Accident Analysis and Preventionsaiv.espaceweb.usherbrooke.ca/References/045_2010... · F. Lai, O....

6
Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4–9 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Accident Analysis and Prevention jo ur nal homep a ge: www.elsevier.com/locate/aap What benefit does Intelligent Speed Adaptation deliver: A close examination of its effect on vehicle speeds Frank Lai , Oliver Carsten Institute for Transport Studies, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 3 November 2009 Received in revised form 7 December 2009 Accepted 2 January 2010 Keywords: Intelligent Speed Adaptation Speed Compliance Gender Age Intention Fleet drivers a b s t r a c t Intelligent Speed Adaptation (ISA) is a driver support system which brings the speed limit information into the vehicle. This paper describes the UK ISA field trials taken place between 2004 and 2006 and presents evidence on how drivers’ choice of speed is altered. The ISA system was observed to have a distinctive effect in transforming the speed distribution from a conventional bell shape to an asymmetric distribution biased towards the high speed end. ISA not only diminished excessive speeding, but also led to a reduction in speed variation, prompting a positive implication to accident reduction. The use of an overridable ISA system also provided an opportunity to investigate where drivers would choose to have ISA based on observed behaviour instead of opinion. Evidence shows that ISA tends to be overridden on roads where it was perhaps needed most. Behavioural difference among driver groups also suggests that ISA tends to be overridden by those drivers who in safety terms stand to benefit most from using it, as with other safety systems. © 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Speeding is a universal road safety problem. It occurs to all types of drivers, and on all categories of roads. Statistical evidence shows that a high proportion of cars travelling over the speed limits, as illustrated in Fig. 1 (Department for Transport, 2009). Speeding has also long been the most common traffic offence dealt with by police action (Ministry of Justice, 2008). Furthermore, a recent one-week operation by police forces across 22 European countries as part of the European Traffic Police Network’s Lifesaver project also discov- ered that 636,038 drivers were detected breaking the speed limits (European Transport Safety Council, 2009). In terms of why drivers exceed speed limits, Corbett (2000) argues that speeding is not seen as a crime by most drivers and is widely believed to be socially acceptable. A recent survey by the motor insurance industry seems to support such a view in that three-quarters of UK drivers admitted to regular speeding (CIS, 2008). Drivers’ personality (e.g. sensation seeking) and demo- graphic characteristics (e.g. gender and age) also play a role in intentional speeding (Zuckerman and Neeb, 1980; Webster and Wells, 2000). Drivers however might unintentionally exceed the speed limits simply due to the inherent difficulty in accurate speed perception. Drivers estimate their own travel speed by a variety of sensory Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 113 3436606; fax: +44 113 3435334. E-mail address: [email protected] (F. Lai). inputs, including visual (Gibson, 1979), auditory (Matthews, 1978), and kinaesthetic (McLane and Wierwille, 1975) cues. Hence, for example, the presence of road environment, noise, and surface quality would all affect drivers’ estimation of their own travel speed. Transitions between high and low speed zones also present a problem in that drivers who have travelled in a high speed zone for a certain duration tend to under-estimate their speed when driving into a lower speed zone (Denton, 1976; Casey and Lund, 1993). The threat of speeding to road safety lies in its strong relation to accident occurrence and severity. It is a straightforward notion that the faster the vehicle travels, the less time the driver has to respond to incidents (i.e. principles of human performance), and the harder the vehicle collides (i.e. the laws of physics). The link between travel speed and accident occurrence and severity has been well documented (e.g. Nilsson, 1982; Baruya, 1998; Maycock et al., 1998; Quimby et al., 1999; Taylor et al., 2000, 2002; Elvik et al., 2004). The inclusion of contributory factors in UK’s official accident records since 2005 also shows that excessive speed and speed- ing are a major cause of accident occurrence as well as strongly influencing accident severity, as depicted in Table 1. 2. Intelligent Speed Adaptation Intelligent Speed Adaptation (ISA) is a driver support system which brings the speed limit information into the vehicle. An ISA system can be configured in various ways depending on the desired level of intervention with the driver’s speed control task: 0001-4575/$ see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2010.01.002

Transcript of Accident Analysis and Preventionsaiv.espaceweb.usherbrooke.ca/References/045_2010... · F. Lai, O....

Page 1: Accident Analysis and Preventionsaiv.espaceweb.usherbrooke.ca/References/045_2010... · F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4–9 5 Fig. 1. Percentage

Wo

FI

a

ARRA

KISCGAIF

1

otiaaote(

aitt(giW

sD

0d

Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4– 9

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Accident Analysis and Prevention

jo ur nal homep a ge: www.elsev ier .com/ locate /aap

hat benefit does Intelligent Speed Adaptation deliver: A close examinationf its effect on vehicle speeds

rank Lai ∗, Oliver Carstennstitute for Transport Studies, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK

r t i c l e i n f o

rticle history:eceived 3 November 2009eceived in revised form 7 December 2009ccepted 2 January 2010

eywords:

a b s t r a c t

Intelligent Speed Adaptation (ISA) is a driver support system which brings the speed limit informationinto the vehicle. This paper describes the UK ISA field trials taken place between 2004 and 2006 andpresents evidence on how drivers’ choice of speed is altered. The ISA system was observed to have adistinctive effect in transforming the speed distribution from a conventional bell shape to an asymmetricdistribution biased towards the high speed end. ISA not only diminished excessive speeding, but also ledto a reduction in speed variation, prompting a positive implication to accident reduction. The use of an

ntelligent Speed Adaptationpeedomplianceenderge

ntention

overridable ISA system also provided an opportunity to investigate where drivers would choose to haveISA based on observed behaviour instead of opinion. Evidence shows that ISA tends to be overridden onroads where it was perhaps needed most. Behavioural difference among driver groups also suggests thatISA tends to be overridden by those drivers who in safety terms stand to benefit most from using it, aswith other safety systems.

leet drivers

. Introduction

Speeding is a universal road safety problem. It occurs to all typesf drivers, and on all categories of roads. Statistical evidence showshat a high proportion of cars travelling over the speed limits, asllustrated in Fig. 1 (Department for Transport, 2009). Speeding haslso long been the most common traffic offence dealt with by policection (Ministry of Justice, 2008). Furthermore, a recent one-weekperation by police forces across 22 European countries as part ofhe European Traffic Police Network’s Lifesaver project also discov-red that 636,038 drivers were detected breaking the speed limitsEuropean Transport Safety Council, 2009).

In terms of why drivers exceed speed limits, Corbett (2000)rgues that speeding is not seen as a crime by most drivers ands widely believed to be socially acceptable. A recent survey byhe motor insurance industry seems to support such a view inhat three-quarters of UK drivers admitted to regular speedingCIS, 2008). Drivers’ personality (e.g. sensation seeking) and demo-raphic characteristics (e.g. gender and age) also play a role inntentional speeding (Zuckerman and Neeb, 1980; Webster and

ells, 2000).

Drivers however might unintentionally exceed the speed limits

imply due to the inherent difficulty in accurate speed perception.rivers estimate their own travel speed by a variety of sensory

∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 113 3436606; fax: +44 113 3435334.E-mail address: [email protected] (F. Lai).

001-4575/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.oi:10.1016/j.aap.2010.01.002

© 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

inputs, including visual (Gibson, 1979), auditory (Matthews, 1978),and kinaesthetic (McLane and Wierwille, 1975) cues. Hence, forexample, the presence of road environment, noise, and surfacequality would all affect drivers’ estimation of their own travelspeed. Transitions between high and low speed zones also presenta problem in that drivers who have travelled in a high speed zonefor a certain duration tend to under-estimate their speed whendriving into a lower speed zone (Denton, 1976; Casey and Lund,1993).

The threat of speeding to road safety lies in its strong relationto accident occurrence and severity. It is a straightforward notionthat the faster the vehicle travels, the less time the driver has torespond to incidents (i.e. principles of human performance), andthe harder the vehicle collides (i.e. the laws of physics). The linkbetween travel speed and accident occurrence and severity hasbeen well documented (e.g. Nilsson, 1982; Baruya, 1998; Maycocket al., 1998; Quimby et al., 1999; Taylor et al., 2000, 2002; Elvik et al.,2004). The inclusion of contributory factors in UK’s official accidentrecords since 2005 also shows that excessive speed and speed-ing are a major cause of accident occurrence as well as stronglyinfluencing accident severity, as depicted in Table 1.

2. Intelligent Speed Adaptation

Intelligent Speed Adaptation (ISA) is a driver support systemwhich brings the speed limit information into the vehicle. AnISA system can be configured in various ways depending on thedesired level of intervention with the driver’s speed control task:

Page 2: Accident Analysis and Preventionsaiv.espaceweb.usherbrooke.ca/References/045_2010... · F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4–9 5 Fig. 1. Percentage

F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4– 9 5

aa

ittidpeaia

2iFiJ

3

sfiu

3

ssswtttptu

TP

S

Fig. 2. The ISA HMI developed in the UK ISA project.

as intention to speed or not to speed (screened by a questionnaire

Fig. 1. Percentage of cars exceeding the speed limit 1998–2008.

dvisory/warning (no intervention), and intervening (either overrid-ble or non-overridable) ISA (Carsten, 2002).

The idea of assisting drivers with their speed control task couldn fact be traced to the early 1980s (Saad and Malaterre, 1982) inhe form of an on-board speed limiter where the limit was set byhe driver. During the 1990s, the research community started tonvestigate in-vehicle systems which would provide support to theriver according to prevailing speed limits. Large-scale trials tooklace in Sweden (Biding and Lind, 2002) where nearly 5000 carsquipped with various forms of ISA were tested. Most of these hadn advisory ISA but nearly 300 vehicles were equipped with anntervening system which consisted of a haptic throttle known asn Active Accelerator Pedal (AAP) (Hjälmdahl and Várhelyi, 2004).

The UK began its research into ISA in 1997 (Carsten and Tate,000). Later on, various ISA trials took place across Europe, includ-

ng the Netherlands (non-overridable ISA), Denmark (warning ISA),inland (recording ISA), Belgium (dynamic ISA), France (interven-ng ISA), and Austria (advisory ISA), as well as outside Europe (e.g.apan, Australia, and Canada) (Carsten, 2004).

. The UK ISA project

The UK ISA project (2001–2006) developed an intervening ISAystem, which was installed in a fleet of 20 vehicles for a two-yeareld trial with a total of eighty participants. The UK ISA project wasnique in comparison with other ISA trials.

.1. Sophisticated system design

The ISA system developed in this project was an overridable ISAystem. It was linked to the vehicle’s powertrain as well as brakeystem. If the driver attempted to go over the speed limit, the ISAystem would cease fuel supply and activate the vehicle’s brakehere necessary (e.g. downhill, or a transition from high speed zone

o low speed zone). The ISA interface was designed to blend intohe vehicle’s existing interface to provide an non-intrusive integra-

ion. An additional LCD display was embedded into the dashboardroviding speed limit and system status (e.g. whether the ISA con-rol was active or not). The ISA system was by default enabledpon engine ignition but drivers could override the ISA control by

able 1roportion of road accident attributable to speed related factors.

2006 2007

Fatal Serious Slight Fatal Serious Slight

Exceeding speed limits 14% 7% 4% 13% 8% 5%Excessive speeds 18% 13% 11% 16% 11% 10%

ource: Department for Transport (2007, 2008).

Fig. 3. Field trial phases.

pressing a button located on the steering wheel (i.e. the right but-ton in Fig. 2). Once the system was overridden, the driver was ableto go over the speed limit. The ISA control would be resumed whenthe vehicle’s speed dropped below the current speed limit, or thevehicle reached a new speed zone, or the driver voluntarily optedback in (by pressing the left button in Fig. 2). Once the ISA systemwas re-engaged, the driver would have to override it again in orderto go over the speed limit. The occurrence of user overriding waslogged. This process provided an ideal opportunity to demonstratedrivers’ interaction with the ISA system.

3.2. Extensive trial design

The field trial adopted an ‘A-B-A’ design with three distinctphases over a six-month duration, as illustrated in Fig. 3. The firstmonth of driving served as a baseline for comparison with the ISAactivated period, and the final month of driving provided the oppor-tunity to identify any carry-over effect as a result of experiencingthe ISA system.

A series of four trials took place, covering two distinctive geo-graphic areas and different types of driver, as depicted in Table 2.The Leeds area consisted of a mainly city environment but also someoutlying rural areas and villages. The Leicestershire area coveredmainly rural single carriageways and small towns. The ISA systemmade use of a map-matching system using GPS and other sensorscombined with on-board digital maps. The speed limit databasecovered the local area as well as the national trunk road network.The intention was to give drivers ISA support for almost all theirregular driving during the ISA-active phase.

Eighty drivers participated in the field trials. Recruitment cri-teria included gender, age (17–39, or over 40 years old), as well

developed based on Theory of Planned Behaviour; Ajzen, 1985).A young male participant dropped out during the trial. Since thetime remaining did not permit recruitment of a replacement driver,

Table 2Geographic characteristics and driver types in the UK ISA trials.

Test area

Driver type Private motorists Fleet drivers

Leeds (mainly urbanenvironment)

Trial 1 Trial 220 participants 20 participants

Leicestershire (mainly ruralenvironment)

Trial 3 Trial 420 participants 20 participants

Propriétaire
Texte surligné
Système non intrusif (non additionnel) et possibilité de désactiver le système pour dépasser la vitesse
Page 3: Accident Analysis and Preventionsaiv.espaceweb.usherbrooke.ca/References/045_2010... · F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4–9 5 Fig. 1. Percentage

6 F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4– 9

Table 3Participants’ demographic characteristics.

Male Female Total

Intender Non-intender Intender Non-intender

Young 11 8 6 11 36

taT

3

avau

Table 4Distribution of distance driven within trial phases.

Phase 1 Phase 2 Phase 3

20 mph 0.3 0.3 0.330 mph 36.2 35.9 35.840 mph 14.8 15.0 15.150 mph 5.7 6.0 6.060 mph 14.8 15.2 13.570 mph 28.2 27.6 29.3

SUM 100.0 100.0 100.0

Old 13 12 11 7 43

Total 25 20 17 18 79

here were in total 79 drivers whose data contributed to the finalnalysis. The categorisation of driver characteristics is shown inable 3.

.3. Comprehensive data collection

The on-board data logging system recorded a wide range of data

t high resolution, which facilitated sophisticated data analysis ofehicle speed and drivers’ overriding patterns against speed limitss well as road categories (i.e. Motorways, A roads, B roads, andnclassified roads).

Fig. 4. The impact of ISA on

Unit: percentage.

Although the data were recorded at 10 Hz (i.e. 10 records persecond), data used for analysis was distance-based rather thantime-based. While time-based data is intuitively valid, it introducesundue weight to the data stream when vehicle speed is zero (e.g. thevehicle stops at junctions) or very low (e.g. the vehicle moves slowly

on a congested road). Since accumulated travel distance within atrip was logged 10 Hz, the raw data were converted to distancebased data by sampling a record per 5 metres of travelling distance.

speed distribution.

Page 4: Accident Analysis and Preventionsaiv.espaceweb.usherbrooke.ca/References/045_2010... · F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4–9 5 Fig. 1. Percentage

F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4– 9 7

F(

TsmfTlddppa

4

csbvrsiinsfAsss

cstaAldcwswv

plwrs

The use of an overridable ISA system provides an opportunityto investigate where drivers were willing to accept the support of

ig. 5. Comparison of key statistics of the speed distribution across trial phases.Note: * and ** denote statistical significance at the 0.05 and 0.01 level respectively.)

his data reduction process also filtered out records without a validpeed limit attached to them (e.g. the vehicle was driven outside theap boundary or on a private road). The reduced database ready

or analysis contained over 110 million records across all four trials.able 4 compares the proportion of distance driven among speedimit zones across trial phases and suggests that the majority ofata were collected from 30 and 70 mph zones. The distribution ofata among speed limit zones was remarkably similar across trialhase, which suggests that seasonal effect was minimal, and thatarticipants had not altered their choice of route to a large extentcross trial phases.

. The impact of ISA on vehicle speed

The impact of ISA on speed distribution is illustrated in Fig. 4. Itlearly shows that ISA imposes a dramatic effect in reshaping thepeed distribution, except for the 60 mph zones, in which speedingehaviour had already rarely been recorded before ISA was acti-ated (i.e. Phase 1); this is in line with the UK national data. Theelatively low speeding figure observed on 60 mph roads is con-idered to be primarily due to the constraints on driving speedmposed by road geometry and traffic, as the 60 mph speed limits generally applied on rural single-carriageway roads. It is worthoting that the ISA system developed within this project allowedome limited speed excursion before the system cut in, and there-ore some driving clustered immediately above the speed limit.nother noticeable trend revealed in Fig. 4 is that the shapes ofpeed distribution from Phase 1 and Phase 3 were generally veryimilar, which implies that, although ISA effectively changed thepeed distribution, the carry-over effect was not prominent.

Fig. 5 illustrates a comparison of the mean and the 85th per-entile speeds across trial phases in each speed limit zone. A ‘V’hape is observable across all speed limit zones; i.e. the mean orhe 85th percentile speeds goes down from Phase 1 to Phase 2,nd then rises again from Phase 2 to Phase3. Repeated measuresNOVA reveals that the speeds from Phase 2 were significantly

ower than Phase 1 and Phase 3 in most speed limit zones. Theifferences between Phase 1 and 3 were not statistically signifi-ant, which suggests that the carry-over effect was minimal. It isorth noting that the statistical significance where applicable was

tronger for the 85th percentile speeds than for the mean speeds,hich suggests that ISA is particularly effective in tackling speed

iolations at higher speeds.Fig. 6 compares participants’ speeding behaviour across trial

hases in terms of the percentage of distance travelled over speed

imits. Again, a ‘V’ shape is observable across all speed limit zones,

hich suggests that ISA effectively reduces speeding. Results ofepeated measures ANOVA confirm that speeding in Phase 2 wasignificantly less than Phases 1 and 3 in most speed limit zones.

Fig. 6. Comparison of speeding behaviour across trial phases. (Note: * and ** denotestatistical significance at the 0.05 and 0.01 level respectively.)

The absence of statistical significance in the results from the 20and 50 mph zones with respect to mean speeds was primarily dueto the sample size; i.e. there were relatively less data collected fromthe two speed limit zones in comparison with other zones—not allparticipants drove in the two speed limit zones during the trialperiod. Albeit the small sample size, when the effect size is suffi-ciently large, it still demonstrates statistical significance as in thecase of the 85th percentile speeds. Also, observed speeding was notsignificantly different between Phase 1 and 3, which again suggeststhat the carry-over effect of ISA was not prominent.

Fig. 7 shows a comparison of speed variability across trial phases,in terms of the coefficient of variation (CV). CV is a dimensionlessmeasure that allows comparison of the variation of populationshaving considerably different mean values, which is of particularuse for this analysis since the speed zones range from 20 mph to70 mph. Speed variability derived from Phase 2 was consistentlysmaller than that from Phase 1 or 3 (i.e. a ‘V’ shape) across all speedlimit zones. Noticeably, the reduction in speed variability appearedto more prominent in urban areas than on rural single-carriageways(i.e. the 60 mph zones) and motorways (i.e. the 70 mph roads).

5. Driver compliance with ISA

Fig. 7. Comparison of speed variability across trial phases.

Page 5: Accident Analysis and Preventionsaiv.espaceweb.usherbrooke.ca/References/045_2010... · F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4–9 5 Fig. 1. Percentage

8 F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4– 9

ttirv

otp2tutspmncfpvtowtt

eartoditlo2

TC

Table 6Breakdown of drivers’ overriding behaviour in 30mph zones by road category.

Road category Percentage of distancetravelled with ISAoverridden

Mean frequency ofoverriding per 1000 km

A roads (urbanarterial roads)

12% 228

B roads 6% 135Unclassified roads(residential streets)

6% 133

appeared to override more frequently than private motorists on70 mph roads.

Fig. 8. Comparison of overriding behaviour across speed zones.

he ISA system and where they chose to override it. Fig. 8 showshat ISA was overridden most often on 70 mph roads. However, its also of great concern that the 20 mph zone had the second highestate of overriding, since here drivers are most likely to encounterulnerable road users such as pedestrians and young children.

Overriding ISA support in fact presents a different picture frombserved speeding (i.e. Fig. 7). Table 5 compares data collected fromhis project with official UK data on vehicle speeds. The average ofercentage of cars exceeding speed limits on UK roads between004 and 2006 was used in order to provide comparable data sincehat is when the ISA trials were taking place. This comparison bringsp two interesting points. First, speeding recorded in the ISA fieldrials was less than in the national data, and was far less in somepeed limit zones. This is most likely to be a reflection of the surveyrotocols adopted by the UK Department for Transport. All auto-atic traffic counters are sited at selected points where traffic is

ot congested under normal conditions to minimise the impact ofongestion, while data collected from the ISA trials included non-ree-flow speeds. Second, there is a large difference between theercentages derived from when ISA was overridden and when theehicle speed was over the speed limit, which is due to the ISA sys-em allowing a small amount of over-speeding without deliberateverriding. This however highlights the value of ISA. When driversished to go over the speed limit, they had to deliberately override

he system control. The need to overcome the natural behaviour ofhe system promoted driver compliance with speed limits.

Of all speed limit zones, the 30 mph zone is of particular inter-st, as it covers a variety of road layouts including residential streetsnd arterial roads (where roads are wider and often are dual car-iageways, which could adversely encourage drivers to speed whenraffic is light). Table 6 depicts a breakdown of the frequency ofverriding and percentage of distance travelled with ISA overrid-en across road categories in 30 mph zones. It suggests that drivers

ndeed overrode more often and drove a larger proportion of dis-ance with ISA overridden on urban arterial roads. Since a very

arge proportion of fatal and serious accidents (approximately 40%n average over the past five years; Department for Transport,008) occurred on A roads with a 30 mph limit and with the strong

able 5omparison of compliance with ISA and speed limits.

Speedlimit

Overriding observed inthis trial

Speeding observedin this trial

UK average between2004 and 2006

20 mph 13.3% 42.4% –30 mph 8.1% 34.9% 51%40 mph 7.7% 23.8% 27%50 mph 7.0% 24.0% –60 mph 1.3% 5.6% 11%70 mph 16.3% 24.5% 55%

Fig. 9. Comparison of overriding behaviour across driver groups.

relationship between speed and accidents (i.e. Table 1), it could beargued that ISA was most by-passed where it was most needed.

Fig. 9 compares overriding behaviour by driver groups, and sug-gests that male drivers, young drivers, drivers who were intendingto break speed limits, and private motorists tended to override theISA system more often than their counterparts, although the differ-ence between private motorists and fleet drivers was marginal.

Fig. 10 further examines the extent of overriding of the ISAsystem on 30 mph roads which are typical of urban areas and70 mph roads which are generally inter-city dual carriagewaysor motorways. It can be seen that the patterns by gender andage are the same for the two road categories. However, intendersand non-intenders demonstrated similar behaviour on urban roadsbut appeared to behave differently on 70 mph roads. There wasa notable difference in behaviour between the private motoristsand the fleet drivers: private motorists tended to override morefrequently than fleet drivers on urban roads, while fleet drivers

Fig. 10. Comparison of overriding behaviour on 30 and 70 mph roads across drivergroups.

Page 6: Accident Analysis and Preventionsaiv.espaceweb.usherbrooke.ca/References/045_2010... · F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident Analysis and Prevention 48 (2012) 4–9 5 Fig. 1. Percentage

alysis

6

othooAcdpobtn2

tso(aTwttGhtomsbpwsrd

batiewc

dfiwoa

A

t

F. Lai, O. Carsten / Accident An

. Discussion

The ISA system was observed to have a distinctive effect in termsf transforming the speed distribution across all speed zones excepthe 60 mph zones. Speeds over the speed limit and in particular veryigh exceeding of the limit were curtailed. When ISA was switchedn, a large proportion of the speed distribution previously spreadver the speed limit was shifted to around or below the speed limit.nalysis of various statistics related to speed (mean and 85th per-entile) revealed a ‘V’ shape across trial phases, i.e. the statistic wentown from Phase 1 to Phase 2, then up from Phase 2 to Phase 3. Thisattern is especially prominent with respect to high percentilesf the speed distribution, which are strong indicators of speedingehaviour. ISA not only diminished excessive speeding, but also ledo a reduction in speed variation, which has been argued to be sig-ificantly correlated with accident occurrence (Taylor et al., 2000,002).

The use of an overridable ISA system also provided an oppor-unity to investigate how drivers would choose to have such aupport, based on observed behaviour instead of opinion. ISA wasverridden the most on motorways, followed by built-up areas20 and 30 mph zones). Urban environments are where driversre most likely to encounter conflicts with vulnerable road users.hus there was some tendency for ISA to be overridden on roadshere it was perhaps needed most. In term of sub-groups within

he driving population, male drivers and young drivers overrodehe system more than their counterparts regardless of speed zones.iven that these two groups of drivers also drove faster and had aigher percentage of distance travelled over the speed limit thanheir counterparts, there is a pronounced tendency for ISA to beverridden by those drivers who in safety terms stand to benefitost from using it, as with other safety systems (e.g. seatbelts). The

ame tendency was observed by comparing overriding behaviouretween drivers who intend to speed and not to speed. In addition,rivate motorists were more likely to override in built-up areashile fleet drivers more frequently overrode on motorways. This

uggests that the need to comply with the speed limits on urbanoads may have been instilled in the fleet drivers, but those samerivers feel little compunction about speeding on fast roads.

In conclusion, ISA demonstrates a very large potential for safetyenefits, considering the strong relationships between speed andccident occurrence as well as severity. In addition, the poten-ial of ISA to road safety would be further extended when it isncorporated into a network-wide traffic management system. Forxample, ISA could be configured to apply lowered speed limitshere and when appropriate, such as roads outside schools within

ertain time periods, or on sharp bends at night time.ISA could also be configured to apply speed limits dynamically

epending on the provision of infrastructure. For instance, the traf-c control centre could broadcast a lowered speed limit for roadsith accidents, or for adverse weather conditions in certain areas

f the network. In such a scenario, ISA would no longer just anutonomous system, but a co-operative system.

cknowledgements

This work was carried out within the ‘Intelligent Speed Adapta-ion’ project funded by the Department for Transport (DfT). The

and Prevention 48 (2012) 4– 9 9

opinions, findings, and conclusions expressed in this paper arethose of the authors alone and do not necessarily reflect those ofDfT.

References

Ajzen, I., 1985. From intentions to action: a theory of planned behaviour. In: Kuhl,J., Beckman, J. (Eds.), Action Control: From Cognitions to Behaviours. Springer,New York, pp. 11–39.

Baruya, A., 1998. Speed–accident relationships on European roads. MASTER ProjectReport 1.1.3. European Commission, Brussels.

Biding, T., Lind, G., 2002. Intelligent Speed Adaptation (ISA): Results of Large-scaleTrials in Borlänge, Lidköping, Lund, and Umeå during 1999–2002. SwedishNational Road Administration, Borlange, Sweden.

Carsten, O., 2002. European Research on ISA: Where Are We Now and What Remainsto be Done. ICTCT, Nagoya, May.

Carsten, O., 2004. ISA—from field trials to reality. PACTS Conference ‘Targets 2010:No Room for Complacency’. London, 10 February.

Carsten, O., Tate, F., 2000. Final Report: Integration. Deliverable 17 of External Vehi-cle Speed Control Project. Institute for Transport Studies, University of Leeds,UK.

Casey, S., Lund, A., 1993. The effects of mobile roadside speedometers on trafficspeeds. Accident Analysis and Prevention 25 (5), 627–634.

Co-operative Insurance, 2008. http://www.prlog.org/10133950-three-quarters-of-drivers-speed-undeterred-by-cameras-survey-by-car-insurance.html.

Corbett, C., 2000. The social construction of speeding as not ‘real’ crime. CrimePrevention and Community Safety: An International Journal 2 (4), 33–46.

Denton, G., 1976. The influence of visual pattern on perceived speed at New-bridge MB Midlothian. Report LR531. Transport and Road Research Laboratory,Crowthorne.

Department for Transport, 2007. Road Casualties Great Britain 2006. Departmentfor Transport, London.

Department for Transport, 2008. Road Casualties Great Britain 2007. Departmentfor Transport, London.

Department for Transport, 2009. Road Statistics 2008—Traffic, Speeds and Conges-tion. Department for Transport, London.

Elvik, R., Christensen, P., Amundsen, A., 2004. Speed and road accidents: an evalua-tion of the Power Model. TOI Research Report 740/2004. Institute of TransportEconomics, Oslo.

European Transport Safety Council, 2009. Speed Monitor, Issue 5. European Trans-port Safety Council, Brussels, June.

Gibson, J., 1979. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Houghton Mifflin,Boston.

Hjälmdahl, M., Várhelyi, A., 2004. Speed regulation by in-car active acceler-ator pedal—effects on driver behaviour. Transportation Research Part F 7,77–94.

Matthews, M., 1978. Speed Limit Compliance and Judgment of velocity by CarDrivers. Proceedings of the Human Factors Association of Canada 11th AnnualMeeting.

Maycock, G., Brocklebank, P., Hall, R., 1998. Road layout design standards and driverbehaviour. TRL Report 332. Transport Research Laboratory, Crowthorne.

McLane, R., Wierwille, W., 1975. The influence of motion and audio cueson driver performance in an automobile simulator. Human Factors 17,488–501.

Ministry of Justice, 2008. Motoring Offences and Breath Test Statistics, England andWales 2006. Ministry of Justice, London.

Nilsson, G., 1982. The Effect of Speed Limits on Traffic Accidents in Sweden. Report68. National Swedish Road Traffic Research Institute (VTI), Linköping, Sweden.

Quimby, A., Maycock, G., Palmer, C., Buttress, S., 1999. The factors that influencea driver’s choice of speed: a questionnaire study. TRL Report 325. TransportResearch Laboratory, Crowthorne.

Saad, F., Malaterre, G., 1982. La regulation de la vitesse: Analyse des aides au controlede la vitesse (Regulation of speed: an analysis of aides to control the speed).ONSER, France.

Taylor, M., Baruya, A., Kennedy, J., 2002. The relationship between speed andaccidents on rural single-carriageway roads. TRL Report 511. TRL Limited,Crowthorne.

Taylor, M., Lynam, D., Baruya, A., 2000. The effects of drivers’ speed on the frequencyof road accidents. TRL Report 421. TRL, Crowthorne, UK.

Webster, D., Wells, P., 2000. The characteristics of speeders. TRL Report 440. Trans-port Research Laboratory, Crowthorne, UK.

Zuckerman, M., Neeb, M., 1980. Demographic influences in sensation seeking andexpressions of sensation seeking in religion, smoking and driving habits. Per-sonality and Individual Differences 1, 197–206.

Propriétaire
Texte surligné