relations to simulate matching

Statistical control is important for increasing precision but should not be relied upon to control biases that may exist prior to assignment

Statistical control is the weakest of the three experimental design principles because its validity depends on knowing a statistical model for responses

Page 22: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Using Principles of Experimental Design

You have to know a lot (be smart) to use matching and statistical control effectively

You do not have to be smart to use randomization effectively

But

Where all are possible, randomization is not as efficient (requires larger sample sizes for the same power) as matching or statistical control

Page 23: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Basic Ideas of Design:Independent Variables (Factors)

The values of independent variables are called levels

Some independent variables can be manipulated, others can’t

Treatments are independent variables that can be manipulated

Blocks and covariates are independent variables that cannot be manipulated

These concepts are simple, but are often confused

Remember: You can randomly assign treatment levels but not blocks

Page 24: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Basic Ideas of Design (Crossing)

Relations between independent variables

Factors (treatments or blocks) are crossed if every level of one factor occurs with every level of another factor

ExampleThe Tennessee class size experiment assigned students to

one of three class size conditions. All three treatment conditions occurred within each of the participating schools

Thus treatment was crossed with schools

Page 25: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Basic Ideas of Design (Nesting)

Factor B is nested in factor A if every level of factor B occurs within only one level of factor A

ExampleThe Tennessee class size experiment actually assigned

classrooms to one of three class size conditions. Each classroom occurred in only one treatment condition

Thus classrooms were nested within treatments

(But treatment was crossed with schools)

Page 26: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Where Do These Terms Come From?(Nesting)

An agricultural experiment where blocks are literally blocks or plots of land

Here each block is literally nested within a treatment condition

Blocks

1 2 … n

T1 T2 … T1

Page 27: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Where Do These Terms Come From?(Crossing)

An agricultural experiment

Blocks were literally blocks of land and plots of land within blocks were assigned different treatments

Blocks

1 2 … n

T1 T2…

T1

T2 T1 T2

Page 28: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Where Do These Terms Come From?(Crossing)

Blocks were literally blocks of land and plots of land within blocks were assigned different treatments.

Here treatment literally crosses the blocks

Blocks

1 2 … n

T1 T2…

T1

T2 T1 T2

Page 29: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Where Do These Terms Come From?(Crossing)

The experiment is often depicted like this. What is wrong with this as a field layout?

Consider possible sources of bias

Blocks

1 2 … n

Treatment 1    

… 

Treatment 2      

Page 30: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Blocking Variables

We often exploit natural structure by adding blocking variables to the design

Examples• districts• states• regions

This may be a good idea if they explain variation

But it raises issues in analysis about how you think about the blocks (fixed or random effects)

We will talk about that later

Page 31: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Think About These Designs

A study was to assign schools to treatments, but you decide to block by districts before assignment to treatments

A study was to have assigned individuals (students) to treatments within schools, but you decide to block by districts before assignment to treatments

Both of these designs occur frequently

Which design would you expect to be the most sensitive?

Page 32: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Districts As Blocks Added to a Hierarchical Design

D1 D2 …

T1 T2 T1 T2 …

S1 S2 S3 S4 S5 S6 S7 S8 …

Page 33: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Districts As Blocks Added to a Randomized Blocks Design

D1 D2 …

T1 T2 T1 T2 …

S1 S2 S1 S2 S3 S4 S3 S4 …

Page 34: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Think About These Designs

1. A study assigns T or C to 20 teachers. The teachers are in five schools, and each teacher teaches 4 science classes

2. A study assigns a reading treatment (or control) to children in 20 schools. Each child is classified into one of three groups with different risk of reading failure.

3. Two schools in each of 10 districts are picked to participate. Each school has two grade 4 teachers. One of them is assigned to T, the other to C

Page 35: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three Basic Designs

The completely randomized designTreatments are assigned to individuals

The randomized block designTreatments are assigned to individuals within blocks(This is sometimes called the matched design, because individuals are matched within blocks)

The hierarchical designTreatments are assigned to blocks, the same treatment is assigned to all individuals in the block

Page 36: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Completely Randomized Design

Individuals are randomly assigned to one of two treatments

Treatment Control

Individual 1 Individual 1

Individual 2 Individual 2

… …

Individual nT Individual nC

Page 37: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Randomized Block Design

Block 1 … Block m

Treatment 1

Individual 1

Individual 1

… …

Individual n1 Individual nm

Treatment 2

Individual n1 +1 Individual nm + 1

… …

Individual 2n1 Individual 2nm

Page 38: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Hierarchical Design

Treatment Control

Block 1 Block m Block m+1 Block 2m

Individual 1

Individual 1 Individual 1

Individual 1

Individual 2 Individual 2 Individual 2 Individual 2

… … … …

Individual n1 Individual nm Individual nm+1 Individual n2m

Page 39: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

Randomization has to be done as an explicit process devised by the experimenter

• Haphazard is not the same as random

• Unknown assignment is not the same as random

• “Essentially random” is technically meaningless

• Alternation is not random, even if you alternate from a random start

This is why R.A. Fisher was so explicit about randomization processes

Page 40: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

R.A. Fisher on how to randomize an experiment with small sample size and 5 treatments

A satisfactory method is to use a pack of cards numbered from 1 to 100, and to arrange them in random order by repeated shuffling. The varieties [treatments] are numbered from 1 to 5, and any card such as the number 33, for example is deemed to correspond to variety [treatment] number 3, because on dividing by 5 this number is found as the remainder. (Fisher, 1935, p.51)

Page 41: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

Think about Fisher’s description

Does it worry you in any way?

Page 42: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

You may want to use a table of random numbers, but be sure to pick an arbitrary start point!

Beware random number generators—they typically depend on seed values, be sure to vary the seed value (if they do not do it automatically)

Otherwise you can reliably generate the same sequence of random numbers every time

It is no different that starting in the same place in a table of random numbers

Page 43: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

Completely Randomized Design (2 treatments, 2n individuals)

Make a list of all individuals

For each individual, pick a random number from 1 to 2 (odd or even)

Assign the individual to treatment 1 if even, 2 if odd

When one treatment is assigned n individuals, stop assigning more individuals to that treatment

Page 44: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

Completely Randomized Design (2pn individuals, p treatments)

Make a list of all individuals

For each individual, pick a random number from 1 to p

One way to do this is to get a random number of any size, divide by p, the remainder R is between 0 and (p – 1), so add 1 to the remainder to get R + 1

Assign the individual to treatment R + 1

Stop assigning individuals to any treatment after it gets n individuals

Page 45: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

Randomized Block Design with 2 Treatments(m blocks per treatment, 2n individuals per block)

Make a list of all individuals in the first block

For each individual, pick a random number from 1 to 2 (odd or even)

Assign the individual to treatment 1 if even, 2 if odd

Stop assigning a treatment it is assigned n individuals in the block

Repeat the same process with every block

Page 46: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

Randomized Block Design with p Treatments(m blocks per treatment, pn individuals per block)

Make a list of all individuals in the first block

For each individual, pick a random number from 1 to p

Assign the individual to treatment p

Stop assigning a treatment it is assigned n individuals in the block

Repeat the same process with every block

Page 47: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

Hierarchical Design with 2 Treatments(m blocks per treatment, n individuals per block)

Make a list of all blocks

For each block, pick a random number from 1 to 2

Assign the block to treatment 1 if even, treatment 2 if odd

Stop assigning a treatment after it is assigned m blocks

Every individual in a block is assigned to the same treatment

Page 48: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

Hierarchical Design with p Treatments(m blocks per treatment, n individuals per block)

Make a list of all blocks

For each block, pick a random number from 1 to p

Assign the block to treatment corresponding to the number

Stop assigning a treatment after it is assigned m blocks

Every individual in a block is assigned to the same treatment

Page 49: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomization Procedures

What if I get a big imbalance by chance?

Classical answers

If there are random assignments you wouldn’t like, include blocking variables

OR

Use statistical control

More complicated alternatives

Adaptive randomization methods (e.g., Efron’s)

Page 50: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models

Page 51: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models in Educational Research

Sampling models are often ignored in educational research

But

Sampling is where the randomness comes from in social research

Sampling therefore has profound consequences for statistical analysis and research designs

Page 52: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models in Educational Research

Which is a better simple random sample (which sample will provide a more precise estimate)?

Sample A, with N = 1,000

Sample B, with N = 2,000

Page 53: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models in Educational Research

Why?

Because if the population variance is σT2

We know that the variance of the sample mean from a sample of size N is

σT2/N

But

Page 54: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models in Educational Research

Simple random samples are rare in field research

Educational populations are hierarchically nested:

• Students in classrooms in schools

• Schools in districts in states

We usually exploit the population structure to sample students by first sampling schools

Even then, most samples are not probability samples, but they are intended to be representative (of some population)

Page 55: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models in Educational Research

Survey research calls this strategy multistage (multilevel) clustered sampling

We often sample clusters (schools) first then individuals within clusters (students within schools)

This is a two-stage (two-level) cluster sample

We might sample schools, then classrooms, then students

This is a three-stage (three-level) cluster sample

Page 56: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models in Educational Research

Which is a better two-stage sample (which sample will provide a more precise estimate)?

Sample A, with N = 1,000

Sample B, with N = 2,000

Now we cannot tell unless we know the number of clusters (m) and number of units (n) in each cluster

Page 57: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision of Estimates Depends on the Sampling Model

Suppose the total population variance is σT2 and ICC is ρ

Consider two samples of size N = mn

A simple random sample or stratified sample

The variance of the mean is σT2/mn

A clustered sample of n students from each of m schools

The variance of the mean is (σT2/mn)[1 + (n – 1)ρ]

The inflation factor [1 + (n – 1)ρ] is called the design effect

Page 58: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision of Estimates Depends on the Sampling Model

Suppose the population variance is σT2

School level ICC is ρS, class level ICC is ρC

Consider two samples of size N = mpn

A simple random sample or stratified sample

The variance of the mean is σT2/mpn

A clustered sample of n students from p classes in m schools

The variance is (σT2/mpn)[1 + (pn – 1)ρS + (n – 1)ρC]

The three level design effect is [1 + (pn – 1)ρS + (n – 1)ρC]

Page 59: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Example

For example, suppose ρ = 0.20

Sample ASuppose m = 100 and n = 10, so N = 1,000 then the

variance of the mean is

(σT2/100 x 10)[1 + (10 – 1)0.20] = (σT

2/1000)(2.8)

Sample BSuppose m = 20 and n = 100, so N = 2,000, then the

variance of the mean is

(σT2/100 x 20)[1 + (100 – 1)0.20] = (σT

2/1000)(10.4)

Page 60: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision of Estimates Depends on the Sampling Model

The total variance can be partitioned into between cluster (σB2

) and within cluster (σW2 ) variance

We define the intraclass correlation as the proportion of total variance that is between clusters

There is typically much more variance within clusters (σW2 )

than between clusters (σB2 )

School level intraclass correlation values are 0.10 to 0.25

This means that (σW2 ) is between 9 and 3 times as large as

(σB2 )

2 2

2 2 2

B B

B W T

σ σρ

σ σ σ

Page 61: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision of Estimates Depends on the Sampling Model

So why does (σB2 ) have such a big effect?

Because averaging (independent things) reduces variance

The variance of the mean of a sample of m clusters of size n can be written as

The cluster effects are only averaged over the number of clusters

2 2 22 B W WBnσ σ σσ

mn m mn

Page 62: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision of Estimates Depends on the Sampling Model

Treatment effects in experiments and quasi-experiments are mean differences

Therefore precision of treatment effects and statistical power will depend on the sampling model

Page 63: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models in Educational Research

The fact that the population is structured does not mean the sample is must be a clustered sample

Whether it is a clustered sample depends on:

• How the sample is drawn (e.g., are schools sampled first then individuals randomly within schools)

• What the inferential population is (e.g., is the inference to these schools studied or a larger population of schools)

Page 64: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models in Educational Research

A necessary condition for a clustered sample is that it is drawn in stages using population subdivisions

• schools then students within schools

• schools then classrooms then students

However, if all subdivisions in a population are present in the sample, the sample is not clustered, but stratified

Stratification has different implications than clustering

Whether there is stratification or clustering depends on the definition of the population to which we draw inferences (the inferential population)

Page 65: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Sampling Models in Educational Research

The clustered/stratified distinction matters because it influences the precision of statistics estimated from the sample

If all population subdivisions are included in the every sample, there is no sampling (or exhaustive sampling) of subdivisions

• therefore differences between subdivisions add no uncertainty to estimates

If only some population subdivisions are included in the sample, it matters which ones you happen to sample

• thus differences between subdivisions add to uncertainty

Page 66: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Inferential Population and Inference Models

The inferential population or inference model has implications for analysis and therefore for the design of experiments

Do we make inferences to the schools in this sample or to a larger population of schools?

Inferences to the schools or classes in the sample are called conditional inferences

Inferences to a larger population of schools or classes are called unconditional inferences

Page 67: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Inferential Population and Inference Models

Note that the inferences (what we are estimating) are different in conditional versus unconditional inference models

• In a conditional inference, we are estimating the mean (or treatment effect) in the observed schools

• In unconditional inference we are estimating the mean (or treatment effect) in the population of schools from which the observed schools are sampled

We are still estimating a mean (or a treatment effect) but they are different parameters with different uncertainties

Page 68: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Fixed and Random Effects

When the levels of a factor (e.g., particular blocks included) in a study are sampled and the inference model is unconditional, that factor is called random and its effects are called random effects

When the levels of a factor (e.g., particular blocks included) in a study constitute the entire inference population and the inference model is conditional, that factor is called fixed and its effects are called fixed effects

Page 69: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Fixed and Random Effects

Remember the idea of adding blocking variables

Technically, if blocking variables (e.g., district) are

• fixed effects: generalizations are limited to the districts observed

• random effects: generalizations to a larger universe of districts

These technicalities are often ignored

The key point is that generalizations are not supported by sampling

Page 70: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Applications to Experimental Design

We will look in detail at the two most widely used experimental designs in education

• Randomized blocks designs

• Hierarchical designs

Page 71: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Experimental Designs

For each design we will look at

• Structural Model for data (and what it means)

• Two inference models– What does ‘treatment effect’ mean in principle– What is the estimate of treatment effect– How do we deal with context effects

• Two statistical analysis procedures– How do we estimate and test treatment effects– How do we estimate and test context effects– What is the sensitivity of the tests

Page 72: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Randomized Block Design

The population (the sampling frame)

We wish to compare two treatments

• We assign treatments within schools

• Many schools with 2n students in each

• Assign n students to each treatment in each school

Page 73: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Randomized Block Design

The experiment

Compare two treatments in an experiment

• We assign treatments within schools

• With m schools with 2n students in each

• Assign n students to each treatment in each school

Page 74: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Randomized Block Design

Diagram of the design

Schools

Treatment 1 2 … m

1   …    

2     …    

Page 75: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Randomized Block Design

School 1

Schools

Treatment 1 2 … m

1   …  

2     …    

Page 76: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Conceptual Model

The statistical model for the observation on the kth person in the jth school in the ith treatment is

Yijk = μ +αi + βj + αβij + εijk

where

μ is the grand mean,

αi is the average effect of being in treatment i,

βj is the average effect of being in school j,

αβij is the difference between the average effect of treatment i and the effect of that treatment in school j,

εijk is a residual

Page 77: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Effect of Context

ijk i j ij ijkY

Context Effect

Page 78: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Two-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates (HLM Notation)

Level 1 (individual level)

Yijk = β0j + β1jTijk+ εijk ε ~ N(0, σW2)

Level 2 (school level)

β0j = π00 + ξ0j ξ0j ~ N(0, σS2)

β1j = π10+ ξ1j ξ1j ~ N(0, σTxS2)

If we code the treatment Tijk = ½ or - ½ , then the parameters are identical to those in standard ANOVA

Page 79: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Effects and EstimatesThe population mean of treatment 1 in school j is

α1 + αβ1j

The population mean of treatment 2 in school j is

α2 + αβ2j

The estimate of the mean of treatment 1 in school j is

α1 + αβ1j + ε1j●

The estimate of the mean of treatment 2 in school j is

α2 + αβ2j + ε2j●

Page 80: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Effects and EstimatesThe comparative treatment effect in any given school j is

(α1 – α2) + (αβ1j – αβ2j)

The estimate of comparative treatment effect in school j is

(α1 – α2) + (αβ1j – αβ2j) + (ε1j● – ε2j●)

The mean treatment effect in the experiment is

(α1 – α2) + (αβ1● – αβ2●)

The estimate of the mean treatment effect in the experiment is

(α1 – α2) + (αβ 1● – αβ2●) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

Page 81: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Inference Models

Two different kinds of inferences about effects

Unconditional Inference (Schools Random)Inference to the whole universe of schools(requires a representative sample of schools)

Conditional Inference (Schools Fixed)Inference to the schools in the experiment(no sampling requirement on schools)

Page 82: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Statistical Analysis Procedures

Two kinds of statistical analysis procedures

Mixed Effects Procedures (Schools Random)Treat schools in the experiment as a sample from a population of schools(only strictly correct if schools are a sample)

Fixed Effects Procedures (Schools Fixed)Treat schools in the experiment as a population

Page 83: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Unconditional Inference(Schools Random)

The estimate of the mean treatment effect in the experiment is

(α1 – α2) + (αβ 1● – αβ2●) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

The average treatment effect we want to estimate is

(α1 – α2)

The term (ε1●● – ε2●●) depends on the students in the schools in the sample

The term (αβ1● – αβ2●) depends on the schools in sample

Both (ε1●● – ε2●●) and (αβ1● – αβ2●) are random and average to 0 across students and schools, respectively

Page 84: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Conditional Inference(Schools Fixed)

The estimate of the mean treatment effect in the experiment is still

(α1 – α2) + (αβ 1● – αβ2●) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

Now the average treatment effect we want to estimate is

(α1 + αβ1●) – (α2 + αβ2●) = (α1 – α2) + (αβ1● – αβ2●)

The term (ε1●● – ε2●●) depends on the students in the schools in the sample

The term (αβ1● – αβ2●) depends on the schools in sample, but the treatment effect in the sample of schools is the effect we want to estimate

Page 85: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Expected Mean SquaresRandomized Block Design

(Two Levels, Schools Random)

Source   df   E{MS}

Treatment (T) 1 σW2 + nσTxS

2 + nmΣαi2

Schools (S) m – 1 σW2 + 2nσS

2

T x S m – 1 σW2 + nσTxS

2

Within Cells   2m(n – 1)   σW2

Page 86: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Mixed Effects Procedures(Schools Random)

The test for treatment effects has

H0: (α1 – α2) = 0

Estimated mean treatment effect in the experiment is

(α1 – α2) + (αβ1● – αβ2●) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

The variance of the estimated treatment effect is

2[σW2 + nσTxS

2] /mn = 2[1 + (nωS – 1)ρ]σ2/mn

Here ωS = σTxS2/σS

2 and ρ = σS2/(σS

2 + σW2) = σS

2/σ2

Page 87: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Mixed Effects Procedures

The test for treatment effects:

FT = MST/MSTxS with (m – 1) df

The test for context effects (treatment by schools interaction) is

FTxS = MSTxS/MSWS with 2m(n – 1) df

Power is determined by the operational effect size

where ωS = σTxS2/σS

2 and ρ = σS2/(σS

2 + σW2) = σS

2/σ2

1 2

1 ( 1)S

α α n

nω ρ

Page 88: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Expected Mean SquaresRandomized Block Design

(Two Levels, Schools Fixed)

Source   Df   E{MS}

Treatment (T) 1 σW2 + nmΣαi

2

Schools (S) m – 1 σW2 + 2nΣβi

2/(m – 1)

S x T m – 1 σW2 + nΣΣαβij

2/(m – 1)

Within Cells   2m(n – 1)   σW2

Page 89: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Fixed Effects Procedures

The test for treatment effects has

H0: (α1 – α2) + (αβ1● – αβ2●) = 0

Estimated mean treatment effect in the experiment is

(α1 – α2) + (αβ1● – αβ2●) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

The variance of the estimated treatment effect is

2σW2 /mn

Page 90: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Fixed Effects ProceduresThe test for treatment effects:

FT = MST/MSWS with m(n – 1) df

The test for context effects (treatment by schools interaction) is

FC = MSTxS/MSWS with 2m(n – 1) df

Power is determined by the operational effect size

with m(n – 1) df

1 2 1 2α α α αn

Page 91: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Comparing Fixed and Mixed Effects Statistical Procedures

(Randomized Block Design)

  Fixed Mixed

Inference Model Conditional Unconditional

Estimand (α1 – α2) + (αβ1● – αβ2●) (α1 – α2)

Contaminating Factors (ε1●● – ε2●●) (αβ1● – αβ2●) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

Operational Effect Size

df 2m(n – 1) (m – 1)

Power higher lower

1 2 1 2α α α αn

1 2

1 ( 1)S

α α n

nω ρ

Page 92: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Comparing Fixed and Mixed Effects Procedures(Randomized Block Design)

Conditional and unconditional inference models

• estimate different treatment effects

• have different contaminating factors that add uncertainty

Mixed procedures are good for unconditional inference

The fixed procedures are good for conditional inference

The fixed procedures have higher power

Page 93: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Hierarchical Design

The universe (the sampling frame)

We wish to compare two treatments

• We assign treatments to whole schools

• Many schools with n students in each

• Assign all students in each school to the same treatment

Page 94: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Hierarchical Design

The experiment

We wish to compare two treatments

• We assign treatments to whole schools

• Assign 2m schools with n students in each

• Assign all students in each school to the same treatment

Page 95: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Hierarchical Design

Diagram of the experiment

Schools

Treatment 1 2 … m m +1 m +2 … 2 m

1             

2               

Page 96: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Hierarchical Design

Treatment 1 schools

Schools

Treatment 1 2 … m m +1 m + 2 … 2 m

1             

2               

Page 97: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Hierarchical Design

Treatment 2 schools

Schools

Treatment 1 2 … m m + 1 m + 2 … 2 m

1             

2               

Page 98: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Conceptual Model

The statistical model for the observation on the kth person in the jth school in the ith treatment is

Yijk = μ + αi + βi + αβij + εjk(i) = μ + αi + βj(i) + εjk(i)

μ is the grand mean,

αi is the average effect of being in treatment i,

βj is the average effect if being in school j,

αβij is the difference between the average effect of treatment i and the effect of that treatment in school j,

εijk is a residual

Or βj(i) = βi + αβij is a term for the combined effect of schools within treatments

Page 99: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

The Conceptual Model

The statistical model for the observation on the kth person in the jth school in the ith treatment is

Yijk = μ + αi + βi + αβij + εjk(i) = μ + αi + βj(i) + εjk(i)

μ is the grand mean,

αi is the average effect of being in treatment i,

βj is the average effect if being in school j,

αβij is the difference between the average effect of treatment i and the effect of that treatment in school j,

εijk is a residual

or βj(i) = βi + αβij is a term for the combined effect of schools within treatments

Context Effects

Page 100: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Two-level Hierarchical DesignWith No Covariates (HLM Notation)

Level 1 (individual level)

Yijk = β0j + εijk ε ~ N(0, σW2)

Level 2 (school Level)

β0j = π00 + π01Tj + ξ0j ξ ~ N(0, σS2)

If we code the treatment Tj = ½ or - ½ , then

π00 = μ, π01 = α1, ξ0j = βj(i)

The intraclass correlation is ρ = σS2/(σS

2 + σW2) = σS

2/σ2

Page 101: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Effects and EstimatesThe comparative treatment effect in any given school j is still

(α1 – α2) + (αβ1j – αβ2j)

But we cannot estimate the treatment effect in a single school because each school gets only one treatment

The mean treatment effect in the experiment is

(α1 – α2) + (β●(1) – β●(2))

= (α1 – α2) +(β1● – β2● )+ (αβ1● – αβ2●)

The estimate of the mean treatment effect in the experiment is

(α1 – α2) + (β● (1) – β● (2)) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

Page 102: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Inference Models

Two different kinds of inferences about effects (as in the randomized block design)

Unconditional Inference (schools random)Inference to the whole universe of schools(requires a representative sample of schools)

Conditional Inference (schools fixed)Inference to the schools in the experiment(no sampling requirement on schools)

Page 103: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Unconditional Inference(Schools Random)

The average treatment effect we want to estimate is

(α1 – α2)

The term (ε1●● – ε2●●) depends on the students in the schools in the sample

The term (β●(1) – β●(2)) depends on the schools in sample

Both (ε1●● – ε2●●) and (β●(1) – β●(2)) are random and average to 0 across students and schools, respectively

Page 104: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Conditional Inference(Schools Fixed)

The average treatment effect we want to (can) estimate is

(α1 + β●(1)) – (α2 + β●(2)) = (α1 – α2) + (β●(1) – β●(2))

= (α1 – α2) + (β1● – β2● )+ (αβ1● – αβ2●)

The term (β●(1) – β●(2)) depends on the schools in sample, but we want to estimate the effect of treatment in the schools in the sample

Note that this treatment effect is not quite the same as in the randomized block design, where we estimate (α1 – α2) + (αβ1● – αβ2●)

Page 105: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Statistical Analysis Procedures

Two kinds of statistical analysis procedures (as in the randomized block design)

Mixed Effects Procedures

Treat schools in the experiment as a sample from a universe

Fixed Effects Procedures

Treat schools in the experiment as a universe

Page 106: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Expected Mean SquaresHierarchical Design

(Two Levels, Schools Random)

Source   df   E{MS}

Treatment (T) 1 σW2 + nσS

2 + nmΣαi2

Schools (S) 2(m – 1) σW2 + nσS

2

Within Schools 2m(n – 1) σW2

   

Page 107: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Mixed Effects Procedures(Schools Random)

The test for treatment effects has

H0: (α1 – α2) = 0

Estimated mean treatment effect in the experiment is

(α1 – α2) + (β●(1) – β●(2)) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

The variance of the estimated treatment effect is

2[σW2 + nσS

2] /mn = 2[1 + (n – 1)ρ]σ2/mn

where ρ = σS2/(σS

2 + σW2) = σS

2/σ2

Page 108: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Mixed Effects Procedures(Schools Random)

The test for treatment effects:

FT = MST/MSBS with (m – 2) df

There is no omnibus test for context effects

Power is determined by the operational effect size

where ρ = σS2/(σS

2 + σW2) = σS

2/σ2

1 2

1 ( 1)

α α n

n ρ

Page 109: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Expected Mean SquaresHierarchical Design

(Two Levels, Schools Fixed)

Source   df   E{MS}

Treatment (T) 1 σW2 + nmΣ(αi + β●(i))2

Schools (S) m – 1 σW2 + nΣΣβj(i)

2/2(m – 1)

Within Schools 2m(n – 1) σW2

   

Page 110: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Mixed Effects Procedures(Schools Fixed)

The test for treatment effects has

H0: (α1 – α2) + (β●(1) – β●(2)) = 0

Note that the school effects are confounded with treatment effects

Estimated mean treatment effect in the experiment is

(α1 – α2) + (β●(1) – β●(2)) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

The variance of the estimated treatment effect is 2σW

2 /mn

Page 111: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Mixed Effects Procedures(Schools Fixed)

The test for treatment effects:

FT = MST/MSWS with m(n – 1) df

There is no omnibus test for context effects, because each school gets only one treatment

Power is determined by the operational effect size

and m(n – 1) df

1 2 (1) (2)α αn

Page 112: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Comparing Fixed and Mixed Effects Procedures(Hierarchical Design)

  Fixed Mixed

Inference Model

Conditional Unconditional

Estimand (α1 – α2) + (β●(1) – β●(2)) (α1 – α2)

Contaminating Factors

(ε1●● – ε2●●) (β●(1) – β●(2)) + (ε1●● – ε2●●)

Effect Size

df m(n – 1) (m – 2)

Power higher lower

1 2

1 ( 1)

α α n

n ρ

1 2 (1) (2)α α

n

Page 113: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Comparing Fixed and Mixed Effects Statistical Procedures (Hierarchical Design)

Conditional and unconditional inference models

• estimate different treatment effects

• have different contaminating factors that add uncertainty

Mixed procedures are good for unconditional inference

The fixed procedures are not generally recommended

The fixed procedures have higher power

Page 114: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Comparing Hierarchical Designs to Randomized Block Designs

Randomized block designs usually have higher power, but assignment of different treatments within schools or classes may be

• practically difficult• politically infeasible• theoretically impossible

It may be methodologically unwise because of potential for

• Contamination or diffusion of treatments• compensatory rivalry or demoralization

Page 115: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Comparing Hierarchical Designs to Randomized Block Designs

But even when there is substantial contamination Chris Rhoads has shown that :

• even though randomized block designs underestimate the treatment effect

• randomized block designs can have higher power than hierarchical designs

This is not widely known yet, but is important to remember

Page 116: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Applications to Experimental Design

We will address the two most widely used experimental designs in education

• Randomized blocks designs with 2 levels

• Randomized blocks designs with 3 levels

• Hierarchical designs with 2 levels

• Hierarchical designs with 3 levels

We also examine the effect of covariates

Hereafter, we generally take schools to be random

Page 117: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Complications

Which matchings do we have to take into account in design (e.g., schools, districts, regions, states, regions of the country, country)?

Ignore some, control for effects of others as fixed blocking factors

Justify this as part of the population definition

For example, we define the inference population as these five districts within these two states

But, doing so obviously constrains generalizability

Page 118: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision of the Estimated Treatment Effect

Precision is the standard error of the estimated treatment effect

Precision in simple (simple random sample) designs depends on:

• Standard deviation in the population σ

• Total sample size N

The precision is

2SE N

Page 119: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision of the Estimated Treatment Effect

Precision in complex (clustered sample) designs depends on:

• The (total) standard deviation σT

• Sample size at each level of sampling (e.g., m clusters, n individuals per cluster)

• Intraclass correlation structure

It is a little harder to compute than in simple designs, but important because it helps you see what matters in design

Page 120: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Intraclass Correlations inTwo-level Designs

In two-level designs the intraclass correlation structure is determined by a single intraclass correlation

This intraclass correlation is the proportion of the total variance that is between schools (clusters)

Typical values of ρ are 0.1 to 0.25, so σS2 is typically 1/9 to

1/3 of σW2 but it has a big impact

2 2

2 2 2

S S

S W T

ρ

Page 121: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision in Two-level Hierarchical DesignWith No Covariates

The standard error of the treatment effect is

SE decreases as m (number of schools) increases

SE deceases as n increases, but only up to point

SE increases as ρ increases

2 1 ( 1)T

n ρSE

m n

Page 122: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

How Does Between-Cluster Variance Impact Precision?

Think about the standard error again

So even though σS2 is smaller than σW

2, it has a bigger

impact on the uncertainty of the treatment effect

Suppose σS2 is 1/10 of σS

2 (a pretty small value of ρ) if

n = 30, σS2 will have 3 times as big an effect on the

standard error as will σW2

222 1 ( 1) 2 1 2 W

T T Sn ρ ρ

SEm n m n m n

Page 123: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Statistical Power

Power in simple (simple random sample) designs depends on:

• Significance level

• Effect size

• Sample size

Look power up in a table for sample size and effect size

Page 124: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Fragment of Cohen’s Table 2.3.5

               

d

n 0.10 0.20 … 0.80 1.00 1.20 1.40

8 05 07 … 31 46 60 73

9 06 07 … 35 51 65 79

10 06 07 … 39 56 71 84

11 06 07 … 43 63 76 87

               

Page 125: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Computing Statistical Power

Power in complex (clustered sample) designs depends on: • Significance level

• Effect size δ

• Sample size at each level of sampling (e.g., m clusters, n individuals per cluster)

• Intraclass correlation structure

This makes it seem a lot harder to compute

Page 126: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Computing Statistical Power

Computing statistical power in complex designs is only a little harder than computing it for simple designs

Compute operational effect size (incorporates sample design information) ΔT

Look power up in a table for operational sample size and operational effect size

This is the same table that you use for simple designs

Page 127: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Two-level Hierarchical DesignWith No Covariates

Basic Idea:Operational Effect Size = (Effect Size) x (Design Effect)

ΔT = δ x (Design Effect)

For the two-level hierarchical design with no covariates

Operational sample size is number of schools (clusters)

1 1

T n

n ρ

1 1

T n

n ρ

Page 128: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Two-level Hierarchical DesignWith No Covariates

As m (number of schools) increases, power increases

As effect size increases, power increases

Other influences occur through the design effect

As ρ increases the design effect (and power) decreases

No matter how large n gets the maximum design effect is

Thus power only increases up to some limit as n increases

1 1

1

1 1 (1 )n n

n

n ρ

1/ ρ

Page 129: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Optimal Allocation in the Two-level Hierarchical Design

Many different combinations of m and n give the same power or precision

How should we choose?

Optimal allocation gives some guidance

Suppose cost per individual is c1 and cost per school is c2, so total cost is 2mc2 + 2mnc1

gives the optimal n (most precision with smallest cost)

2

1

1

O

cn

c

Page 130: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Optimal Allocation in the Two-level Hierarchical Design

The optimal sample size n is often much smaller than you might think

For example, if ρ = 0.20

• nO = 14 if c2 = 50c1

• nO = 6 if c2 = 10c1

• nO = 2 if c2 = c1

But remember that optimality is only one factor in choosing sample sizes

Practicality and robustness of the sample (e.g., to attrition) are also important considerations

Page 131: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Two-level Hierarchical DesignWith Covariates (HLM Notation)

Level 1 (individual level)

Yijk = β0j + β1jXijk+ εijk ε ~ N(0, σAW2)

Level 2 (school Level)

β0j = π00 + π01Tj + π02Wj + ξ0j ξ ~ N(0, σAS2)

β1j = π10

Note that the covariate effect β1j = π10 is a fixed effect

If we code the treatment Tj = ½ or - ½ , then the parameters are identical to those in standard ANCOVA

Page 132: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision in Two-level Hierarchical DesignWith Covariates

The standard error of the treatment effect

SE decreases as m increases

SE deceases as n increases, but only up to point

SE increases as ρ increases

SE decreases as RW2 and RS

2 increase

2 21 12

2W S W

T

n ρ R nR RSE

m n

Page 133: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Two-level Hierarchical DesignWith Covariates

Basic Idea:

Operational Effect Size = (Effect Size) x (Design Effect)

ΔT = δ x (Design Effect)

For the two-level hierarchical design with covariates

The covariates increase the design effect

2 21 1

TA 2

W S W

n

n ρ R nR R

Page 134: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Two-level Hierarchical DesignWith Covariates

As m and effect size increase, power increases

Other influences occur through the design effect

As ρ increases the design effect (and power) decrease

Now the maximum design effect as large n gets big is

As the covariate-outcome correlations RW2 and RS

2 increase, the design effect (and power) increases

21 (1 )SR ρ

2 21 1 2W S W

n

n ρ R nR R

Page 135: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Optimal Allocation in the Two-level Hierarchical Design With Covariates

Optimal allocation can also be computed when there are covariates to give some guidance on cluster size (n)

Suppose cost per individual is c1 and cost per school is c2, so total cost is 2mc2 + 2mnc1

Then the optimal cluster size

gives the optimal n (most precision with smallest cost)

2

221

1 1

1

WO

S

Rcn

c R

Page 136: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Hierarchical Design

Here there are three factors• Treatment• Schools (clusters) nested in treatments• Classes (subclusters) nested in schools

Suppose there are• m schools (clusters) per treatment• p classes (subclusters) per school (cluster)• n students (individuals) per class (subcluster)

Page 137: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Hierarchical DesignWith No Covariates

The statistical model for the observation on the lth person in the kth class in the jth school in the ith treatment is

Yijkl = μ + αi + βj(i) + γk(ij) + εijkl

where μ is the grand mean, αi is the average effect of being in treatment i, βj(i) is the average effect of being in school j, in treatment i γk(ij) is the average effect of being in class k in treatment i, in

school j, εijkl is a residual

Page 138: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Hierarchical DesignWith No Covariates (HLM Notation)

Level 1 (individual level)

Yijkl = β0jk + εijkl ε ~ N(0, σW2)

Level 2 (classroom level)

β0jk = γ0j + η0jk η ~ N(0, σC2)

Level 3 (school Level)

γ0j = π00 + π01Tj + ξ0j ξ ~ N(0, σS2)

If we code the treatment Tj = ½ or - ½ , then

π00 = μ, π01 = α1, ξ0j = γk(ij), η0jk = βj(i)

Page 139: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Hierarchical Design Intraclass Correlations

In three-level designs there are two levels of clustering and two intraclass correlations

At the school (cluster) level

At the classroom (subcluster) level

2 2

2 2 2 2S S

SS C W T

ρ

2 2

2 2 2 2C C

CS C W T

ρ

Page 140: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision in Three-level Hierarchical DesignWith No Covariates

The standard error of the treatment effect

SE decreases as m increases

SE deceases as p and n increase, but only up to point

SE increases as ρS and ρC increase

1 1 ( 1)2 S CT

pn ρ nSE

m pn

Page 141: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Three-level Hierarchical DesignWith No Covariates

Basic Idea:

Operational Effect Size = (Effect Size) x (Design Effect)

ΔT = δ x (Design Effect)

For the three-level hierarchical design with no covariates

The operational sample size is the number of schools

1 ( 1) 1

T

S C

pn

pn n ρ

Page 142: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Three-level Hierarchical DesignWith No Covariates

As m and the effect size increase, power increases

Other influences occur through the design effect

As ρS or ρC increases the design effect decreases

No matter how large n gets the maximum design effect is

Thus power only increases up to some limit as n increases

1 ( 1) 1 S C

pn

pn n ρ

11 S Cp

Page 143: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Optimal Allocation in the Three-level Hierarchical Design With No Covariates

Optimal allocation can also be computed in three level designs to give guidance on (p and n)

Suppose cost per individual is c1 , the cost per class is c2, and the cost per school is c3, so total cost is 2mc3 + 2mpc2 + 2mpnc1

Then the optimal sample sizes size (most precision with smallest cost) are

And

2

1

1

S C

OC

cn

c

3

2

C

OS

cp

c

Page 144: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Hierarchical DesignWith Covariates (HLM Notation)

Level 1 (individual level)

Yijkl = β0jk + β1jkXijkl + εijkl ε ~ N(0, σAW2)

Level 2 (classroom level)

β0jk = γ00j + γ01jZjk + η0jk η ~ N(0, σAC2)

β1jk = γ10j

Level 3 (school Level)

γ00j = π00 + π01Tj + π02Wj + ξ0j ξ ~ N(0, σAS2)

γ01j = π01

γ10j = π10

The covariate effects β1jk = γ10j = π10 and γ01j = π01 are fixed

Page 145: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision in Three-level Hierarchical DesignWith Covariates

SE decreases as m increases

SE deceases as p and n increase, but only up to point

SE increases as ρS and ρC increase

SE decreases as RW2, RC

2, and RS2 increase

2 2 2 2

2

1 ( 1) 1

T

2S W S W S C W C

SEm

pn n ρ R pnR R nR R

pn

Page 146: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Three-level Hierarchical DesignWith Covariates

Basic Idea:

Operational Effect Size = (Effect Size) x (Design Effect)

ΔT = δ x (Design Effect)

For the three-level hierarchical design with covariates

The operational sample size is the number of schools

2 2 2 21 ( 1) 1

TA 2

S W S W S C W C

pn

pn n ρ R pnR R nR R

Page 147: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Three-level Hierarchical DesignWith Covariates

As m and the effect size increase, power increases

Other influences occur through the design effect

As ρS or ρC increase the design effect decreases

No matter how large n gets the maximum design effect is

Thus power only increases up to some limit as n increases

2 211 1 1S S C CpR R

2 2 2 21 ( 1) 1 2

S W S W S C W C

pn

pn n ρ R pnR R nR R

Page 148: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Optimal Allocation in the Three-level Hierarchical Design With Covariates

Optimal allocation can also be computed in three level designs to give guidance on (p and n)

Suppose cost per individual is c1 , the cost per class is c2, and the cost per school is c3, so total cost is 2mc3 + 2mpc2 + 2mpnc1

Then the optimal sample sizes size (most precision with smallest cost) are

and

.

2

221

1 1

1

W S CO

C C

Rcn

c R

23

22

1

1

C CO

S S

Rcp

c R

Page 149: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomized Block Designs

Page 150: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Two-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates (HLM Notation)

Level 1 (individual level)

Yijk = β0j + β1jTijk+ εijk ε ~ N(0, σW2)

Level 2 (school Level)

β0j = π00 + ξ0j ξ0j ~ N(0, σS2)

β1j = π10+ ξ1j ξ1j ~ N(0, σTxS2)

If we code the treatment Tijk = ½ or - ½ , then the parameters are identical to those in standard ANOVA

Page 151: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomized Block Designs

In randomized block designs, as in hierarchical designs, the intraclass correlation has an impact on precision and power

However, in randomized block designs designs there is also a parameter reflecting the degree of heterogeneity of treatment effects across schools

We define this heterogeneity parameter ωS in terms of the amount of heterogeneity of treatment effects relative to the heterogeneity of school means

Thus

ωS = σTxS2/σS

2

Page 152: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Randomized Block Designs

There are other ways to express this heterogeneity of treatment effect parameter

For example, (random effects) meta-analyses may give you direct access to an estimate of the variance of effect sizes (τ2)

A direct argument shows that

which gives ωS in terms of τ2

2 1S

τ ρω

ρ

Page 153: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision in Two-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates

The standard error of the treatment effect

SE decreases as m (number of schools) increases

SE deceases as n and p increase, but only up to point

SE increases as ρ increases

SE increases as ωS = σTxS2/σS

2 increases

1 ( 1)2 S

Tn ρ

SEm n

Page 154: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

How Does Between-Cluster Variance Impact Precision?

Think about the standard error again

So even though σTxS2 is smaller than σW

2, it has a bigger

impact on the uncertainty of the treatment effect

Suppose σTxS2 is 1/10 of σW

2 (a pretty small value) if n

= 30, σTxS2 will have 3 times as big an effect on the

standard error as will σW2

221 ( 1)2 2 1 2S W

T T S T Sn ρ ρ

SE ρ+m n m n m n

Page 155: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Two-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates

Basic Idea:Operational Effect Size = (Effect Size) x (Design Effect)

ΔT = δ x (Design Effect)

For the two-level randomized block design with no covariates

Operational sample size is number of schools (clusters)

1 1

T n

n ρ

/ 2

1 1

T

S

n

n ρ

Page 156: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision in Two-level Randomized Block DesignWith Covariates

The standard error of the treatment effect

SE decreases as m increases

SE deceases as n increases, but only up to point

SE increases as ρ increases

SE increases as ωS = σTxS2/σS

2 increases

SE (generally) decreases as RW2 and RTS

2 increase

2 21 12

2S W S TS W

T

n ρ R n R RSE

m n

Page 157: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Two-level Randomized Block DesignWith Covariates

Basic Idea:

Operational Effect Size = (Effect Size) x (Design Effect)

ΔT = δ x (Design Effect)

For the two-level randomized block design with covariates

The covariates increase the design effect

2 2

/ 2

1 1

TA 2

S W S TS W

n

n ρ R n R R

Page 158: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Optimal Allocation in the Two-level Randomized Block Design

Optimal allocation can also provide guidance on sample size allocation in randomized block designs

Suppose cost per individual is c1 and cost per school is c2, so total cost is mc2 + 2mnc1

gives the optimal n (most precision with smallest cost)

2

221

1 1

2 1

WO

TS S

Rcn

c R

Page 159: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Randomized Block Designs(Assigning Classes to Treatments)

Page 160: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Randomized Block Designs(Assigning Classes to Treatments)

We will only discuss the randomized block design that assigns classrooms to treatments within schools

You could also assign individuals within classes to treatments

That yields another randomized block design

We will not discuss that design here

Page 161: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates

Here there are three factors• Treatment• Schools (clusters) crossed with treatments• Classes (subclusters) nested in schools and

treatments

Suppose there are• m schools (clusters) per treatment• 2p classes (subclusters) per school (cluster)• n students (individuals) per class (subcluster)

Page 162: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates

The statistical model for the observation on the lth person in the kth class in the ith treatment in the jth school is

Yijkl = μ +αi + βj + γk(ij) + αβij + εijkl

where μ is the grand mean, αi is the average effect of being in treatment i, βj is the average effect of being in school j,

γk(ij) is the effect of being in the kth class,αβij is the difference between the average effect of

treatment i and the effect of that treatment in school j, εijkl is a residual

Page 163: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates (HLM Notation)

Level 1 (individual level)

Yijkl = β0jk + εijkl ε ~ N(0, σW2)

Level 2 (classroom level)

β0jk = γ00j + γ01jTj + η0jk η ~ N(0, σC2)

Level 3 (school Level)

γ00j = π00 + ξ0j ξoj ~ N(0, σS2)

γ01j = π10 + ξ1j ξ1j ~ N(0, σTxS2)

If we code the treatment Tj = ½ or - ½ , then

π00 = μ, π10 = α1, ξ0j = βj , ξ1j = αβij , η0jk = γk(ij)

Page 164: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Randomized Block Design Intraclass Correlations

In three-level designs there are two levels of clustering and two intraclass correlations

At the school (cluster) level

At the classroom (subcluster) level

2 2

2 2 2 2S S

SS C W T

ρ

2 2

2 2 2 2C C

CS C W T

ρ

Page 165: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Randomized Block Design Heterogeneity Parameters

In three-level designs, as in two-level randomized block designs, there is also a parameter reflecting the degree of heterogeneity of treatment effects across schools

We define this parameter ωS in terms of the amount of heterogeneity of treatment effects relative to the heterogeneity of school means (just like in two-level designs)

Thus

ωS = σTxS2/σS

2

Page 166: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Three-level Randomized Block Design Heterogeneity Parameters

There are other ways to express this heterogeneity of treatment effect parameter

For example, (random effects) meta-analyses of studies that assign classes to treatments may give you direct access to an estimate of the variance of effect sizes (τ2)

A direct argument shows that in this design

which gives ωS in terms of τ2

2 1 S C

SS

τ ρ ρω

ρ

Page 167: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Precision in Three-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates

The standard error of the treatment effect

SE decreases as m increases

SE deceases as p and n increase, but only up to point

SE increases as ωS increases

SE increases as ρS and ρC increase

1 1 ( 1)2 S S CT

pn ρ nSE

m pn

Page 168: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Three-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates

Basic Idea:

Operational Effect Size = (Effect Size) x (Design Effect)

ΔT = δ x (Design Effect)

For the three-level randomized block design with no covariates

The operational sample size is the number of schools

/ 2

1 ( 1) 1T

S S C

pn

pn n ρ

Page 169: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Three-level Randomized Block DesignWith No Covariates

As m and the effect size increase, power increases

Other influences occur through the design effect

As ρS or ρC increases the design effect decreases

No matter how large n gets the maximum design effect is

Thus power only increases up to some limit as n increases

/ 2

1 ( 1) 1S S C

pn

pn n ρ

11 2 S S Cp

Page 170: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Three-level Randomized Block DesignWith Covariates

SE decreases as m increases

SE deceases as p and n increases, but only up to point

SE increases as ρS, ρC, and ωS increase

SE decreases as RW2, RC

2, and RTS2 increase

2 2 2 2

2

1 ( 1) 1

T

2S S W S TS W S C W C

SEm

pn n ρ R pn R R nR R

pn

Page 171: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Three-level Randomized Block DesignWith Covariates

Basic Idea:

Operational Effect Size = (Effect Size) x (Design Effect)

ΔT = δ x (Design Effect)

For the three-level randomized block design with covariates

The operational sample size is the number of schools

2 2 2 2

/ 2

1 ( 1) 1

TA

2S S W S TS W S C W C

pn

pn n ρ R pn R R nR R

Page 172: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Power in Three-level Randomized Block DesignWith Covariates

As m and the effect size increase, power increases

Other influences occur through the design effect

As ρS or ρC increases the design effect decreases

No matter how large n gets the maximum design effect is

Thus power only increases up to some limit as n increases

2 211 2 1 1 TS S S C CpR R

2 2 2 2

/ 2

1 ( 1) 1 2

S S W S TS W S C W C

pn

pn n ρ R pn R R nR R

Page 173: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Optimal Allocation in the Three-level Randomized Block Designs With Covariates

Optimal allocation can also be computed in three level randomized block designs to give guidance on (p and n)

Suppose cost per individual is c1 , the cost per class is c2, and the cost per school is c3, so total cost is mc3 + 2mpc2 + 2mpnc1

Then the optimal sample sizes size (most precision with smallest cost) are

and

.

2

221

1 1

1

W S CO

C C

Rcn

c R

23

22

1

1

C CO

TS S

Rcp

c R

Page 174: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

What Unit Should Be Randomized?(Schools, Classrooms, or Students)

Experiments cannot estimate the causal effect on any individual

Experiments estimate average causal effects on the units that have been randomized

• If you randomize schools the (average) causal effects are effects on schools

• If you randomize classes, the (average) causal effects are on classes

• If you randomize individuals, the (average) causal effects estimated are on individuals

Page 175: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

What Unit Should Be Randomized?(Schools, Classrooms, or Students)

Theoretical Considerations

Decide what level you care about, then randomize at that level

Randomization at lower levels may impact generalizability of the causal inference (and it is generally a lot more trouble)

Suppose you randomize classrooms, should you also randomly assign students to classes?

It depends: Are you interested in the average causal effect of treatment on naturally occurring classes or on randomly assembled ones?

Page 176: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

What Unit Should Be Randomized?(Schools, Classrooms, or Students)

Relative power/precision of treatment effect

Assign Schools(Hierarchical Design)

Assign Classrooms(Randomized Block)

Assign Students(Randomized Block)

1 1 ( 1)S Cpn ρ n

pn

1 1 ( 1)S S C Cpn ρ n

pn

1 1 ( 1)S S Cpn ρ n

pn

Page 177: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

What Unit Should Be Randomized?(Schools, Classrooms, or Students)

Precision of estimates or statistical power dictate assigning the lowest level possible

But the individual (or even classroom) level will not always be feasible or even theoretically desirable

Page 178: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Questions and Answers About Design

Page 179: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Questions and Answers About Design

1. Is it ok to match my schools (or classes) before I randomize to decrease variation?

2. I assigned treatments to schools and am not using classes in the analysis. Do I have to take them into account in the design?

3. I am assigning schools, and using every class in the school. Do I have to include classes as a nested factor?

4. My schools all come from two districts, but I am randomly assigning the schools. Do I have to take district into account some way?

Page 180: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Questions and Answers About Design

1. I didn’t really sample the schools in my experiment (who does?). Do I still have to treat schools as random effects?

2. I didn’t really sample my schools, so what population can I generalize to anyway?

3. I am using a randomized block design with fixed effects. Do you really mean I can’t say anything about effects in schools that are not in the sample?

Page 181: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Questions and Answers About Design

1. We randomly assigned, but our assignment was corrupted by treatment switchers. What do we do?

2. We randomly assigned, but our assignment was corrupted by attrition. What do we do?

3. We randomly assigned but got a big imbalance on characteristics we care about (gender, race, language, SES). What do we do?

4. We randomly assigned but when we looked at the pretest scores, we see that we got a big imbalance (a “bad randomization”). What do we do?

Page 182: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Questions and Answers About Design

1. We care about treatment effects, but we really want to know about mechanism. How do we find out if implementation impacts treatment effects?

2. We want to know where (under what conditions) the treatment works. Can we analyze the relation between conditions and treatment effect to find this out?

3. We have a randomized block design and find heterogeneous treatment effects. What can we say about the main effect of treatment in the presence of interactions?

Page 183: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Questions and Answers About Design

1. I prefer to use regression and I know that regression and ANOVA are equivalent. Why do I need all this ANOVA stuff to design and analyze experiments?

2. Don’t robust standard errors in regression solve all these problems?

3. I have heard of using “school fixed effects” to analyze a randomized block design. Is the a good alternative to ANOVA or HLM?

4. Can I use school fixed effects in a hierarchical design?

Page 184: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Questions and Answers About Design

1. We want to use covariates to improve precision, but we find that they act somewhat differently in different groups (have different slopes). What do we do?

2. We get somewhat different variances in different groups. Should we use robust standard errors?

3. We get somewhat different answers with different analyses. What do we do?

Page 185: Basic Experimental Design Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 26, 2010.

Thank You !