A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon...

24
Chapter 7 The Trait Approach to Personality Key themes The history of trait approaches to personality Defining traits Lexical hypothesis approach to personality traits The contribution of Gordon Allport Raymond Cattell and the Sixteen Personality Factor Inventory Hans Eysenck and the three-factor structure The Big Five Evaluating trait approaches Learning outcomes After studying this chapter you should: Be able to describe the nomothetic approach to personality research Appreciate the long history of attempts to describe and explain differences in personality Understand what is meant by the lexical hypothesis Be familiar with the approach to data analysis employed by trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait approach Know about Hans Eysenck’s attempts to uncover the basic structure of personality Understand the approaches that have resulted in the identification of the Big Five personality traits

Transcript of A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon...

Page 1: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

Chapter 7The Trait Approachto Personality

Key themes

� The history of trait approaches to personality

� Defining traits

� Lexical hypothesis approach to personality traits

� The contribution of Gordon Allport

� Raymond Cattell and the Sixteen Personality Factor Inventory

� Hans Eysenck and the three-factor structure

� The Big Five

� Evaluating trait approaches

Learning outcomes

After studying this chapter you should:� Be able to describe the nomothetic approach to personality

research� Appreciate the long history of attempts to describe and

explain differences in personality� Understand what is meant by the lexical hypothesis� Be familiar with the approach to data analysis employed by

trait theorists� Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait

approach� Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

approach� Know about Hans Eysenck’s attempts to uncover the basic

structure of personality� Understand the approaches that have resulted in the

identification of the Big Five personality traits

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:44 PM Page 154

Page 2: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 155

Easygoing, intelligent, funny, caring, profes-sional man, aged 44, good in a crisis, seekswarm, friendly, intelligent woman of a similarage. Enjoys good food, wine, cinema and theatre.

Vibrant, charismatic, passionate, energeticsportswoman, aged 32, loves all outdoor activi-ties, foreign travel, cooking, reading and gar-dening seeks country-dwelling male with similarinterests.

At this point, you may be wondering what lonely-hearts advertisements are doing in a textbook on per-sonality. However, we want you to think about the imageof the individuals that these advertisements convey.Most of us are good at doing this; from a short descrip-tion of an individual, we can build up a mental picture ofthem and make decisions about which individuals weare attracted to and might like to meet and which holdno appeal for us. What we are doing is using our knowl-edge of personality traits to build up an image of theperson from their description. We have highlighted the

Introduction

words that label personality traits in the advertisementsabove, and it is worth taking a moment to reflect onwhat you understand by each of them and how youvalue them. Is the picture of a vibrant, charismatic, pas-sionate, energetic woman one you value you positivelyor one you find unappealing?

What these types of adverts suggest is that from afew personality traits and statements about interests,we may be able to build up an image of the individual.This in effect is what trait personality theorists aim todo, but in a more rigorous, scientific way. Traits theoristsemploy the nomothetic approach to personality thatwe covered in Chapter 1. The aim is to identify thosepersonality variables or traits that occur consistentlyacross groups of people. Each individual can then belocated within this set of variables. The aim is to identifythe main traits that usefully distinguish between types ofpeople. In achieving this, they hope to uncover thebasic structure of personality. As you may recall fromChapter 1, this is one of the major aims of studyinghuman personality. It is a major undertaking, and we willnow explore the progress that has been made, startingwith the Ancient Greeks.

Source: Photodisc/Getty Images

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:44 PM Page 155

Page 3: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

156 PART 1 PERSONALITY

Emergence of personality traits

The Ancient Greek philosopher, Aristotle (384–322 BC),provides the first written description of personality traits,or dispositions as he preferred to call them. He describedindividual differences in traits such as modesty, braveryand vanity, seeing them as important determinants ofwhether a person behaved ethically. One of his students,Theophrastus (371–287 BC), published an account of 30personality characters or types. These were early attemptsto describe the commonly acknowledged differences be-tween individuals and to identify individuals with similardispositions. The task can be thought of as putting someorder or structure into our everyday observations so thatthey are easier to conceptualise and discuss.

Another Ancient Greek philosopher, Hippocrates(460–377 BC), described physical illness as being caused bythe balance of bodily fluids, or humours as he labelledthem. These fluids included blood, black bile, yellow bileand phlegm. Another Ancient Greek, a physician namedGalen (AD 130–200), expanded on Hippocrates’ theory ofthe humours and applied it to describe human tempera-ment or personality (Stelmack and Stalikas, 1991). Whenthe humours were in balance, an equitable temperamentwas the result. If the humours were out of balance, thenphysical illness and mental disturbance occurred. Theterms Galen used to describe these mental disturbances arestill part of the English language. An excess of black bileresulted in a melancholic temperament, associated withdepressed mood and feelings of anxiety. Strong activity inthe body fluids resulted in an individual with strong emo-tions described as being of choleric temperament,

meaning that they had a tendency to easily become angry.Individuals of phlegmatic temperament were calm, asthere was low humorous activity, while individuals ofsanguine temperament were confident and optimistic.

In the Middle Ages the German philosopher ImmanuelKant (1724–1804) revisited the humoural temperamentsand produced a description of four personality types.These were based on the strength of the individual’s feel-ings and how active the person was. Melancholic individu-als had weak feelings, while sanguine individuals hadstrong feelings. Phlegmatic individuals had low levels ofactivity, while choleric individuals had much higher levelsof activity.

These early writers all described types of personalityrather than personality traits. This is an important distinc-tion. Personality types describe discrete categories intowhich individuals can be placed. Personality traits are con-tinuous dimensions, and individuals can be positionedalong the dimension depending on how much of the traitthey possess.

It was Wilhelm Wundt, the founding father of modern-day psychology, who changed the categorical types of per-sonality into trait dimensions. He revisited the humouralterms in his description of personality, reclassifying theold types in two dimensions based on their mood stabilityand the strength of their emotions. Individuals could thenbe placed along the dimensions of mood stability andstrength of emotions rather than being simply placed inone category. Wundt’s classification system is displayed inFigure 7.1.

It is true to say that little progress was made in terms ofclassifying personality traits from the time of the AncientGreeks to the middle of the nineteenth century, when theclinical theories emerged – as we have already seen in

Emotional

Emotional dimension

Changeabilitydimension

Unchangeable Changeable

Unemotional

Figure 7.1 Wundt’s emotionality and changeability dimensions of personality.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:44 PM Page 156

Page 4: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 157

Chapter 1. The reason for the delay in the emergence oftrait theories is easily understandable. There are a hugenumber of terms in all languages to describe personalitytraits. For trait approaches to personality to develop scien-tifically, some systematic way of structuring these termsand identifying the common dimensions underlying themwas necessary. It was the invention of statistical techniquessuch as correlation and factor analysis that made this possi-ble, as we shall see.

Defining personality traits

Up until now, we have used the term ‘trait’ to describe per-sonality. We are sure you have understood what we havebeen saying, but we should begin this section with a defi-nition of exactly what psychologists mean by a personalitytrait. Frequently, terms that have a very specific meaningin psychology are also part of our everyday language. Thiscan result in some confusion about the precise meaning ofterms; hence psychology’s obsession with defining theterms that we use. According to Burger (1997), ‘A trait is adimension of personality used to categorise people ac-cording to the degree to which they manifest a particularcharacteristic.’

Two assumptions underlie trait theory. The first as-sumption is that personality characteristics are relativelystable over time; the second is that traits show stabilityacross situations. A person’s behaviour may alter on differ-ent occasions, but the assumption is that there is some in-ternal consistency in the ways that individuals behave. Forexample, someone who is described as an extravert may bevery outgoing and chatty at a party but less so in a psy-chology seminar. In both situations, they are likely to bemore sociable than an introverted individual. We alsoassume that personality traits influence behaviour. Theperson is outgoing and chatty because they are an ex-travert. These are somewhat circular arguments, and thepsychologist has to move beyond them. Trait theoristshave to be able to make a distinction between the internalqualities of the individual and the way they behave. Thecausal relationship between the two then has to be ex-plained if we are to avoid circular arguments. To say thatindividuals become angry easily because they have anangry disposition does not get us very far. We need toknow where their angry disposition has come from andhow it influences their day-to-day behaviour.

It follows logically from the trait approach that traittheorists are more interested in general descriptions ofbehaviour than in understanding the individual and mak-ing predictions about individual behaviour. They take thetrait continuum and provide descriptions of how groupsof people at different points on the continuum might beexpected to behave. For example, they might compare a

group high in aggression with a group with low scores onthe same trait and observe how they behave in a debate.They are interested in typical group behaviour. It is fre-quently a descriptive rather than an explanatory ap-proach. Some trait theorists are more interested indescribing personality and predicting behaviour than inidentifying what caused the behaviour. This can lead tocircular reasoning. An individual is said to behave in acertain way because they are an anxious person. Whenasked to explain why an individual is anxious, the response is that they are anxious because they have be-haved in a certain way. Increasingly, however, the identifi-cation processes are only the first stage. Trait theorists arebecoming more interested in providing explanations forbehaviour. Trait approaches make it relatively easy tomake comparisons among people; individuals can beplaced on a continuum relative to others, and groups canalso be compared. However, trait theorists have little tosay about personality change. The theorists with an inter-est in personality change have come from a clinical back-ground, while trait theorists are more likely to beacademic psychologists.

To recap, within psychology, traits are considered thefundamental units of personality. They represent dispo-sitions to respond in certain ways. For a long time, therewere arguments about how much the situation influ-enced the individual’s behaviour and how much wasdown to their personality traits. It is now generallyaccepted that while situational factors will affect behav-iour, dispositional effects on that behaviour will still beobservable. Mischel (1999) has produced an elegant defi-nition of a personality trait that incorporates this. Hesuggests that a trait is the ‘conditional probability of acategory of behaviours in a category of contexts’. Hence,if a person is an extravert, then degrees of extraverted be-haviour will be observable from that person in a varietyof situations.

The development of trait theorieswithin psychology

During the rest of this chapter we are going to take youthrough the development and establishment of the coretrait theories of personality in psychology. These includethe work of:

� Sheldon� Early lexical approaches� Allport� Cattell� Eysenck� The five-factor model.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:44 PM Page 157

Page 5: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

158 PART 1 PERSONALITY

Sheldon and somatypes

Although the psychoanalyst Jung (Chapter 3) introduced theterms ‘extraversion’ and ‘introversion’, the real founding figureof trait psychology is considered to be an American psycholo-gist, William Sheldon (1899–1977). He outlined what came tobe a very well-known description of personality called so-matypes, which is based on physique and temperament. Fromhis surveys of thousands of individuals, he concluded thatthere are three basic types of physique: endomorphy, meso-morphy and ectomorphy (Sheldon, 1970). Using correla-tional techniques, he demonstrated that each body type wasassociated with a particular temperament. A summary of histheory is displayed in Table 7.1.

While accepting that everyone had the same internal or-gans, Sheldon felt that individuals were different in terms ofwhich organs were most prominent in their bodies and thuswhere their body’s focus lay. Table 7.1 represents theextremes of each type, but Sheldon produced a detailed atlasof male body types where bodies were matched against theseextremes using a seven-point grading scale. He planned asimilar female body atlas, but this was never produced. Youmay still come across descriptions of Sheldon’s body types inpopular texts. In terms of personality theorising, Sheldon’swork was important as it marked the start of the utilisationof psychometric approaches to the study of personality. Hecarried out extensive surveys of large populations, collecteddifferent measures from individuals using questionnairesand applied statistical techniques to the analysis of his data.

Early lexical approaches topersonality and the lexicalhypothesis

Several of the early researchers used dictionaries or Roget’sThesaurus to try to identify and count the number ofwords that describe personality traits. Sir Francis Galton

(1822–1911) was an Englishman who is best known for hisearly studies on genetic influences on intelligence, but hewas also interested in the relationships between languageand personality. He suggested that the most meaningfulpersonality descriptors will tend to become encoded inlanguage as single words. Galton (1884) provides the firstdocumented source of a dictionary and/or thesaurusbeing used to elicit words describing personality.

This approach has come to be known as the lexical hy-pothesis. It suggests that it is the individual differences be-tween people that are important that become encoded assingle terms. This appears to be a sensible assumption.Two additional criteria are included in the lexical hypoth-esis. First, frequency of use is also assumed to correspondwith importance. Again, it seems logical that the words weuse most to describe personality will be labelling theaspects of personality that we think are most important.Secondly, the number of words in a language that refer toeach trait will be related to how important that trait is indescribing human personality. An example from a the-saurus is included in Table 7.2 to help clarify what wemean. From the table, you can see that the personalitydescriptor ‘honest’ has 31 synonyms listed, suggesting thatit is a more important descriptor of personality than theword ‘aberrant’, which is not listed in the Oxford ConciseThesaurus (1999). Similarly, the word ‘warm’ describes amore useful descriptor of personality than ‘pedantic’ does.

Table 7.1 Sheldon’s theory of physique and temperament.

Physique Temperament

Focus on part of body Physique Temperament Description

Ectomorph Nervous system andthe brain

Light-boned with aslight musculature

Cerebrotonia A need for privacy,restrained, inhibited

Mesomorph Musculature and thecirculatory system

Large, bony with well-defined muscles

Somatotonia Physically assertive, com-petitive, keen on physicalactivity

Endomorph Digestive system, particularly the stomach

Rounded body tending towards fatness

Visceratonia Associated with a love ofrelaxation and comfort; likefood and are sociable

While most of the early work was conducted on theEnglish language, it is assumed that if the lexical hypothesisis a valid theory, then it should apply cross-culturally(Norman, 1963). This is the final assumption of the lexicalhypothesis. We will return to the cross-cultural questionlater in the chapter. To summarise, it states that if individ-ual differences between people are important, there will bewords to describe them; the more frequently a personalitydescriptor is used, the more important the personalitycharacteristic; and finally, the more synonyms of the wordthere are, the more important the difference.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:44 PM Page 158

Page 6: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 159

Table 7.2 Evidence for the lexical hypothesis.

Personality descriptor Synonyms Number

Gordon Allport

Initially, lexical researchers were limited to counting theterms used, identifying synonyms, and producing lists ofthese words. One of the first psychologists to produce such alist was the American Gordon Allport (1897–1967). With acolleague, he identified 18,000 words, of which 4,500described personality traits (Allport and Odbert, 1936). All-port published the first psychology text on personality traits,Personality Traits: Their Classification and Measurement(1921), and he is believed to have taught the first course onpersonality in the United States in 1924. While promotingthe concept of personality traits, Allport (1961) was quiteclear about the limitations of the trait approach. He felt thatit was almost impossible to use an individual’s personalitytraits to predict how they will behave in a specific situation.He acknowledged that there is variability in everyone’s be-haviour, but that there is also some constancy. Personalitytraits constitute this constant portion of behaviour. He sug-gested that personality traits have a physical presence in ournervous systems. He suggested that advances in technologywould one day enable psychologists to identify personalitytraits from inspection of the nervous system.

Although interested in traits, Allport adopted a unifiedapproach to personality, suggesting that it is the way thatthe component traits come together that is important. It ishow the traits come together that produces the uniquenessof all individuals, which he was keen to stress. Together,these traits produce a unified personality that is capable ofconstant evolution and change. Allport felt that change is acomponent part of the personality system that is necessary

to allow us to adapt to new situations and grow to copewith them. He adopted a very positive conceptualisation ofhuman nature. He suggested that human beings are nor-mally rational, creative, active and self-reliant. This was avery different view of human nature from the Freudian onethat was dominant at the time.

Allport made the distinction between nomothetic andidiographic approaches to the study of personality. We cov-ered this distinction in Chapter 1, but we have included areminder of these terms in Stop and think: Nomotheticand idiographic approach, as this is an important distinc-tion that you need to be familiar with. Allport felt thatboth approaches bring unique insights into our under-standing of personality. He felt that the nomothetic ap-proach allows the identification of common personalitytraits (Allport, 1961). He saw these common traits as waysof classifying groups of individuals with one group beingclassified as being more dominant, happier or whateverthan another comparable group. He felt that such compar-isons based on common traits are not particularly useful.Of more use is what he termed the personal dispositionof the individual. The personal disposition represents theunique characteristics of the individual. This approach em-phasises the uniqueness of each person, and Allport (1961)felt that this was potentially a more fruitful approachtowards developing a real understanding of personality.

Personality traits were further classified into cardinal,central and secondary traits. Cardinal traits are singletraits that may dominate an individual’s personality andheavily influence their behaviour. These may be thought ofas obsessions or ruling passions that produce a need that

honest trustworthy, truthful, veracious, trusty,honourable, creditable, decent, law-abiding, uncorrupted, uncorrupt, incorruptible, ethical, moral, virtuous,principled, upright, high-minded, dependable, reliable, reputable, above-board, straight, square-dealing,fair, just, candid, frank, sincere, direct,ingenuous, sound

31

warm amiable, friendly, cheerful, cordial, affable, pleasant, genial, kindly, hospitable, hearty, affectionate, mellow, loving

13

pedantic perfectionistic, scrupulous, finicky,fussy, punctilious, fastidious, meticulous, exact, quibbling

9

aberrant – meaning odd or peculiar not included

Source: Oxford Concise Thesaurus, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 1999.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:44 PM Page 159

Page 7: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

160 PART 1 PERSONALITY

demands to be fulfilled. For example, someone may have acardinal trait of competitiveness that permeates virtuallyevery aspect of their behaviour. They strive to be best ateverything they do. Central traits are the 5 to 10 traits thatAllport felt best describe an individual’s personality.Secondary traits are more concerned with an individ-ual’s preferences and are not a core constituent of their per-sonality. Secondary traits may only become apparent in

particular situations – unlike central traits, which have amore general applicability.

The other major contribution that Allport made to per-sonality theorising relates to the concept of self. He empha-sised the importance of the concept to any theory ofpersonality as he felt it is crucial to the development ofidentity and individuality. He hypothesised that childrenare not born with a concept of self, but that it gradually de-

Stop and think

Nomothetic and idiographic approach

The nomothetic approach comes from the ancient Greekterm for ‘law’ and is based on the assumption that there isa finite set of variables in existence that can be used to de-scribe human personality. The aim is to identify these per-sonality variables or traits that occur consistently acrossgroups of people. Each individual can then be locatedwithin this set of variables. By studying large groups ofpeople on a particular variable, we can establish the aver-age levels of that variable in particular age groups, or inmen and women, and in this way produce group averages,generally called norms, for variables. Individuals can thenbe described as being above or below the average ornorm on a particular variable. The nomothetic approachconcentrates on the similarities between individuals.

The idiographic approach focuses on the individualand describes the personality variables within that indi-vidual. The term comes from the ancient Greek idios,meaning ‘private or personal’. Theorists, who adopt thisapproach in the main, are only interested in studyingindividuals one at a time. They see each person ashaving a unique personality structure. Differences be-tween individuals are seen to be much greater than thesimilarities. The possible differences are infinite. Idio-graphic approaches produce a unique understanding ofthat individual’s personality. These approaches are usu-ally based on case studies of individuals.

The aim of lexical approaches is to find underlying dimensions tothe many ways we describe our personality.Source: Pearson Education Ltd.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 160

Page 8: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 161

velops. He felt that it is a lifelong process of development.The child first becomes aware of the separateness of them-selves from others in their environment and from thiscomes their sense of self-identity. As a result of their expe-riences while becoming integrated into their family andwider society, they develop self-esteem. Allport felt that theconcept of self presented a challenge to psychologists as it isdifficult to define precisely, consisting as it does of severalcomponent parts. He used the term proprium as a syn-onym for the self, suggesting that the terms represented allthe constituent parts that go to make up the concept of self.

Allport’s major impact on personality theory was in termsof stressing the limitations of the trait approaches as theywere currently adopted. He raised the issue about the relativeinfluence of personality and situation in determining behav-iour, something still of concern to psychologists. His inclu-sion of the concept of self as a legitimate and central concernof personality theorists was also important in the trait tradi-tion of personality research. His distinction between nomo-thetic and idiographic approaches to the study of personalityis an important one. He did not develop any standardisedmeasures of personality traits as such; this was left to othertheorists, as we shall see. His list of 4,500 personality traits istoo long to be of much practical use in assessing personality.

Raymond Cattell and theemergence of the factor analytic approach

The real advances in trait approaches were only possibleafter the invention of the technique of factor analysis. A

detailed description of the principles underlying factoranalysis is given in Chapter 25. You may find it helpful toread that section so that you can fully understand the restof this chapter. Allport did not engage with factor analy-sis; but the next theorist that we examine, RaymondCattell, made full use of the technique, having been in-structed in it by Spearman, the inventor of factor analysis.(See Profile: Raymond Cattell.) Following from this earlyscientific training, Cattell was keen to apply empiricalmethods to discover the basic structure of personality.From the lists of personality traits, he noted that manytraits are very similar, and he argued that the existing listscould be reduced to a much smaller number of traits. Thissmaller number of traits would represent the basic com-ponents of personality. Cattell’s work thus marks the be-ginning of the search for the structure of personalityusing factor analysis. Put simply, the procedure involvesidentifying lists of the most frequently used sets of wordsthat seem to describe aspects of personality; large samplesof individuals are then asked to rate the degree to whichthe attributes apply to them. This data set is then factoranalysed to identify which attributes cluster together.Clusters are composed of items that correlate with eachother. So, for example, you might have the variables ‘de-termined’, ‘persistent’, ‘productive’ and ‘goal-directed’ thatturn out to be highly correlated with each other and thusform a cluster or factor that you could perhaps callachievement oriented. What this method gives you is ageneral measure of some ability, in this instance achieve-ment orientation, that you obtain by measuring the indi-vidual’s ratings of their determination, persistence,productivity and goal-directedness.

Profile

Raymond Cattell

Raymond Bernard Cattell was born in a village just out-side Birmingham, UK, in 1905. His first degree was inchemistry and physics at the University of London. Hehad become interested in psychology and undertooka PhD in psychology at the same university. His su-pervisor was the inventor of factor analysis, CharlesSpearman. This resulted in Cattell being very welltrained in the new statistical technique of factor analysisand adopting it as an analysis tool. Sir Cyril Burt, the psy-chologist who specialised in intelligence, was also in thesame department; and Cattell was influenced by his ap-parently rigorous approach to research. You will find outmore about Burt in Chapter 13 when you examine intel-ligence. Cattell undertook some studies on personality

and worked in a child guidance clinic to get clinical ex-perience. In 1937, he emigrated to the United Statesand to a position at Columbia University. He hasworked at Clark University, Harvard and the Universityof Illinois, where he was director of the Laboratory ofPersonality Assessment. Cattell is not always easy toread in the original. A lot of his work deals with mathe-matical issues involved in factor analysis. He was aprolific writer, publishing 35 books and over four hun-dred journal articles. In addition, he produced a varietyof personality tests, including the Culture Fair IntelligenceTests, Motivation Analysis Test and the much-used Sixteen Personality Factor questionnaire (16PF test).Cattell died in February 1998.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 161

Page 9: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

Types of traits

Cattell (1965) defined personality as being the characteris-tics of the individual that allow prediction of how they willbehave in a given situation. His approach to personalitywas a broad one, and he identified a range of traits, as weshall see. Later in his career, he became interested in theways that personality traits and situational variables inter-act to affect the way that individuals behave. Traits are con-ceptualised as being relatively stable, long-lasting buildingblocks of personality.

Cattell makes distinctions between types of traits. Thefirst distinction relates to whether traits are genetically de-termined or the result of environmental experiences. Thegenetically determined traits are called constitutionaltraits while the environmentally induced traits are calledenvironmental-mold traits. This distinction representsthe nature versus nurture debate that occurs repeatedly inevery area of psychology. In this application, it askswhether individual differences are caused by inherited as-pects of our personality, or are they explained by how wehave been treated and the environmental experiences wehave had? Cattell (1982) was keen to try to establish the rel-ative contribution of genetics and environment to variouspersonality traits. He developed a statistical procedurecalled multiple abstract variance analysis (MAVA) toaccomplish this. He administered personality tests to assessa particular trait in relation to complex samples consistingof family members raised together, family members raisedapart, identical twins raised together, identical twinsraised apart, unrelated children raised together and unre-lated children raised apart. Using complex statistical proce-dures, the test allows the researcher to calculate the precisedegree of influence that genetic and environmental factorshave in the development of a particular personality trait.

Next, Cattell defines three different types of traits: ability,temperament and dynamic traits. Ability traits determinehow well you deal with a particular situation and how wellyou reach whatever your goal is in that situation. For exam-ple, the various aspects of intelligence are good examples ofability traits. He also identifies individual differences in thestyles that people adopt when they are pursuing their goals.These are labelled temperament traits. Some people maybe laid back and easygoing, or irritable, or anxious and soon, in the way that they typically approach life. These thenare examples of temperament traits.

Cattell, like many of the other theorists we have exam-ined, was interested in what motivates human behaviour.You will recall from Chapter 1 that this is a core area for per-sonality theories to explain. He suggested that we havedynamic traits that motivate us and energise our behav-iour (Cattell, 1965). For example, an individual may be mo-tivated to succeed and be very competitive, or they may beambitious, or driven to care for others, be artistic and so on.

As Cattell (1965) considered the question of motivation tobe at the heart of personality theorising, the dynamic traitswere heavily researched. He concluded that there are threetypes of dynamic traits: attitudes, sentiments and ergs.Attitudes are defined as hypothetical constructs that expressour particular interests in people or objects in specific situ-ations. Attitudes help to predict how we will behave in aparticular situation. Cattell (1950) defined sentiments ascomplex attitudes that include our opinions and intereststhat help determine how we feel about people or situations.Cattell (1979) considered ergs to be innate motivators. Hesuggested that ergs are innate drives. They cause us to recog-nise and attend to some stimuli more readily than others,and to seek satisfaction of our drives.

Cattell suggests that all these types of dynamic traits areorganised in very complex and interrelated ways to producedynamic lattice. The aim is to explain how we have to ac-quire particular traits to achieve our goals. For example, ifyour goal is learning to ski, you need to learn to copy the in-structor. You have to demonstrate patience and persever-ance in practising. You have to tolerate being a figure of funwhen you fall over, and you may have to conquer fear to goon the drag lift and so on. How others react to you will alsoaffect the lattice as will your attitudes towards others andthe mood you are in. This then gives a hint of the complex-ity involved. It is fair to say that this system certainly doesnot simplify the explanation of behaviour in any real way,and other psychologists have not followed up this work.

A further distinction is between common traits andunique traits. Common traits are those shared by manypeople. They would include intelligence, sociability, de-pendency and so on. Unique traits are rarer and specific toindividuals. A unique trait might be an interest in collect-ing fishing reels by a particular maker or an interest in aparticular entertainer or the like. They are specialised inter-ests, if you like, that motivate individuals to pursue certainrelated activities. While Cattell’s work is concerned almostexclusively with common traits, he includes the concept ofunique traits to emphasise the uniqueness of human be-ings. He also stressed that the uniqueness of individuals isalso due to the unique ways that common traits come to-gether in different individuals. Different individuals willhave different mixtures of common traits making up theirpersonalities, thus making them unique.

Cattell (1950) suggested an important distinction be-tween surface traits and source traits. Surface traits arecollections of traits descriptors that cluster together inmany individuals and situations. For example, individualswho are sociable also tend to be carefree, hopeful and con-tented. These are all surface traits; and when you measureindividuals on each of these surface traits, you find thattheir scores on each one are correlated with all the others.That is, if an individual scores highly on sociability, theyalso score highly on the carefree trait, the hopefulness trait

162 PART 1 PERSONALITY

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 162

Page 10: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 163

and the contentedness trait. The technique of factor analy-sis suggests that there is an underlying trait, what Cattellcalls a source trait, that is responsible for the observedvariance in the surface traits. In this case, it is the sourcetrait of extraversion. Extraversion is measured by the scoresof the surface traits of sociability, carefreeness, hopefulnessand contentedness. The surface traits relate to the overtbehaviours that individuals display. The source trait, on theother hand, is the major difference in personality that isresponsible for all these related differences in observedbehaviour. In simple terms, being high in the source trait ofextraversion causes you to display behaviour that is moresociable, to have more hopeful attitudes and so on.

The source traits are identified using the statistical tech-nique of factor analysis, as we have previously discussed.Source traits are important as they represent the actual un-derlying structure of personality. If psychologists can identifythe basic structure of personality, then they will be better ableto predict behaviour. This has become the main quest fortrait theorists. As we have seen, there are an enormous num-ber of personality traits; but identifying the source traits willreduce this number. By using a smaller number of sourcetraits, psychologists can then construct personality tests thatinclude only measures of surface traits that relate to thesource traits. Personality tests produced in this way will pro-vide better measures of individual differences in personality.

Cattell (1957) makes it clear that it is necessary to use abroad range of personality descriptors to ensure that theappropriate source traits are discovered. He began his questfor the underlying structure of personality with the list of4,500 trait names as identified by Allport and Odbert(1936). You may recall that we mentioned this list earlier inthe chapter in the section on Allport. Firstly, using teams ofraters, Cattell removed all the synonyms. This left him witha list of 171 trait names. By getting raters to assess individ-uals on these traits, he reduced the list further to produce36 surface traits. Ten other surface traits were identified infurther studies on personality assessment and from a re-view of the psychiatric literature. Thus, Cattell concludedthat 46 surface traits are sufficient to describe individualdifferences in personality (Cattell and Kline, 1977).

Beginning with these 46 surface traits, Cattell used a va-riety of approaches to uncover the source traits of person-ality. The aim was to factor-analyse measures of all the 46surface traits collected from large samples of individuals.As you will see in the material on factor analysis (Chap-ter 25), large numbers of participants are required for factoranalysis. Cattell used different data collection procedures toobtain his data sets. One source of data he called L-data,short for ‘life record data’. These are measurements of be-haviour taken from the person’s actual life. Ideally it mightbe things like the A-level grades the person got, the degreethey were awarded, the number of car accidents they hadand so on. Such data could be difficult to obtain, so Cattell

settled for ratings of the individual’s behaviour by individ-uals who knew them well in a particular situation. In aschool setting it might be teacher’s ratings of aspects of theindividual’s ability, sociability, conscientiousness, and/orfellow students’ ratings. In a work setting, it might be rat-ings by colleagues or managers, for example. These individ-uals would rate aspects of their target colleague’s behaviourusing a 10-point Likert Scale.

A second type of data collection involved using person-ality questionnaires. Cattell called this Q-data. This is thepaper-and-pencil questionnaire that is widely used as anassessment tool in psychology.

Cattell’s final method of generating data involved gettingparticipants to complete tests under standardised testingconditions, but the tests are such that the responses cannotbe faked. He called the data collected T-data and claimedthat it represents truly objective test data. In normal ques-tionnaires, respondents may lie about some of their answersto create a good impression, for example. However, partici-pants completing the objective tests that produce T-data donot know what is being measured, so they cannot distorttheir answers. Cattell (1965) gives the Rorschach inkblot testas an example of such a test. Participants are presented witha series of different inkblots and have to report what theysee. Clinical psychologists then interpret this information.

From the factor analyses of huge data sets gatheredusing these different procedures, Cattell identified16 major source factors (Cattell, 1971; Cattell and Kline,1977). Further research identified another 7 factors; but hisbest-known measure of personality, the Sixteen PersonalityFactor (16PF) questionnaire uses the original 16 factors asthey are the most robust measures. Cattell and his co-researchers have identified these 16 source traits as repre-senting the basic structure of personality. He also rankedthe traits in terms of how important they were in predict-ing an individual’s behaviour. In the following list, we willpresent the 16 factors in this order, so that the most predic-tive items come first. Each factor represents a continuumalong which individuals are ranked. At one end, individualspossess extremely high levels of the factor; at the other end,their levels are extremely low. Cattell (1965) was at pains topoint out that almost all of the source traits have positiveand negative aspects at each end of the continuum. We willhighlight an example as we go through the trait descrip-tions. In labelling the source traits, we will use the factorletters that Cattell used to describe each factor, followed bywhat the scales measure (which has come to be the popularname for each of the scales) and then by the technical labelsthat Cattell has assigned to each factor (in parentheses). Bydoing this, we want to ensure that you will recognise thetraits in other texts, where any of these names may be used.

� Factor A, Outgoing–Reserved (affectothymia–schizo-thymia). This factor measures whether individuals are

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 163

Page 11: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

outgoing or reserved. It is the largest factor. The technicallabels Cattell chose for the endpoints of this factor, affec-tothymia (outgoing) and schizothymia (reserved), reflectthe history of the employment of this trait in psychiatry.The outgoing–reserved dimension was shown to be impor-tant in determining which individuals were hospitalised formental illness (Cattell, 1965).

� Factor B, Intelligence (High ‘8’–Low ‘8’). Cattell wasthe first to include intelligence as an ability trait. Herated it as the second best predictor of behaviour in hisinitial analysis of the factors that best predict actualbehaviour.

� Factor C, Stable–Emotional (high ego strength–lowego strength). This source trait measures emotionalstability and the ability an individual has to control theirimpulses and solve problems effectively (Cattell, 1965).At the positive end, individuals are rated as being stableindividuals who cope well in their lives and are realisticin their approach to life. At the negative end, individualsare emotionally labile. They are more neurotic andhighly anxious.

� Factor E, Assertive–Humble (dominance–submissive-ness). At the dominant end individuals display the sur-face traits of boastfulness, aggression, self-assertiveness,conceit, forcefulness, wilfulness, egotism and vigour.Humble or submissive individuals are seen to be mod-est, unsure, quiet, obedient, meek and retiring. This traitis the first to display a mixture of positive and negativeattributes at each end of the scale. Dominant individu-als have positive qualities of vigour and forcefulness butare boastful and egotistical.

� Factor F, Happy-go-lucky–Sober (surgency–desur-gency). When discussing this term, Cattell defendedhis creation of new terms like surgency to describe hissource traits. He suggests that the common names fortraits often do not accurately represent what psycholo-gists mean, so it is better to use a technical term that canbe defined more precisely. High surgency individualsare cheerful, sociable, responsive, joyous, witty, humor-ous, talkative and energetic. He suggests that this ismore than simply happy-go-lucky, the popular namefor the term. Desurgent individuals are pessimistic, in-clined to depression, reclusive, introspective, given toworrying, retiring and subdued. Cattell (1980) statedthat this is the most important single predictive factor inchildren’s personalities. He explored the influence of ge-netic factors on this trait and suggested that 55 per centof the variance on this trait is due to heredity.

� Factor G, Conscientious–Expedient (high superego–lowsuperego). Cattell (1965) compares this factor to Freud’sconcept of the superego. Individuals high in conscien-tiousness are persistent and reliable and exercise good self-control. At the other end of the continuum, expedient

individuals tend to take the line of least resistance ratherthan be guided by their principles.

� Factor H, Venturesome–Shy (parmia–threctia). HereCattell contrasts the bold, genial, adventurous, gregari-ous, individual (venturesome) with the shy, aloof, self-contained, timid individual (shy). Cattell’s technicallabels for these terms are not as obscure as they seem atfirst sight. The terms relate to the autonomic nervous sys-tem, specifically the sympathetic and parasympatheticsystems. The sympathetic nervous system produces thebody’s fight-or-flight response in the presence of a stres-sor of some sort. Put simply, the parasympathetic systemis involved in maintaining more normal, relaxed func-tioning. Parmia is an abbreviation of ‘parasympatheticimmunity’, meaning that the individual remains calmunder potentially threatening circumstances. They areimmune to the effects of the sympathetic system. Simi-larly, threctia stands for ‘threat reactivity’ and hence isused to label an individual who has a reactive sympa-thetic system. Cattell (1982) undertook studies on theheritability of this trait and concluded that the geneticfactor accounted for approximately 40 per cent of thevariance.

� Factor I, Tender-minded–Tough-minded (premsia–harria). The popular name describes this trait well.Tough-minded individuals are mature, independent-minded, self-sufficient and realistic. Tender-minded in-dividuals are gentle, imaginative, anxious, impatient,demanding, immature, creative, neurotic and sentimen-tal. The technical terms are derived from the phrases‘protected emotional sensitivity’ (premsia) and ‘hardrealism’ (harria).

� Factor L, Suspicious–Trusting (protension–alaxia).Individuals high in factor L are at the suspicious end ofthe continuum and, as well as being suspicious, are jealousand withdrawn from others. Those scoring low on factorL are trusting, composed and understanding. Cattell(1957) explains that the technical term protension is de-rived from the words ‘projection’ and ‘tension’. Alaxia isfrom the term ‘relaxation’.

� Factor M, Imaginative–Practical (autia–praxernia).The individual high in factor M is unconventional, in-tellectual and imaginative. They may often be uncon-cerned with the practicalities of life. The technical termautia comes from the word ‘autistic’. Praxernia is derivedfrom ‘practical and concerned’. Such individuals areconventional, practical, logical, with a tendency to worry, and conscientious.

� Factor N, Shrewd–Forthright (shrewdness–artlessness).Here the descriptors fit the labels well. The shrewd indi-vidual is astute, worldly, smart and insightful (Cattell andKline, 1977). The forthright individual is spontaneous,unpretentious and somewhat naïve.

164 PART 1 PERSONALITY

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 164

Page 12: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 165

� Factor O, Apprehensive–Placid (guilt-proneness–assurance). High levels of guilt-proneness are concep-tualised as a purely negative trait by Cattell and Kline(1977). It is seen to be typical of criminals, alcoholics,other drug abusers and individuals suffering from manicdepression. Individuals low in factor O are placid,resilient and self-confident (Cattell and Kline, 1977).

If you recall, at the beginning of this section you weretold that the factors are presented in their order of impor-tance in explaining individual differences in behaviour.The remaining four Q factors, therefore, are not particu-larly good predictors of behaviour; but some of them havebeen researched extensively.

� Factor Q1, Experimenting–Conservative (radicalism–conservatism). It is suggested that conservatives have ageneral fear of uncertainty and thus opt for the knownand the well established. Radicals, on the other hand,prefer the non-conventional and conform less to therules of society than conservatives do (Cattell, 1957).

� Factor Q2, Self-sufficiency–Group-tied (self-sufficiency–group adherence). This factor is self-explanatory. It de-scribes the individual’s preference to go it alone or theirneed to be part of a group.

� Factor Q3, Controlled–Casual (high self-concept–lowintegration). Individuals high in factor Q3 are compul-sive individuals. They crave a controlled environmentthat is highly predictable. Individuals low in factor Q3

are undisciplined, lax individuals who have a preferencefor disorganisation in their surroundings.

� Factor Q4, Tense–Relaxed (high ergic tension–low ergictension). Again, this factor is largely self-explanatory.Those high in factor Q4 are tense, driven individuals;while at the other end of the continuum, individuals arerelaxed and easygoing (Cattell, 1973).

Contribution of Cattell

As we have seen, Cattell was keen to develop a comprehen-sive, empirically based trait theory of personality. Heacknowledged the complexity of factors that all contributeto explain human behaviour, including genetics and envi-ronmental factors as well as ability and personality charac-teristics. Cattell (1965, 1980) was adamant that the test ofany good personality theory was its ability to predictbehaviour; he even produced an extremely complex math-ematical equation that he suggested could do this. Hewrote about the effect of learning on personality develop-ment and even turned his attention to classifying abnormalbehaviour. While he produced vast amounts of empiricallybased work and attempted to develop a truly comprehen-sive theory of personality, he is best known in psychologyfor the 16PF (Cattell, Eber and Tatsuoka, 1970).

The Sixteen Personality Factor (16PF) questionnaire hasbecome a standard measure of personality and has beenused consistently since its publication. However, the inter-nal consistencies of some of the scales were quite low, and ithas been revised and improved (Conn and Rieke, 1994). Todo this, the questionnaire has been changed substantially,with over 50 per cent of the items being new or signifi-cantly modified.

Although these revisions have produced a better measurepsychometrically, it does mean that studies using the 16PFcannot be directly compared with the work that uses the ear-lier measure. The earlier measure had good predictability.Studies were undertaken that linked participation in churchactivities to differences in personality characteristics (Cattell,1973; Cattell and Child, 1975). Other researchers demon-strated that the 16PF was a good predictor of success in dif-ferent school subjects (Barton, Dielman and Cattell, 1971).

Given the amount that Cattell published, it is perhapssurprising that this work has not had more impact. Part ofthe reason for this is that much of his work is difficult tounderstand. His use of obscure labels for his factors and thecomplex systems that he postulated are not reader friendly.He put great emphasis on the objectivity of his approachand did not acknowledge the inherent subjectivity involvedin factor analysis, linked to the initial selection of traits theresearcher chooses to measure and the explanatory labelsthey select for their underlying factors. Trait approachesgenerally will be evaluated at the end of the chapter so arenot being repeated here. What we need to remember at thispoint is that Cattell suggested that the underlying structureof personality consists of 16 factors.

Which one of Cattell’s 16 personality factors mightdescribe this man: outgoing-reserved, stable-emotional, happy-go-lucky-sober, venturesome-shy,apprehensive-placid, experimenting-conservative?Source: The Kobal Collection

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 165

Page 13: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

166 PART 1 PERSONALITY

Hans Eysenck’s trait theory of personality

When Hans Eysenck began to work in the area of personality,he observed that there were two schools within psychology.The first consisted of personality theorists whose main focuswas on the development of theories, with little if any empha-sis on evaluating these theories with empirical evidence. Thesecond group was made up of experimental psychologistswho had little interest in individual differences. Eysenck(1947) stressed the need for an integration of these two ap-proaches. He outlined his goals as being to identify the maindimensions of personality, devise means of measuring themand test them using experimental, quantitative procedures.He felt that these steps would lead to the development ofsound personality theory. (See Profile: Hans Eysenck.)

Eysenck (1947, 1952) accepted the conventional wisdomthat assumed that children inherit personality characteris-tics from their parents and other members of their family.At the time he was writing, the main theoretical slant inpsychology was that babies were relatively blank slates andthat while development was limited by differences in intel-ligence or physical skills, it was environmental experiences,particularly parenting styles, that largely influenced the de-velopment of personality. This was a legacy from the strongtradition of behaviourism. Over fifty years ago, Eysenckwas stressing the importance of genetic inheritance, a viewthat has gained ground within psychology. We know fromphysiology that there are differences in physiological func-tioning between individuals and that these biological dif-ferences often translate into different behaviour. Eysenck’searly claim that there is a large biological determinant to

personality was originally met with scepticism; but as youwill read in Chapter 8, it has become accepted as support-ing evidence has emerged from biological research.

Eysenck began by examining historical approaches topersonality, including the work of Hippocrates and Galenthat we covered earlier. His aim was to uncover the under-lying structure of personality. The historical evidence sug-gested to him that there are different personality types, andthe definition of personality that he adopted incorporatesthis concept. Eysenck (1970) defines personality as beingthe way that an individual’s character, temperament, intel-ligence, physique and nervous system are organised. Hesuggests that this organisation is relatively stable and long-lasting. Traits are the relatively stable, long-lasting charac-teristics of the individual. In common with other traitresearchers, Eysenck has utilised factor analysis. Hecollected measurements of personality traits from largesamples of individuals and factor-analysed them. Aftermany years of research, he concluded that there are threebasic personality dimensions, which he called types, andthat all traits can be subsumed within these three types. Be-fore we examine the three types, we need to become famil-iar with Eysenck’s model of personality.

Eysenck’s structure of personality

Beginning with observations of individual behaviour that hecalls specific responses, Eysenck developed a hierarchicaltypology. An example of the methodology he used will makethis clearer. For example, you would watch someone talkingwith their friends one evening and carefully observe theirspecific responses. If this person spends a great deal of their

Profile

Hans Eysenck

Hans J. Eysenck was born in Berlin in 1916 during theFirst World War, to parents who were both actors. Hisparents divorced when he was only two years old and,as his mother was a silent film star, he went to live withhis grandmother. Aged 6 years old, Eysenck appearedin a film alongside his mother. His father would haveliked him to pursue an acting career, but his mother dis-couraged it. As a young man he was opposed to Hitlerand the Nazi party and left Germany in 1934. He hadbeen told that he could not go to university unless hejoined the Nazi party, and he was unwilling to do this.He went first to France before finally settling inEngland. In London, he studied for his undergraduatedegree at the University of London, and it is said that he

only specialised in psychology as he did not have theprerequisite subjects to study physics. He obtained hisPhD in 1940 and tried to join the Royal Air Force to fightin the Second World War; but he was not accepted, ashe was German and considered to be an enemy alien.Instead he went to work at a mental hospital and con-tinued with his research career. After the war, he wentto work at the Maudsley Hospital in London, where hesoon established the first training course in clinical psy-chology. Eysenck continued to work at the Maudsley,where he was a prolific researcher. He publishedaround 45 books and hundreds of research papers andedited chapters. He continued working up until hisdeath from cancer in 1997.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 166

Page 14: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 167

time talking with friends, you can begin to observe some ofwhat Eysenck calls their habitual responses. Thus, habit-ual responses are the ways that individuals typically behavein a situation. From continued observations of the same in-dividual, you might observe that this person seeks out occa-sions to interact with others and really enjoys social events.The conclusion would be that this person is very sociable, orin personality terms, they possess the trait of sociability. Thisstructure of personality is shown in Figure 7.2. From the di-agram, you can see that specific responses that are found to-gether in the individual make up habitual responses, andcollections of habitual responses that the individual pro-duces make up the next level of personality traits. Using fac-tor analyses, Eysenck argued that traits such as sociability,liveliness, activity, assertiveness and sensation seeking are

highly correlated. This means that an individual’s scores oneach of these traits are likely to be very similar. This collec-tion of traits then forms a supertrait or personality type.Each supertrait represents a continuum along which indi-viduals can be placed, depending on the degree of the attrib-ute they possess.

Eysenck originally suggested that there are two super-traits. The first is a measure of sociability with extraversionat one end of the continuum and introversion at the other.Extraverts are sociable and impulsive people who like ex-citement and whose orientation is towards external reality.Introverts are quiet, introspective individuals who are ori-ented towards inner reality and who prefer a well-orderedlife. The personality traits that make up extraversion areshown in Figure 7.3.

Trait

Personality type Trait level Habitual response (HR) Specific response

SR1

SR4

SR5

SR6

SR7

SR8

HR1

HR2

HR3

HR4

SR3

SR2

Sociable AssertiveActive

Sensation-seeking Carefree Dominant Surgent Venturesome

Lively

Extraversion

Figure 7.3 Traits that make up extraversion.Source: Based on Eysenck and Eysenck (1985a).

Figure 7.2 Eysenck’s hierarchical model of personality.Source: Adapted from Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The Biological Basis of Personality. Springfield, IL: CharlesC. Thomas, p. 36. Reprinted courtesy of Charles C. Thomas Publisher, Ltd.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 167

Page 15: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

168 PART 1 PERSONALITY

The second personality type or supertrait isneuroticism. Individuals can be placed on this dimensionaccording to the degree of neuroticism they possess.Eysenck (1965b) defines neurotics as emotionally unstableindividuals. He describes several types of neurotic behav-iour. Some individuals high in neuroticism may have unrea-sonable fears (phobias) of certain objects, places, animals orpeople. Others may have obsessional or impulsive symp-toms. The distinguishing feature of neurotic behaviour isthat the individual displays an anxiety or fear level that isdisproportionate to the realities of the situation. The traitsthat make up neuroticism are shown in Figure 7.4. Eysenckdoes separate out one group of neurotics who are free fromanxiety and fear, and he labels this group psychopaths.These are individuals who behave in an antisocial mannerand seem unable to appreciate the consequences of their ac-tions despite any punishment meted out (H. Eysenck,1965b). Such individuals are described as acting as if theyhave no conscience and showing no remorse for things theyhave done. Psychopathic personalities are likely to be foundwithin the prison population.

The recognition of this group of psychopaths byEysenck led to the identification of a third personality fac-tor. As the two personality types (extraversion and neuroti-cism) did not adequately explain all of Eysenck’s data, headded a third type, psychoticism. It is the severity of thedisorder that differentiates psychotics from neurotics. Psy-chotics display the most severe type of psychopathology,frequently being insensitive to others, hostile, cruel and in-humane with a strong need to ridicule and upset others.The traits that come together to form psychoticism areshown in Figure 7.5.

Eysenck and Eysenck (1985a) stated that despite havingall these undesirable traits, psychotics still tend to be creativeindividuals. Eysenck quoted several sources of evidence tosupport his hypothesis. First, he provided historical exam-ples of individuals he felt were geniuses and who had all dis-played personality traits typical of psychoticism. He definedgeniuses as being extremely creative individuals, and he sug-gested that many of the traits associated with psychoticismcould be perceived as aiding a creative career. Traits such asbeing egocentric, so you always put yourself first; being

Anxious Low self-esteemGuilt feelings

EmotionalMoodyTense Irrational Shy

Depressed

Neuroticism

Creative

Aggressive Cold ImpersonalEgocentric

AntisocialTough-mindedImpulsive Unempathic

Psychoticism

Figure 7.4 Traits that make up neuroticism.Source: Based on Eysenck and Eysenck (1985a).

Figure 7.5 Traits that make up psychoticism.Source: Based on Eysenck and Eysenck (1985a).

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 168

Page 16: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 169

tough-minded, so that you pursue your own goals regardlessof others or circumstances; being unempathic, so that youare not affected by other people’s emotions and problems.Psychological studies of great individuals have demonstratedthat they have needed to be self-centred and persistent toovercome the obstacles that they faced in their lives and thatthey also possess the ability to think in unusual, almostbizarre ways (Simonton, 1994). Eysenck (1995) cited evi-dence that psychotic individuals perform well on tests of cre-ativity that require divergent thinking. By divergentthinking, he meant the ability to produce novel ideas that aredifferent from those that most people produce. He claimedthat psychotics and geniuses have an overinclusive cognitivestyle that allows them to consider divergent solutions toproblems. These views of Eysenck’s are not universally ac-cepted. As Simonton (1994) points out, humanistic psychol-ogists such as Maslow and Rogers asserted that creativity isthe result of optimum mental health, which implies bal-anced personalities. Eysenck (1995) did admit that more re-search is required in this area.

Eysenck (1967) claimed that these three types or super-traits make up the basic structure of personality, and he de-veloped an instrument to measure the three types and theirsupporting traits. This is called the Eysenck PersonalityQuestionnaire (EPQ; H. Eysenck and S. Eysenck, 1975). Hesuggested that there is a link between the clinical condi-tions of neurosis and psychoses and his scales of neuroti-cism and psychoticism. Individuals who score highly onneuroticism or psychoticism are not necessarily neurotic orpsychotic, but he argued that they are at risk of developingthese disorders. High scores indicate a predisposition,which may develop under adverse circumstances.

Eysenck’s next task was to explain why individuals whodiffered along the supertrait dimensions should behave dif-ferently. His theoretical exposition, while not ignoring en-vironmental influences, was heavily biological. Indeed,Eysenck (1982a) claimed that about two-thirds of the vari-ance in personality development can be attributed to bio-logical factors. Environment plays a part particularly ininfluencing how traits are expressed, but Eysenck wouldargue that biology has imposed limits on how much an in-dividual personality can change. A full account of Eysenck’sbiological explanation of personality differences is pro-vided in Chapter 8 with the other biological theories.

Research evidence for Eysenck’s types

Many predictions have been made from this theory, andthere is a high level of support over a period of 40 years.For example, Eysenck (1965b) reported that extravertscompared with introverts prefer to socialise. They likelouder music and brighter colours, and they are morelikely to smoke, drink more alcohol and engage in more

varied sexual activities. These differences generally con-tinue to be reported in the literature. Amirkham, Risingerand Swickert (1995) found that extraverts are more likelythan introverts to attract and maintain networks of friendsand to approach others for help when they are undergoinga crisis. Eysenck and Eysenck (1975) reported that extraverts,due to their need for variety in their lives, have more ca-reer changes or job changes. Extraverts are also more likelyto change relationship partners more frequently. Camp-bell and Hawley (1982) looked at the study habits and thepreferred location for studying of students and found thatintroverts prefer to study in quiet areas, while extravertsstudy in areas where there are other people and opportu-nities to socialise. Extraverts also took more study breaksthan introverts did, indicating that they have a higher needfor change in their activities and environment. Davies andParasuraman (1992) reported that extraverts tire moreeasily than introverts on tasks requiring vigilance and aremore likely to make errors. While there continues to be asignificant amount of research utilising versions of theEPQ (H. Eysenck and S. Eysenck, 1975, 1991), the neuroti-cism and the extraversion scales have proved to be goodreliable measures psychometrically; the psychoticism scaleis more problematic, with much lower internal reliabilitystatistics. You can refer to Chapter 24 for a detailed expla-nation of reliability statistics. Eysenck (1967) admittedthat this scale is less robust and did refine it somewhat (H.Eysenck, 1992), but despite this, it remains the weakestmeasure.

If the three-factor solution represents the basic struc-ture of personality, it should be found cross-culturally.Eysenck and Eysenck undertook a considerable pro-gramme of cross-cultural research to explore whether histheory held. His EPQ was carefully translated into manydifferent languages. This research is summarised inEysenck and Eysenck (1982). He reported that the primaryfactors were found in at least 24 nations in both males andfemales. His sample included African, Asian, North Ameri-can and many European cultures. From this data and fromtwin studies, described in Chapter 8, Eysenck concludedthat the three-factor structure has a genetic basis and rep-resents the basic structure of personality.

Sybil Eysenck produced a child’s version of the EPQ,called the Junior Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (S. Eysenck, 1965). It was also translated into many lan-guages, and again the cross-cultural evidence was consis-tent. Studies of children found the same three factorscross-culturally. This provided additional evidence for histheory. He followed up this research with longitudinalstudies to demonstrate that the structure was stable acrosstime (H. Eysenck, 1967, 1982a, 1990b, 1993; S. Eysenck,Barrett and Barnes, 1993; S. Eysenck, Makaremi andBarrett, 1994). S. Eysenck concluded that all this researchprovided confirmatory evidence that there is a genetic basis

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 169

Page 17: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

170 PART 1 PERSONALITY

for the primary personality types. They are all found cross-culturally, despite social pressures within different culturesto develop in specific ways. The same structures are foundin children as in adults. Reviews of studies of identical andfraternal twins, raised together and raised apart, found thesame structures and personality similarities between indi-vidual biological relatives and lend considerable supportfor a significant genetic component to personality. As men-tioned previously, details of genetic studies of personalityare included in Chapter 8.

Eysenck (1990b) does still see a role for the environmentin the development of personality. He suggested that whileindividuals’ genes provide a strong tendency to become acertain type of person, some modification is possible. Hesuggested that the way that children are socialised was cru-cial here. However, he did not provide a detailed develop-mental theory to explain how the environment mightintervene in development or to specify the environmentthat would promote healthy development.

Psychopathology and Eysenck’stherapeutic approach

Eysenck was a behaviourist, and therefore he placed a lot ofemphasis on how learned behaviour was acquired. Thus,healthy and abnormal behaviour is the result of the waythat individuals respond to the stimuli in their environ-ment. Some individuals are more susceptible to developingpsychopathology because of their inherited vulnerabilities.For example, Eysenck suggested that individuals who scorehighly on the personality trait neurosis are more likely todevelop clinical neuroses than are those with low scores.

Eysenck’s approach to treatment involved behaviourtherapy. You may recall that we covered this in Chapter 4.He was extremely hostile to all other therapies but particu-larly targeted psychoanalytic approaches. Indeed, Eysenck(1965a) claimed that the only effective therapy was behav-iour therapy. As mentioned in the Profile box, Eysenck de-veloped clinical psychology training in the UnitedKingdom and was an active clinician as well as a personal-ity researcher for much of his life.

Eysenck’s contribution to traittheorising

Eysenck’s theorising is fairly comprehensive, although notall aspects of it are equally well developed. This is particu-larly true of the developmental aspects and the biologicalbasis, as you will see in Chapter 8. He also focuses heavilyon genetic factors and pays much less attention to the so-cial context within which much behaviour occurs and thatmay affect personality and behaviour in particular situa-tions. He would argue that personality determines to some

extent the situations that individuals choose to be in, butthat is debatable to some extent. In terms of heuristicvalue, Eysenck has been very influential. His critique of alltherapies, apart from behaviour therapy, stimulated thera-pists to evaluate their work and led to a large increase inevaluative research on therapies. His work also has signifi-cant applied value. He demonstrated a rigorous approachto personality theorising. He moved beyond many person-ality trait researchers in that he tried to provide not merelya description of personality structure but also an explana-tion of what caused differences in personality, with his ge-netic studies and his biological theory. He also provided afairly robust measure of personality. His work has stimu-lated an enormous amount of research. Eysenck foundedthe journal Personality and Individual Differences, and itscontinued growth and development attests to his influenceover many years.

In one other aspect, his theory can perhaps be criti-cised for being too parsimonious, having only three fac-tors. Do three factors really represent the basic structureof personality? This question of the number of factorsnecessary to describe personality structure is what weshall discuss next. There has been considerable debate inthe psychological literature about the number of factorsrequired for an adequate description of personality and,as we shall see, Eysenck before his death contributed tothis discussion.

The five-factor model

Psychologists increasingly agree that five supertraits mayadequately describe the structure of personality. The evi-dence to support this contention has come from severalsources. There is still some debate, as we shall see, abouthow to label these factors; but this is perhaps unsurprisinggiven that assigning labels is the most subjective aspect offactor analysis. Researchers are likely to have differentopinions about which words best describe the constituenttraits that make up a supertrait. We shall begin by examin-ing the evidence for five factors, and then we will look atwhere this leaves Eysenck’s three-factor model and Cattell’ssixteen-factor model. Finally, we will evaluate the traitapproach to personality.

Evidential sources for the five-factor model

There are three evidential sources for the five-factor model:

� the lexical approach;� factor analysis evidence for the five-factor model;� other evidence in support of the five-factor model of

personality.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 170

Page 18: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 171

The lexical approach

You will recall that earlier in this chapter, we discussed thelexical hypothesis. This is the hypothesis that it is the differ-ences in personality that are important for social interaction,and human societies have labelled these differences as singleterms. Several detailed accounts of the lexical approach andits history are available if you want to explore this theory fur-ther (De Raad, 2000; Saucier and Goldberg, 2001).

You will recall that Cattell’s 16PF came from the factoranalysis of the list of 4,500 trait names identified by Allportand Odbert (1936). Cattell produced a 16-factor solution.Fiske (1949) reanalysed the same data but could not repro-duce the 16 factors; he published instead a five-factor solu-tion. This work was ignored for a long time. Tupes andChristal (1961/1992) reported five factors from analyses oftrait words in eight different samples. Norman (1963) re-visited the earlier research and reproduced the same five-factor structure using personality ratings of individualsgiven by their peers. Digman and Takemoto-Chock (1981)carried out further analyses and confirmed Norman’s five-factor solution. Goldberg (1981) reviewed all the researchand made a convincing argument for the Big Five. Sincethen, Goldberg and his team have carried out an extensiveresearch programme investigating personality traits, andGoldberg (1990) concluded that in the English languagetrait descriptors are versions of five major features of per-sonality: love, work, affect, power and intellect. Since then,the research has spread to other languages. Saucier andOstendorf (1999) used a set of five hundred personalitytraits and found a five-factor structure in the Germanlanguage, for example.

Saucier and Goldberg (2001) have described the lexicalapproach to investigating whether the five-factor structureis universally applicable as an emic approach to research.(See Stop and think: Lexical approaches produce descrip-tive models of personality traits, below.) Basically, what theresearchers do is to use the personality terms that are foundin the native language of the country. They contrast thiswith what they call the etic approach, which uses person-

ality questionnaires translated from another language thatin practice tends to be English. Saucier and Goldberg(2001) report that etic approaches tends to replicate thefive-factor structure while there is more variability re-ported in studies using emic approaches. Perugini and Di-Blas (2002) discuss this issue further in relation to emicand etic data they collected on Italian samples. They pointout that in the etic approach, the questionnaires beingtranslated are based on five-factor structures found in theoriginal language. Goldberg and his research team make acase for the necessity of further study of cultural differ-ences in personality trait use that are being found usingemic approaches as a core part of the search for the univer-sal structure of personality. Goldberg’s research team hasmade available copyrighted free adjective scales that can beused to measure the five factors and personality scales formeasuring them. These can be accessed from his websiteand the address included at the end of the chapter.

Factor analysis evidence for the five-factormodel of personality

This is the second source of evidence for the existence of astructure of five factors. Costa and McCrae (1985, 1989,1992, 1997) are arguably the most influential researchers inthis area, and their factor solution has come to be called theBig Five Model. This approach requires large samples ofparticipants to complete at least two personality question-naires. The resultant data set is then factor-analysed to un-cover clusters of traits. The consistent finding is theemergence of five factors or dimensions of personality.

It is important to stress that it is the analysis of data thathas produced the factors, not exploration of a theory aboutthe number of factors necessary in a model. This is not theusual approach in psychology. Usually researchers beginwith a theoretically based hypothesis about some aspect ofbehaviour. They then collect their data, and their results ei-ther support or refute their original theory driven hypoth-esis. In contrast, with the five-factor research, the

Stop and think

Lexical approaches produce descriptive models of personality traits

You may recall from our early discussion that lexical ap-proaches produce descriptive models of personalitytraits, and you need to bear this fact in mind. At thisstage in their development, the lexical approaches donot explain why this structure is found, other than torefer to the lexical hypothesis. There are no explanatory

models offered. However, they are a valuable source ofconfirmatory evidence for the existence of the five-factor model. If Saucier and Goldberg’s suggestion toexplore the differences uncovered by emic studies isfollowed up, this then might lead to explanatory modelslinking differences to cultural practices.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 171

Page 19: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

172 PART 1 PERSONALITY

hypothesis that five factors represent the basic structure ofpersonality has come from the data that was collected. Inother words, the Big Five model is a data-derived hypothe-sis as opposed to a theoretically based one.

These are the factors described by the American person-ality researchers Costa and McCrae (1992), who measuredpersonality with their well-known Neuroticism, Extraver-sion, Openness Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R). The BigFive factors are Openness, Conscientiousness, Extra-version, Agreeableness and Neuroticism. You can usethe acronym OCEAN to help you remember what the fac-tors are called. More detailed descriptions of each factorare now provided. Each factor represents a continuumalong which individuals can be placed according to theirscores:

� Openness – This factor refers to the individual havingan openness to new experiences. It includes the charac-teristics of showing intellectual curiosity, divergentthinking and a willingness to consider new ideas and anactive imagination. Individuals scoring highly on open-ness are unconventional and independent thinkers. In-dividuals with low scores are more conventional andprefer the familiar to the new.

� Conscientiousness – This factor describes our degree ofself-discipline and control. Individuals with high scoreson this factor are determined, organised and plan forevents in their lives. Individuals with low scores tend tobe careless, easily distracted from their goals or tasksthat they are undertaking and undependable. If youlook closely at the trait descriptors included in consci-entiousness, you will see that they are all attributes likelyto become apparent in work situations. For this reason,they are sometimes referred to as the will to achieve orwork dimension.

� Extraversion – This factor is a measure of the individ-ual’s sociability. It is the same factor as described byEysenck earlier in this chapter and by the psychoanalystJung in Chapter 3. Individuals who score highly on ex-traversion are very sociable, energetic, optimistic,

friendly and assertive. Individuals with high scores arelabelled extraverts. As with the Eysenck and Jung de-scriptions, individuals with low scores are labelled in-troverts. Introverts are described as being reserved,independent rather than followers socially, even-pacedrather than sluggish in terms of their pace of work.

� Agreeableness – This factor relates very much to char-acteristics of the individual that are relevent for socialinteraction. Individuals with high scores are trusting,helpful, softhearted and sympathetic. Those with lowscores are suspicious, antagonistic, unhelpful, scepticaland uncooperative.

� Neuroticism – This factor measures an individual’semotional stability and personal adjustment. Costa andMcCrae (1992) suggest that although a range of emo-tions exists, individuals who score highly on one alsorate highly on others. In psychological terms, the vari-ous emotional states are highly correlated. Thus, the in-dividual who scores highly on neuroticism experienceswide swings in their mood and they are volatile in theiremotions. Individuals with low scores on the neuroti-cism factor are calm, well adjusted and not prone to ex-treme maladaptive emotional states. (Indeed, in somefive-factor models of personality, this dimension is re-ferred to as emotional stability.)

These are the five main dimensions popularly knownas the Big Five. Within each of the main dimensions thereare more specific personality attributes that cluster to-gether, and all contribute to the category score. Thesesubordinate traits are sometimes called facets (Costa andMcCrae, 1992). The Big Five model is a hierarchicalmodel similar in concept to Eysenck’s model. Each of theBig Five factors consists of six facets or subordinatetraits. The facets included in the NEO-PI-R (Costa andMcCrae, 1992) are shown in Table 7.3. Thus, an individ-ual’s scores on the traits of fantasy, aesthetics, feelings,actions, ideas and values combine to produce their scoreson the openness factor. The NEO-PI-R then allows meas-urement at a general factor level or on more specific

Table 7.3 The constituent facets of the Big Five factors.

Openness Conscientiousness Extraversion Agreeableness Neuroticism

FantasyAesthetics Order Gregariousness Straightforwardness Angry hostility

Dutifulness Assertiveness Altruism DepressionsAchievement striving Activity Compliance Self-consciousnessSelf-discipline Excitement seeking Modesty ImpulsivenessDeliberation Positive emotions Tender-mindedness Vulnerability

Competence Warmth Trust Anxiety

FeelingsActionsIdeasValues

Source: Based on Costa and McCrae (1985).

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 172

Page 20: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 173

factors. Obviously, the more specific the measure, thegreater the likelihood of using it to actually predict be-haviour.

Other evidence in support of the Big Five

There is too much research supporting the Big Five for usto review it all here. Instead, we will cite some examplesfrom the main areas. In terms of how well this model fitswith other measures of personality, the evidence is largelypositive. McCrae and Costa (1989) factor-analysed scoreson the Myers–Briggs Type Inventory and found that itsupports a five-factor structure. Boyle (1989) reportedthat the five-factor model is also broadly compatible withCattell’s fourteen-factor measure and Eysenck’s three-factor measure. The latest measure of the 16PFI allowsscoring on the Big Five (Conn and Rieke, 1994). Goldberg(1993) compared the five-factor model with Eysenck’sthree-factor model and concluded that two of the factors –extraversion and neuroticism – are very similar, and thatpsychoticism can be subsumed under agreeableness andconscientiousness.

The NEO-PI-R has also been translated into severalother languages, and the same factor structure has beenreplicated (McCrae and Costa, 1997; McCrae et al., 1998,2000). If you recall, this evidence is not uncontentious,based as it is on the etic approach to personality researchthat we discussed earlier. These researchers (McCrae andCosta, 1997; McCrae et al., 1998, 2000) have alsodemonstrated that the observed personality differencesare stable over time and have a genetic basis. To sum-marise, Costa and McCrae (1992) claim that the five fac-tors represent the universal structure of personalitybased on all the evidence we have discussed. The factorsare found in different languages, ages of people andraces.

Evaluation of the Big Five and trait approaches

Can we conclude then that the Big Five represent the struc-ture of personality? Unfortunately, it is premature to saythat there is total consensus on the model. There is increas-ing agreement that there are five factors, but there is stillsome level of disagreement about the exact nature of eachof the five factors. Indeed, Saucier and Goldberg (1998)and Saucier (1995) argue that research should look for so-lutions beyond the current five-factor models. This is thescientific approach – to search for contradictory evidenceinstead of purely focusing on searching for confirmation,as the present research does.

There is some debate about how the factors should belabelled. Labelling factors depends on the researcher’s

judgement about the best descriptor for the cluster of cor-related traits. For example, the agreeableness factor has alsobeen labelled conformity (Fiske, 1949) and likeability(Norman, 1963). The same debate applies for all the otherfactors.

Peabody and Goldberg (1989) have also demonstratedthat the measures that are included in a questionnaire cru-cially affect the final factors produced. If a questionnairedoes not have many items that measure openness, for ex-ample, then the description of openness that is producedwill be narrower. There is still some argument about thenumber of traits, with studies reporting different numbersbetween Eysenck’s three and seven (Briggs, 1989; Churchand Burke, 1994; Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Joireman, Tetaand Kraft, 1993). McCrae and Costa (1995) suggest thatthe number depends on the nature of the trait measuresthat are included. They point out that five-factor modelstend not to include evaluative traits like moral/immoral. Ifevaluative traits are included, Almagor, Tellegen andWaller (1995) have suggested that a seven-factor solutionemerges.

There has been some debate about what exactly someof the factors mean (Digman 1990). Are they perhaps lin-guistic categories that do not actually represent the under-lying structure of personality? Is it that the five factorsrepresent out ability to describe personality traits in lan-guage and are nothing to do with underlying structures?There is no easy answer to this question, although the ac-cumulating weight of research evidence would seem tonegate it. Is it perhaps that our cognitive abilities onlyallow for a five-factor structure but the reality is morecomplex and subtle?

Briggs (1989) has criticised the model for being atheo-retical. As we have discussed earlier, the model is datadriven and was not derived from a theoretical base. Thereare currently some attempts to address this with geneticstudies and the search for a physiological basis for the ob-served differences, as you will see in Chapter 8. This criti-cism applies more generally to the trait approach, althoughtheorists such as Eysenck saw theory building as being cru-cial within his approach.

One of the more general criticisms of trait approachesto personality is related to how the various measures areinterpreted and used. For example, Mischel (1968, 1983a,1990) has pointed out that many of these measures arelargely descriptive and do not predict behaviour particu-larly well. Despite this claim, many of these measures arewidely used to make important decisions about individu-als’ lives and in workplace situations are often blindly in-terpreted by people who are not psychologists. Mischel(1968) demonstrates that on average, personality traitmeasures statistically account for only around 10 percent of the variance observed in behaviour. In otherwords, 90 per cent of the variance in behaviour is down

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 173

Page 21: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

174 PART 1 PERSONALITY

to something other than the effect of personality. How-ever, Kraus (1995) has shown that the variance figure isnot insignificant and is similar to that found in studiesmeasuring the relationship between attitudes and behav-iour. Mischel’s criticism of the overreliance on traitmeasures to assess individuals has had beneficial effectsin work settings. The practice currently is to use multiplemeasures of personality assessment in work settings. Psy-chometric assessments, individual and group tasks andinterviews are frequently used together as an assessmentpackage, and this prevents overreliance on the psycho-metric tool.

Final comments

In summary, we have described the nomothetic approachto personality research. You should now appreciate the longhistory of attempts to describe and explain differences inpersonality. You should now understand what is meant bythe lexical hypothesis and be familiar with the approach todata analysis employed by trait theorists. You should alsobe aware of the contributions of Allport, Cattell andEysenck to understanding personality, as well as the ap-proaches that have resulted in the identification of the BigFive personality traits.

Summary

� Two assumptions underlie trait theory. The first is thatpersonality characteristics are relatively stable overtime. The second is that traits show stability acrosssituations.

� Trait theorists are aiming to find the basic structure ofpersonality and to produce reliable ways of measur-ing personality differences.

� William Sheldon outlined a description of personality,called somatypes, based on physique and tempera-ment. He described three basic types of physique –endomorphy, mesomorphy and ectomorphy – anddemonstrated that each body type was associatedwith a particular temperament.

� The lexical hypothesis was first put forth by SirFrancis Galton. It suggests that it is the important in-dividual differences between people that come to beencoded as single word terms (trait descriptors).The lexical hypothesis led to attempts to categorisethe important personality traits. With the advent offactor analysis, these trait lists were analysed to tryto uncover the underlying structure.

� Gordon Allport identified 18,000 words, of which4,500 described personality traits.

� Allport conceptualised human nature as normallybeing rational, creative, active and self-reliant. Heused the idiographic approach to discover personaldispositions. He described three types of personalitytraits: cardinal, central and secondary.

� Allport emphasised the importance of the concept ofself to any theory of personality. He hypothesisedthat children were not born with a concept of self butthat it gradually developed, and it was a lifelongprocess. Allport was a pioneer in trait theory, and oneof his important contributions was to alert psycholo-gists to the limitations of trait approaches.

� Cattell’s work marks the beginning of the search forthe structure of personality using factor analysis. He

made a distinction between traits that are geneticallydetermined and those that are the result of environ-mental experiences. He defined three different typesof traits: ability, temperament and dynamic. He subdi-vided dynamic traits into three types: attributes, senti-ments and ergs. All these types of dynamic traits areorganised in complex and interrelated ways to pro-duce a dynamic lattice. He makes a further distinctionbetween common traits and unique traits. The latteraccount for the uniqueness of human beings.

� Cattell made an important distinction between sur-face traits and source traits. Surface traits are collec-tions of trait descriptors that cluster together in manyindividuals and situations. Using factor analysis, heuncovered underlying traits that he called sourcetraits. These are responsible for the observed vari-ance in the surface traits.

� Cattell used a variety of approaches to uncover thesource traits of personality. He finally produced 16factors and claimed that they represent the basicstructure of personality. He developed the 16PF as ameasurement tool.

� Eysenck’s goals were to identify the main dimen-sions of personality, devise means of measuringthem and test them using experimental, quantitativeprocedures. He defined personality as being theway that an individual’s character, temperament,intelligence, physique and nervous system are or-ganised. Traits are the relatively stable, long-lastingcharacteristics of the individual.

� Eysenck developed a hierarchical model of personal-ity types. At the bottom level are specific behaviouralresponses called habitual responses. These cometogether to make up personality traits. Clusters oftraits come together to make up personality types.Using factor analysis, Eysenck identified three typesor supertraits that he hypothesised made up thebasic structure of personality. He developed the

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 174

Page 22: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 175

Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) to measurethese three types and their underlying traits.

� Eysenck claimed that about two-thirds of the vari-ance in personality development can be attributed tobiological factors. Environment influences how traitsare expressed, but Eysenck argues that biology hasimposed limits on how much an individual personal-ity can change.

� There is good support for neuroticism and extraver-sion, including cross-cultural, developmental andlongitudinal stability data. Psychoticism is the leastreliable dimension.

� Eysenck provided not merely a description of per-sonality structure but also an explanation of whatcauses differences in personality, with his geneticstudies and his biological theory. His work has stimu-lated an enormous amount of research.

� There is a growing consensus that five supertraitsmake up the basic structure of personality. Whilethere are arguments about the names accorded tothese factors, those chosen by Costa and McCraeare the most popular. The Big Five factors are

Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agree-ableness and Neuroticism (OCEAN).

� There are several sources of evidence underpinningthe Big Five structure of personality. The first of theseuses the lexical approach to uncover the structures.The second approach uses the factor analysis of per-sonality questionnaires.

� The Big Five model is hierarchical, similar in conceptto Eysenck’s model. Each of the Big Five factorsconsists of six facets or subordinate traits. Costa andMacrae’s NEO-PI-R measures both the subordinatetraits and the supertraits.

� There is increasing agreement that there are five fac-tors, but there is still some level of disagreementabout the exact nature of each of the five factors.Debate continues about how the factors should belabelled.

� The lack of an underpinning theory is problematic forsome psychologists. This trait approach is datadriven, not theoretically driven, although theoreticalsupport is now developing.

Connecting up

� The personality theories covered in this chapter repre-sent some of the most commonly used theories in theliterature regarding main personality and individualdifferences. We go on to discuss the biological aspects ofEysenck’s personality in the next chapter (Chapter 8),alongside other biological models of personality. InChapter 10 we also explore an additional model of per-sonality based on the lexical hypothesis, the HEXACO

model of personality, which is an extension of the fivefactor model of personality.

� You may also want to return to Chapter 3 to look at theorigins of extraversion in Jung’s theory of personality.

� Throughout the rest of book, when we consider per-sonality variables in a number of chapters, we gener-ally refer to the three-and five-factor models ofpersonality.

Critical thinking

Discussion questions

� Can you identify any traits that Allport would classify asunique?

� How useful are Allport’s categorisations of types oftraits? Can you identify examples of each type oftrait?

� Compare and contrast Cattell’s concept of ergs toFreud’s categories of instincts.

� Does Eysenck make a convincing case for his three-factor structure of personality?

� Have psychologists finally uncovered the basic structureof personality?

� Can you identify any problems with the currentapproaches to determining the structure of personality?

� ‘The five-factor model of personality is now dominantin the research literature’. Evaluate the validity of thisstatement.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 175

Page 23: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

176 PART 1 PERSONALITY

Essay questions

� Three, five or sixteen? Critically examine how many per-sonality factors there are.

� Evaluate Eysenck’s claim that his three factors areuniversal.

� Critically evaluate the evidence for the five-factor struc-ture of personality.

� Discuss the contribution of Gordon Allport to the traitapproach of personality.

� Discuss the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the traitapproach.

� Compare and contrast two of the following threetheories of personality– three-factor– five-factor– sixteen-factor

� Evaluate the five-factor model of personality and dis-cuss its relationship to Cattell’s theory of personality.

Going further

Books

� Allport, G. W. (1961) Pattern and growth inpersonality. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Thisis one of Allport’s later texts, and it is written in an ac-cessible style. It gives a comprehensive account of hisposition.

� Cattell, R. B. and Kline, P. (1977). The scientific analysisof behaviour. This book provides a fairly detailedaccount of Cattell’s theory, methodology and research.

� Costa, P. T. Jr and McCrae, R. R. (2003). Personality inadulthood, a five-factor theory. London: Guilford Press.

� Saucier, G., Hampson, S. E. and Goldberg, L. R. (2000).Cross-language studies of lexical personality factors. InS. E. Hampson (ed.), Advances in Personality Psychology.London: The Psychology Press. This reading is an excel-lent summary of the lexical approach applied cross-culturally.

� Eysenck, H. J. (1970). The structure of human personality(3rd edn). London: Methuen. This text goes into moredetail about Eysenck’s model and is presented in anaccessible format.

� Eysenck, H. J. and Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personalityand individual differences: A natural science approach.New York: Plenum Press. This book provides an excel-lent overview of Eysenck’s work.

� De Raad, B. (2000). The Big Five personality factors: Thepsycholexical approach to personality. Seattle, WA:Hogrefe and Huber. This book provides an excellentsummary of lexical approaches.

Journals

� Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P.and Kraft, M. (1993). A comparison of three structuralmodels for personality: The big three, the Big Five, and

the alternative five. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 65, 757–768. This paper provides a goodexample of the research in this area. Journal of Personal-ity and Social Psychology is published by the AmericanPsychological Association and is available online viaPsycARTICLES.

� Saucier, G. and Goldberg, L. R. (1998). What is beyondthe Big Five? Journal of Personality, 66, 495–524. Thispaper includes a critique of much of the currentresearch effort and some timely warnings about futuredirections. Journal of Personality is published by Black-well Publishing and is available online with BlackwellSynergy, Swets Wise and Ingenta.

� McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A.,Hrebícková, M., Avia, M. D., Sanz, J., Sánchez-Bernados,M. L., Kusdil, M. E., Woodfield, R., Saunders, P. R. andSmith, P. B. (2000). Nature over nurture: Temperament,personality, and life span development. Journal of Per-sonality and Social Psychology, 78, 173–186. This paperbegins to outline a theoretical underpinning for the BigFive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology is pub-lished by the American Psychological Association and isavailable online via PsycARTICLES.

You will regularly find research articles relating to thepersonality theories described in this chapter in thefollowing journals:

� European Journal of Personality. Published by Wiley.Available online via Wiley InterScience.

� Journal of Personality. Published by Blackwell Publish-ing. Available online via Blackwell Synergy, SwetsWiseand Ingenta.

� Personality Assessment. Published by the Society forPersonality Assessment. Available online via BusinessSource Premier.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 176

Page 24: A01 MALT2908 02 SE FM - is.muni.cz · trait theorists Be aware of the contribution of Gordon Allport to the trait approach Appreciate the contribution of Raymond Cattell to the trait

CHAPTER 7 THE TRAIT APPROACH TO PERSONALITY 177

� Journal of Research in Personality. Published by Acade-mic Press. Available online via IngentaJournals.

� Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. Pub-lished by Sage Publications for the Society for Person-ality and Social Psychology. Available online viaSwetsWise, Sage Online, Ingenta and Expanded Acad-emic ASAP.

� Personality and Social Psychology Review. Publishedby the Society for Personality and Social Psychology,Inc. Available online via Business Source Premier.

� Personality and Individual Differences. Published byPergamon Press. Available online via Science Direct.

Web links

� Goldberg’s International Personality Item Pool (IPIP).A scientific computer-supported system for the develop-ment of advanced measures of personality and otherindividual differences. The IPIP website is intended toprovide rapid access to measures of individual differ-ences, all in the public domain, to be developed conjointlyamong scientists worldwide (http://ipip.ori.org/ipip/).

� A good website outlining many of the personalitytheories covered in this chapter of the book is athttp://www.personalityresearch.org/.

Film and literature

� Abigail’s Party (1970, BBC Play for Today). If you wantto see a film example of the contrast between an ex-traverted individual and an introverted individual, thenthe BBC Television film of the play Abigail’s Party is agreat example. The contrasts between the two main fe-male characters typify these two personality traits. Thisis perhaps a little dated, but well worth viewing if youcan get a copy or get the opportunity to see the play.Other films, that may be more easier to get hold of, thatdepict stories around introverted individuals include

Amélie (2001, directed by Jean-Pierre Jeunet) andEdward Scissorhands (1990, directed by Tim Burton).

� Cruel Intentions (1999, directed by Roger Kumble),Dangerous Liaisons (1988, directed by Stephen Frears)and Collateral (2004, directed by Michael Mann). Inthis chapter we outlined the concept of psychoticism, apersonality that emphasises hostile, cruel and inhumanetraits with a strong need to ridicule and upset others.All these films have lead characters who clearly showthese traits.

Explore the website accompanying this text at www.pearsoned.co.uk/maltby for further resources to helpyou with your studies. These include multiple-choice questions, essay questions, weblinks and ideas foradvanced reading.

M07_MALT2908_02_SE_C07.QXD 10/21/09 12:45 PM Page 177