Bahaya video games kekerasan

17
1

description

Presentasi di Santa Laurensia, BSD

Transcript of Bahaya video games kekerasan

Page 1: Bahaya video games kekerasan

1

Page 2: Bahaya video games kekerasan

“The man accused of killing at least 93 people in Norway described the video game Call of Duty: Modern Warfare 2 as "part of my training-simulation" in his 1500-page manifesto published online just before the massacre” (Birmingham Metro, 2011)

“Stefan Martin-Urban... pulled a pistol from behind his back and methodically shot strangers, his behaviour was eerily similar to characters in the video games he played obsessively”. (Denver Post, 2009)

In 2006, a bill prohibiting the sale or rental of games that portray “killing, maiming, dismembering or sexually assaulting an image of a human being” to people younger than 18 years old was passed in California. This was overturned in 2011.

But is this just Moral Panic? What is the impact of violent video games on gamers?

2

Page 3: Bahaya video games kekerasan

Video game genres: sports stimulations, racers, adventures, puzzlers, miscellaneous, platformers,, beat ‘em ups and shoot ‘em ups (Griffiths, 1993)

Effects video games larger than those associated with TV & film viewing

Increasingly sophisticated graphics: less abstract

Interactive nature of games Online and portable Structural characteristics Video game addiction 3

Page 4: Bahaya video games kekerasan

Video games as most popular entertainment form (Prachett, 2005),

Time spent playing continuing to increase (Escobar-Chaves & Anderson, 2008).

Almost 30% of 9 year old boys (compared with 12% of girls) were reported as spending one or more hours daily playing these games (Growing Up in Ireland, 2011)

Disregard stereotype of male, teenage gamer profile (Griffiths, Davies and Chappell , 2003)

But what are the effects of playing....is it something of concern?

Research with children

Boys Girls

(1985) Harris & Williams

4 hours per week

similar

(1996) Buchanan & Harris

7 hours 4.5 hours

(2007) Anderson, Gentile & Buckley

13 hours 5.5 hours

(2008)Gentile et al.

12.4 hours14.6 hours

4

Page 5: Bahaya video games kekerasan

Games are particularly attractive platforms to young people, they offer an attractive forum for learning (Swing & Anderson, 2008).

Video game industry has started to recognise and design games aimed at improving

i) Cognition and learning: support multi-sensory, active, and experiential and problem-based learning & activation of prior knowledge.

ii) Health Benefits: Physiotherapy/OT/Distract

iii) Social Benefits: Displacement hypothesis 5

Page 6: Bahaya video games kekerasan

89% of games in the market may contain elements of violence, and almost half of these games may include a form of serious violence taking place against another character (Children Now, 2001).

Aggressive Behaviour, Aggressive Cognition Aggressive Affect. School performance, peer

relations, ability to take others perspective and/or sympathise , increased arousal.

6

Page 7: Bahaya video games kekerasan

A reduction in helping/prosocial behaviour and empathy and an increase in condoning of attitudes towards violence.

Adults, adolescents & childrenCorrelation with more positive

attitudes towards violence in wars, intimate partner violence and general normative aggressive beliefs

Neural desensitization to violence predicts increased aggression following violent video game exposure (Engelhardt, 2011)

7

Page 8: Bahaya video games kekerasan

Which types of victims elicit higher levels of blame and lack of empathy and understanding from others ?

Victims of sexual violenceGender differences Victims who are children: Bullying researchPrevious research has explored the impact on young

people’s attitudes towards criminals (Lee et al, 2010) and towards crime

Viera & Krcmar (2011) the structure of violent video games limit children’s abilities to develop perspectives of victims

Greitemeyer & McLatchie (2011) : Dehumanising process

8

Page 9: Bahaya video games kekerasan

What is the impact of violent video game play on attitudes towards victims of crime?

Address criticism of the research in the field

Gamers who play violent video games will exhibit less concern for different types of victims than gamers who play non-violent video games.

Gender and Age differences in concern scores

9

Page 10: Bahaya video games kekerasan

Sample: 206 participants (129 males and 76 females) The participants were aged between 12 and 24 years (mean = 16.85

years; SD = 2.57 years).

Materials: i) Victim Concern Scale: Clemments et al (2006); 21-item scale asking

participants to rate their concern for different types of victims. General Victims Vulnerable or Violent Crime Victims Property Theft Victims Culpable Victims

ii) Violent Video Game Exposure: Dill & Anderson (2000): an overall violent video game exposure score for each participant

10

Page 11: Bahaya video games kekerasan

Male and female mean victim concern scores

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

Male Female

Overall

General

Vulnerable

Culpable

Property

Concern for Victims

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70Victim Concern

11

Page 12: Bahaya video games kekerasan

Significant negative correlation between a) exposure and overall concern (r = -.167, p<0.05), b) exposure and concern for general victims (r = -.302,

p<0.05) c) exposure and victim concern for vulnerable

victims , r =.154, p<0.05 ) d) for culpable victims, r =-.238, p<0.05.

This showed that high levels of violent game play were associated with lower levels of overall victim concern, concern for general victims, vulnerable victims and for culpable victims.

12

Page 13: Bahaya video games kekerasan

The results suggest that young people who play more violent video games have less concern for general victims and for culpable victims, and these effects cannot be explained by gender or age differences in the participant sample.

Young people who play violent video games also reported less concern for victims overall and for vulnerable victims and this effect was not due to age differences.*

Males: less concern for culpable and general victimsAge differences in terms of types of victims This study is the first to explore directly young people’s

exposure to violent video games and their concern for victims of crime

Less concern for what could be considered the less serious crimes or those who they may believe may be less affected by these crimes.

13

Page 14: Bahaya video games kekerasan

The long-term exposure to violent video games may lead to people developing particular views of culpability and levels of blame, with higher levels of culpability(and therefore less concern) evident as young people played more violent video games

Individual experiences that people have playing violent video games

Gender differences Greater exploration of attitudes Long term effects

14

Page 15: Bahaya video games kekerasan

Most young people spend more time watching screen media than in any other activity apart from sleeping (Strasberg et al, 2009).

Build protective factors Parental role in monitoring the media use.” A new bill has been submitted to the U.S. House of

Representatives that will require most video games to include a warning label that states: "WARNING: Exposure to violent video games has been linked to aggressive behaviour.“ CBS News March 20th 2012

But what about attitudes?Definition of harm and impact of harm changing? Individual and Societal Implications of findings

15

Page 16: Bahaya video games kekerasan

16

“I could play it for hours, I don't know what it is that makes it so addictive. I think its the fact that you play the bad guy and have to complete all the

missions and try not to get caught. Its not really hurting anyone though, right?”

Female Gamer (Aged, 20 years)

Page 17: Bahaya video games kekerasan

Anderson, C.A., & Dill, K.E. (2000). Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings and behaviour in the labratory and in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 772-790.

Anderson, C. A., Gentile, D. A., & Buckley, K. E. (2007). Violent video game effects on children and adolescents: Theory, research, and public policy. New York: Oxford University Press.

Clements, C. B., Brannen, D. N., Kirkley, S. M., Gordon, T. M. and Church, W. T. (2006), The measurement of concern about victims: Empathy, victim advocacy and the Victim Concern Scale (VCS). Legal and Criminological Psychology, 11: 283–295.

Engelhardt, C.E., Bartholow, B.D., & Saults, J.S. (2011). Violent and nonviolent video games differentially affect physical aggression for individuals high versus low in dispositional anger. Aggressive Behavior, 37, 539-546.

Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., Yukawa, S., Ihori, N., Saleem, M., Ming, L. K., Liau, A. K., Khoo, A., Bushman, B. J., Huesmann, L. R., & Sakamoto, A. (2009). The effects of prosocial video games on prosocial behaviors: International evidence from correlational, longitudinal, and experimental studies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(6), 752-763.

Greitemeyer, T. and McLatchie, N. (2011). Denying humanness to others: A newly discovered mechanism by which violent video games increase aggressive behavior. Psychological Science, 22(5):659-665.

Griffiths, M. D. (1993). Are computer games bad for children? The Psychologist: Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 6, 401–407.

Harris, M.B. & Williams, R. (1985). Video games and school performance. Education, 105(3), 306-309.

Strasburg, V.C., Wilson, B.J. & Jordan, A.B. (2009). Children, Adolescents and the Media. (2nd Ed). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Viera, E.T. & Krcmar, M. (2011) The Influences of Video Gaming on US Children's Moral Reasoning About Violence Journal of Children and Media, 5(2), pp 113-131

17