A.primate communication

27
Forms and meaning of primate communication Presenter: Roya Shariati ANU College of Archaeology and Anthropology Supervisor: Prof. Colin Groves

Transcript of A.primate communication

Page 1: A.primate communication

Forms and meaning of primate communication

Presenter: Roya ShariatiANU College of Archaeology and

AnthropologySupervisor: Prof. Colin Groves

Page 2: A.primate communication

What is Communication?

Communication is the transfer of information from one Primate

to another. The message or signal is passed between a sender

and a receiver. The ability to detect and respond to stimuli is

essential for survival. By detecting changes in the external

environment, humans and other animals are able to respond

appropriately.

Page 3: A.primate communication

Form Of Communication

GesturesFacial ExpressionGaze FollowingVocalizationOlfactory communicationElectro Communication

Pictures from science daily web and wikipidia

Pictures from Sciencedaily website and Wikipedia

Page 4: A.primate communication

Vocalization:

Many of primates communicate through vocalizations.

Primate communication also involves mental representation in some

cases, most clearly in the representational vocalizations used by

primate species.

Vocalization are believed to serve a variety of functions, including

territorial advertisement; intergroup intimidation and spacing;

announcing the precise locality of specific individuals, food sources, or

danger; and strengthening intragroup cohesion.

Page 5: A.primate communication

Vocal organs (Chiba & Kiiama 1940)• Non-human primates have vocal organs similar to ours, but

– the size of their tongue relative to the size of the mouth is much larger

– their pharynx (area between the larynx and the mouth) is shorter

Page 6: A.primate communication

Primate vocalizations

• Both non-human primates and humans use the larynx for the source of sound production and the oral cavity above the larynx as the filter.

Kratzenstein‘s geometric-acoustic considerations based on reflections within elliptical cones (Gessinger 1994)

• Non-human primates have much larger tongue: not advantageous in manipulating the shape of the vocal tract.

• Non-human primates have shorter pharynx: less room to play with the shape of the vocal tract.

– But they have lower risk of choking on food

Page 7: A.primate communication

Source-filter theoryThe source-filter theory describes speech production as a two stage process involving the generation of a sound source, with its own spectral shape and spectral fine structure, which is then shaped or filtered by the resonant properties of the vocal tract.

The vowel, its nature and structure / by Tsutomu Chiba and Masato Kajiyama 1941

Page 8: A.primate communication

Source-filter theory: illustration

Page 9: A.primate communication

Source filter theory and vocalization

• So what a vocalization sounds like depends on whatthe sound source is like and how that source is filtered

• The source can be created by vibrating the vocal foldsin the larynx

– How fast the vocal folds vibrate will determine the fundamental frequency of the sound

• The shape of the vocal tract above the larynx determines the nature of the filter – which frequencies are amplified and which are attenuated

– One can make sounds of equal pitch but different quality (timbre)

Page 10: A.primate communication

Barks and pant-threats• Lips are slightly protruded and maximally spread

apart at the beginning of each sound– The lower jaw, or the mandible drops to the lowest point– This is more prominent for barks than for

pant-threats– The teeth are partially separated during the production

• Barks are more likely to be associated with physical aggression

Page 11: A.primate communication

Noisy and undulating screams

• Produced by individuals being threatened by a dominant member of the group

– Noisy screams are more consistently associated with physical attack than undulating screams

• The lips are retracted, revealing the teeth– Lip retraction appears more prominent for

noisy scream than for undulating scream

Page 12: A.primate communication

Coos and grunts• Coos and grunts are produced during group

movement, affiliative activities, and when one approaches another to groom

• When producing coos, the lips are separated fairly wide and somewhat protruded while the mandible is lowered

• When producing grunts, lips are separated, often to a negligible degree, and there is no lip protrusion

Page 13: A.primate communication

links• Rhesus monkey callshttp://www.wjh.harvard.edu/~mnkylab/media/rhesuscalls.htmlhttp://www.soundboard.com/sb/Rhesus_Monkey_sounds.aspx

• Rhesus monkey articulation– Hauser et al. (1993). The role of articulation in the production ofrhesus monkey, Macaca mulatta, vocalizations. Anim. Behav., 45,423-433.http://galliform.psy.mq.edu.au/Reprints/Hauser_Evans&Marler_1993.pdf

• – Lieberman et al. (1980). Vocal tract limitations on the vowel repertoires of rhesus monkey and other nonhuman primates, Science, 164, 1185-1187.

http://web.haskins.yale.edu/Reprints/HL0080.pdf

Page 14: A.primate communication

Call Context

• Food Many primates produce distinct calls when discovering or eating food.

• Predators Many primates produce alarm calls in response to predator (Cheney and

Seyfarth, 1990),

• Mating In most of species both sexes produce copulation calls (Hauser, 1996)

Caller Identity

• Individual• Sex• Group membership

Page 15: A.primate communication

Rhesus food discovery vocalizations

• When rhesus macaques discover food, they call out, at which point other group members come running

• Hauser (1996) catalogued five different food calls– ‘warbles’, ‘chirps’, and ‘harmonic arches’ are

associated with rare and high-quality foods (e.g. coconut)– ‘coos’ and ‘grunts’ are for common, low-quality

foods

• ‘Silent discoverers’ who are caught with food are severely beaten up, and this is true for high and low ranking individuals

• Males make food calls less often than females

Page 16: A.primate communication

Food calls are intentional, not reflexive

• When individuals discover food, they first scan the area, looking for enemies and/or allies

• Members of social groups have different calling behaviours than males who live peripherally, waiting to join a social group

– When peripheral males find food, they never call

– These peripheral silent discoverers never receive targeted aggression

• These observations suggest that rhesus macaques can intentionally withhold information about food discoveries

Page 17: A.primate communication

Rhesus mating vocalizations

• Males produce extremely loud and individually distinctive screams during mating

• Hauser (1996) found that males who call during copulation obtain significantly more mating than males who were silent

• However, vocal males also received significantly more aggression than did silent males

• This suggests there are potential survival costs to calling (natural selection pressure), but potential reproductive benefits (sexual selection pressure)

Page 18: A.primate communication

Gibbon songs

• Gibbons, unique among non-human primates, produce complex songs in order to attract mates, advertise pair bonds (duets), repel introducers, and warn each other of predators

• http://www.gibbons.de/main/sound/sounds/07hool_sounds/hoDuett88a2027.wav

Page 19: A.primate communication

Gibbons (Hylobates spp.) produce loud

and long song bouts that are mostly

exhibited by mated pairs. Typically,

mates combine their partly sex-specific

repetoire in relatively rigid, precisely

timed, and complex vocal interactions to

produce well-patterned duets. A cross-

species comparison reveals that singing

behaviour evolved several times

independently in the order of primates.

Most likely, loud calls were the substrate

from which singing evolved in each line.

(Geissmann, T., 2000).Singing male white-handed gibbon (Hylobates lar, Zoo

Rapperswil)

Page 20: A.primate communication

Another specialization is the occurrence of duet singing in all gibbons with the exception of H. klossii and H. moloch (Geissmann 1993). Duets are mostly sung by mated pairs . Typically, mates combine their repertoire in relatively rigid, more or less precisely timed vocal interactions to produce well-patterned duets.http://www.gibbons.de/main/sound/sounds/08conc_sounds/coDuett94b3270.wav

Page 21: A.primate communication

Vervet monkeys

Page 22: A.primate communication

Vervet monkey alarm calls

• Vervet monkeys produce acoustically distinct alarm calls in response to

different classes of predator

– Large cats (e.g. leopard, cheetah)

– Birds of prey (e.g. martial eagle, crowned hawk eagle)

– Snakes (e.g. pythons, mambas)

• Hearing the leopard call, the members of the troupe run up to the top of

the nearest tree

• Hearing the eagle call, the members of the troupe run into a nearby bush

or under the lower branches of a nearby tree

• Hearing the snake call, the troupe of vervets all stand up on their hind

legs in the open and look around on the ground

Page 23: A.primate communication

Conclusion:communicate with a group. Animal communication is more based on the need to warn of danger or to describe the location of food, it is more of a necessary means of survival. Human language however adopts these characteristics of needing communication for survival but adds to them to form a language system that is both needed for survival but is also a social communication device, that is used spontaneously, something an animal does not especially require in order to survive in the animal world.(Tomasello and Call 1997).

Page 24: A.primate communication

Books

Bradbury, J. W. & Vehrencamp, S. L. (1998) Principles of animal communication., Oxford, Blackwell

Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990) How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species., Chicago, Chicago University Press

Chiba, T. & Kajiyama, M. (1941). The Vowel, its Nature and Structure. Tokyo.

Hauser, M. D. (1996) The evolution of communication., Cambridge, MIT Press

Owings, D. H. & Morton, E. S. (1998) Animal vocal communication: A new approach, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

Articles in Books

Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J. R. (1978) 'Animal signals: Information or manipulation' In

Behavioural ecology (Krebs, J. R.&Davies, N. B., eds), pp Oxford University Press

Fischer, J. (2002) 'Developmental modifications in the vocal behavior of non-human

primates' In Primate audition: Ethology and neurobiology (Ghazanfar, A. A., ed), pp 109-125, CRC Press

Marler, P., Evans, C. S. & Hauser, M. D. (1992) 'Animal signals? Reference, motivation or both?' In Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative

and developmental approaches. (Papoucek, H., Jürgens, U.&Papoucek, M., eds), pp 66-86, Cambridge University Press

Owren, M. J. & Linker, C. D. (1995) 'Some analysis methods that may be useful to acoustic primatologists' In Current topics in primate vocal

communication

(Zimmermann, E., Newman, J. D.&Juergens, U., eds), pp 1-27, Plenum Press

Snowdon, C. T. (1986) 'Vocal communication' In Comparative primate biology, vol. 2a, conservation and ecology (Mitchell, G.&Irwin, J., eds), pp

495-530, Alan R. Liss

Page 25: A.primate communication

Articles in Journals

Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1980) Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour 28,362-367Crockford, C., Herbinger,

I., Vigilant, L. & Boesch, C. (2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology 110,221-243

Fichtel,C. & Kappeler, P.M. (2002) Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system.

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 51: 262-275.

Fitch, W. T. (1997) Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. Journal of the Acoustical

Society of America 102,1213-1222

Fitch, W. T., Neubauer, J. & Herzel, H. (2002) Calls out of chaos: The adaptive significance of nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocal

production. Animal Behaviour 63,407-418

Gouzoules, H. & Gouzoules, S. (1990) Body size effects on the acoustic structure of pigtail macaque (macaca nemestrina) screams. Ethology

85,324-334

Gouzoules, S., Gouzoules, H. & Marler, P. (1984) Rhesus monkey (macaca mulatta) screams: Representational signalling in the recruitment of

agonistic aid. Animal Behaviour 32,182-193

Gros-Louis, J. (2004) The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys, cebus capucinus, from the perspective of the

signaller. Animal Behavior 67,431-440

Page 26: A.primate communication

Hamilton, W. J., III & Arrowood, P. C. (1978) Copulatory vocalisations of chacma baboons (papio ursinus), gibbons (hylobates hoolock), and

humans. Science 200,1405-1409

Marler, P. (1955) Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature 176,6-7

Marshall, A. J., Wrangham, R. W. & Clark, A. P. (1999) Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees? Animal Behaviour

58,825-830

Miller, C. T., Scarl, J. S. & Hauser, M. D. (2004) Sex-specific sensory biases underlie sex differences in tamarin long call structure. Animal

Behaviour 68:713-720.

Mitani, J. & Gros-Louis, J. (1998) Chorusing and convergence in chimpanzees: Tests of three hypotheses. Behaviour 135,1041-1064

Rendall, D., Owren, M. J. & Rodman, P. S. (1998) The role of vocal tract filtering in identity cueing in rhesus monkey (macaca mulatta)

vocalizations. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 103,602-614

Page 27: A.primate communication

Rendall, D., Owren, M. J., Weerts, E. & Hienz, R. D. (2004) Sex differences in the acoustic structure of vowel-like vocalizations in baboons and

their perceptualdiscrimination by baboon listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 115,411-421

Semple, S. (1998) The function of barbary macaque copulation calls. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London 265,287-291

Semple, S. & McComb, K. (2000) Perception of female reproductive state from vocal cues in a mammal species. Proceedings of the Royal

Society, London 267,707-712

Zahavi, A. (1975) Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53,205 214

Zuberbuhler, K., Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1999) Conceptual semantics in a nonhuman primate. Journal of Comparative Psychology

113,33-42

Zuberbuhler, K., Noe, R. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1997) Diana monkey long-distance calls: Messages for conspecifics and predators. Animal

Behaviour 53,589-604