4106221.pdf

download 4106221.pdf

of 22

Transcript of 4106221.pdf

  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    1/22

    The Relative Importance of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Rewards as Determinants of WorkSatisfactionAuthor(s): Clifford J. MottazSource: The Sociological Quarterly, Vol. 26, No. 3, Special Feature: The Sociology of NuclearThreat (Autumn, 1985), pp. 365-385

    Published by: Wileyon behalf of the Midwest Sociological SocietyStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/4106221.

    Accessed: 18/06/2013 05:26

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at.http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

    .JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of

    content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms

    of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    .

    WileyandMidwest Sociological Societyare collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access

    to The Sociological Quarterly.

    http://www.jstor.org

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=blackhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=msshttp://www.jstor.org/stable/4106221?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/4106221?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=msshttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=black
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    2/22

    THERELATIVEMPORTANCEF INTRINSIC

    AND

    EXTRINSIC

    EWARDS S

    DETERMINANTS

    F WORK

    SATISFACTION*

    Clifford

    J.

    Mottaz

    University

    of Wisconsin-River

    alls

    This

    study nvestigates

    he

    nature nd

    sources

    f

    overall

    work

    satisfaction

    n

    several

    occupational

    roups.

    The effects

    of three

    ypes

    of workrewards

    n

    work

    satisfaction

    are assessed.

    They

    are:

    ntrinsic

    ask

    rewards,

    xtrinsic ocial

    rewards

    ndextrinsic

    organizational

    ewards.Data

    rom

    1,385

    workers

    epresenting

    ive

    occupational

    roups

    suggest

    that intrinsic

    ewards ollowed

    by

    extrinsic ocial

    rewards,

    are

    powerful

    determinantsf

    satisfaction

    cross

    all

    occupational

    roups.

    Extrinsic

    rganizational

    rewards

    ppear

    o

    emerge

    s an

    mportant

    eterminant

    nly

    n

    lower-level

    ccupations.

    The

    mplications

    f

    these

    indings

    or

    ob

    redesign rograms

    re

    subsequently

    iscussed.

    INTRODUCTION

    The

    concept

    of

    work

    satisfaction

    has

    occupied

    a

    prominent

    place

    in the social

    science

    literature

    or

    many years.

    A

    vast

    amount

    of researchhas

    been conducted

    on this

    subject,

    and

    related work

    attitudes,

    by

    both

    psychologists

    and

    sociologists.

    In

    fact,

    Locke

    (1976)

    reports

    that over

    3,300

    articles have been

    compiled

    on this

    topic

    to

    date,

    and the

    number

    continues

    to

    grow.

    One of

    the main

    reasons for the

    tremendous nterest

    n this

    area is the

    belief

    that

    work

    satisfaction

    affects worker

    productivity,

    absenteeism,

    turnover,

    and hence

    organizational

    effectiveness.

    Anotherreason

    for

    the

    popularity

    of

    the

    concept

    stems from

    the

    notion

    that work

    satisfaction

    may

    have

    serious

    consequences

    for

    the

    well-being

    of

    the individual n terms of physicalandmentalhealth,and satisfactionwith life

    in

    general.

    Despite

    years

    of

    study

    and the

    current

    popularity

    of

    the

    term,

    there

    is no

    single, agreed

    upon

    definitionof work

    satisfaction.How the

    concept

    is defined

    depends

    on the

    theoretical

    *

    Direct all

    communications

    o:

    Professor

    Clifford

    J.

    Mottaz,

    Department

    f

    Sociology,

    University

    of Wisconsin-

    River

    Falls,

    River

    Falls,

    WI

    54022.

    The

    Sociological

    Quarterly,

    Volume

    26,

    Number

    3,

    pages

    365-385.

    Copyright

    ?

    1985

    by

    JAI

    Press,

    Inc.

    All

    rights

    of

    reproduction

    n

    any

    form

    reserved.

    ISSN: 0038-0253

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    3/22

    366

    THE

    SOCIOLOGICAL

    UARTERLY

    ol. 26/No.

    3/1985

    slant

    of

    the

    writer.

    One theoretical

    approach

    hat has

    shown some

    degree

    of

    promise

    in

    currentresearch s the social

    psychological,

    or interactionist,model of worksatisfaction.

    From this

    perspective,

    the

    concept

    is

    commonly

    defined

    as

    a

    positive

    orientation

    oward

    work based

    upon

    a

    congruency

    between the

    worker's

    perception

    of the work

    situation

    (along

    a

    variety

    of

    work

    dimensions)

    and his/her work

    values

    regarding

    those same

    dimensions

    (Smith,

    Kendall,

    and

    Hulin, 1969; Locke,

    1969;

    Kalleberg,

    1977;

    Katzell,

    1979).

    This

    view of work

    satisfaction

    suggests

    that

    two

    types

    of

    factors

    are

    operative:

    (1)

    perceived

    work

    characteristics,

    which

    represent

    work

    rewards;

    and

    (2)

    work

    values,

    which

    represent

    he

    importance

    hat

    ndividuals ttach

    o

    these

    perceived

    work

    characteristics.

    In this

    sense,

    work

    satisfaction

    represents

    a

    person-environment

    fit.

    It

    is

    a

    function

    of

    the interaction

    between

    the

    worker's

    values

    and various

    aspects

    of

    the

    workplace.

    The

    advantage

    f theinteractionist

    erspective

    s that t directsattentiono bothworkcharacteristics

    (rewards)

    and

    individual

    differences in

    the

    analysis

    of

    work

    satisfaction,

    rather

    than

    emphasizing

    one at

    the

    expense

    of

    the other.

    WORK

    REWARDS

    The area of

    work

    consists of

    numerous

    nterrelated actors.

    Therefore,

    efforts

    to

    improve

    the

    quality

    of

    life in the

    workplace

    require

    a

    thorough

    understanding

    of

    how

    these

    dimensions

    relate to work

    satisfaction.

    Organizational

    esearchers

    have devoted

    a

    great

    deal of

    attention

    toward this end.

    As a

    result,

    several

    important

    correlates

    of

    work

    satisfactionhave been

    clearly

    identified. Some of these factors include the task itself,

    salary,

    promotions,

    style

    of

    supervision,

    cohesiveness

    of

    work

    groups,

    security,

    fringe

    benefits,

    and

    working

    conditions

    (Fournet,

    Distefano,

    and

    Pryer,

    1966;

    Locke, 1976;

    Gruenberg,

    1979).

    These

    factors

    constitute

    potential

    sources

    of rewardsto the

    worker.

    In

    a recent

    article,

    Katz and Van

    Maanen

    1977)

    argue

    that the various

    aspects

    of

    work

    form

    three

    conceptually

    and

    empirically

    distinct

    clusters

    or

    dimensions

    of

    work

    rewards,

    which

    they

    call the

    loci of

    work satisfaction.

    These

    clusters

    include

    task,

    social,

    and

    organizational

    ewards.This

    classification

    oughly

    corresponds

    o the distinction

    ommonly

    made between ntrinsic

    nd

    extrinsic ewards

    Herzberg,

    1966;

    Wernimont, 966;

    Gruneberg,

    1979).

    The task dimension refers to those intrinsicrewards

    directly

    associated with

    doing

    the

    job. They

    are derived

    from the content of

    the task itself

    and include such

    factors

    as

    interesting

    and

    challenging

    work,

    self-direction and

    responsibility,

    variety,

    creativity,

    opportunities

    o

    use one's

    skills and

    abilities,

    and

    sufficient

    feedback

    regarding

    the

    ef-

    fectiveness

    of

    one's

    efforts. The social

    dimension

    refers to

    those extrinsicrewards

    derived

    from

    interacting

    with

    others on the

    job.

    They

    are

    based

    on

    the

    quality

    of

    interpersonal

    relationships

    and

    include such

    factors

    as

    friendly,

    helpful,

    and

    supportive

    co-workers

    and

    supervisors.

    The

    organizational

    dimension refers to

    those

    extrinsic

    rewards

    provided

    by

    the

    organization

    or

    the

    purpose

    of

    facilitating

    or

    motivating

    task

    performance.

    They

    are

    tangible

    rewards

    that are visible to others and include such

    factors

    as

    pay,

    promotions,

    fringe

    benefits,

    security,

    and the like. These factors have been

    traditionally

    referredto

    as

    instrumentalewards. n

    short,

    work rewardsareviewedas

    resulting

    romthe interaction

    of

    the individualwith the task

    itself,

    fellow

    workers,

    andthe

    organization.

    Katz and

    Van

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    4/22

    The

    Relative

    mportance

    f

    Intrinsic

    nd

    Extrinsic

    ewards

    367

    Maanen found that

    each

    reward

    type

    was related

    to

    work satisfaction

    to some

    degree.

    They concludedthat attentionmust be

    paid

    to all threetypes of rewardsfor a thorough

    analysis

    of work

    satisfaction.

    Focusing

    on

    the

    task

    itself,

    Hackman

    and

    others

    (Hackman

    and

    Lawler,

    1971;

    Hackman

    and

    Oldham,

    1975,

    1979)

    have

    identified

    ive

    conceptually

    ndependent

    ask characteristics

    which,

    they argue,

    can be

    applied

    to

    any job.

    These

    characteristics

    re

    skill

    variety,

    task

    identity,

    task

    significance,

    autonomy,

    and

    feedback.

    The

    results

    of their research

    ndicate

    that

    these

    task rewards

    are

    strongly

    related to work

    attitudes.

    Previous

    and

    subsequent

    studies

    tend

    to

    support

    heir

    findings

    (Blauner,

    1964;

    Herzberg,

    1966;

    Kirsch

    and

    Lenger-

    mann,

    1971;

    Pierce

    and

    Dunham, 1976;

    Sims and

    Szilagyi,

    1976;

    Mottaz,

    1981,

    1982).'

    WORKVALUES

    Work

    values refers to

    the relative

    importance

    assigned

    to

    the various

    aspects

    of work

    by

    the

    individual.

    Recently,

    researchers

    have come to

    recognize

    that

    individuals

    may

    differ

    considerably

    in

    what

    they

    are

    looking

    for

    in

    work.

    Some

    assign greater

    importance

    to

    pay

    while

    others are more

    concerned with

    interesting

    and

    challenging

    jobs.

    Interest

    in

    individual

    differences with

    regard

    to

    work

    values

    has led

    some researchers

    o

    introduce

    background

    factors

    into

    studies of

    work

    satisfaction.

    Some

    of

    these

    factors

    are

    sex

    (Golembiewski, 1977; Weaver,

    1977),

    age

    (Glenn,

    Taylor,

    and

    Weaver,

    1977),

    education

    (King, Murry,

    and

    Atkinson, 1982;

    Gruenberg,

    1980;

    Glenn and

    Weaver, 1982a);

    tenure

    (Katz,

    1978),

    maritalstatus

    (Seybolt

    and

    Gruenfeld, 1976;

    King

    et al. 1982), urban-rural

    background

    Turner

    nd

    Lawerence,1965;

    Seybolt

    and

    Gruenfeld,

    1976),

    andrace

    (Weaver,

    1978).

    Unfortunately,

    most

    studies have shown that

    background

    actors account

    for

    very

    little

    variance

    in

    work satisfaction

    (Campbell,

    Converse,

    and

    Rogers,

    1976;

    Weaver,

    1977;

    King

    et

    al.,

    1982).

    Nevertheless,

    several

    of

    the

    studies

    cited above

    suggest

    that

    background

    actors

    may

    moderatethe

    relationship

    between work

    rewards

    and

    work

    sat-

    isfaction,

    and

    therefore

    must be

    included

    in the

    analysis

    as control

    variables.

    Considerable

    attention

    has

    been focused

    on

    the

    relationship

    between

    work values

    and

    occupational

    evel.

    However,

    the

    findings

    n

    this area

    are

    not

    entirely

    consistent.

    A

    number

    of

    surveys

    on work

    attitudes have found

    that

    workers

    in

    both

    upper

    and lower-level

    occupations

    tend to

    report

    relatively high

    levels of work satisfaction.

    Considering

    the

    relatively

    unrewarding

    nature

    of

    tasks

    in

    lower-level

    occupations,

    researchers

    have

    been

    faced

    with the

    problem

    of

    accounting

    or

    these

    results.

    One

    of

    the most

    prevalent

    xplanations

    is that

    workers

    at

    different

    levels of the

    occupational

    hierarchy

    differ

    considerably

    in

    terms of

    work-related values and

    hence

    utilize

    qualitatively

    different criteria

    in their

    assessment of work

    (Morse

    and

    Weiss, 1955;

    Friedman

    and

    Havighurst,

    1962; Blauner,

    1964; Friedlander,

    1965;

    Goldthrope

    et

    al., 1969;

    Bacharch

    and

    Aiken,

    1979).

    More

    specifically,

    it is

    suggested

    that lower-level

    workers

    have

    an

    instrumental

    orientation

    toward work

    and

    are

    mainly

    interested

    in

    extrinsic

    rewards.

    Consequently,

    the

    major

    determinants f work

    satisfactionare

    pay, fringe

    benefits,

    promotions,

    andthe like.

    Work

    itself is

    simply

    a means to

    keep busy

    and

    earn

    a

    living.

    On the other

    hand,

    it is

    argued

    that

    higher

    level workers

    place greater

    mportance

    on

    intrinsicrewards

    and thusthe

    major

    determinantsof work satisfaction

    are characteristics

    of the

    task

    itself. Several

    studies

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    5/22

    368

    THE

    SOCIOLOGICAL

    UARTERLY

    ol.

    26/No.

    3/1985

    tend to

    support

    this

    argument

    Troxell,

    1954;

    Centers

    and

    Bugental,

    1966;

    Friedlander,

    1965, 1966; Seeman, 1967; Tausky, 1969; Ronan, 1970; Simonds and Orife, 1976).

    Other research

    on

    the

    topic

    indicates

    a

    decline

    in

    work

    satisfaction,

    especially

    among

    lower-level

    workers,

    and attribute his to

    changes

    in

    work-related

    values

    (Kirsch

    and

    Lengermann,

    1971;

    Sheppard

    and

    Herrick, 1972;

    Aronowitz,

    1973;

    U.S.

    Department

    of

    Health,

    Education,

    and Welfare

    1973;

    Strauss,

    1974;

    Yankelovich,

    Skelly,

    and

    White,

    1977; DeBoer,

    1978;

    Cooper

    et

    al., 1979;

    Quinn

    and

    Staines,

    1979;

    Glenn

    and

    Weaver,

    1982b).

    Specifically,

    it is

    suggested

    that

    intrinsic

    rewards

    have

    increased

    in

    importance

    for a

    large

    proportion

    of

    workers

    n

    lower-level

    occupations

    while extrinsic

    rewardshave

    become

    relatively

    less salient.

    The

    general

    mplication

    s that

    workers

    n

    both

    upper-level

    and

    lower-level

    occupations

    possess

    an

    intrinsic

    orientation

    oward

    work and

    hence

    the

    majordeterminantsof work satisfactionare task rewards.Supportfor the argumentcan

    be

    found in

    studies

    by

    Locke

    (1973),

    Cooper

    et

    al.,

    (1979)

    and

    Gruenberg

    1980).

    As

    a

    whole,

    the

    existing

    survey

    data

    in

    this area

    is

    inconsistent

    and

    confusing,

    to

    say

    the least.

    Some

    studies have found the

    work

    orientation

    and

    the determinants

    of

    work

    satisfaction to

    differ

    by

    occupational

    evel,

    while

    other studies

    have failed

    to show

    any

    meaningful

    occupational

    differenceswith

    regard

    o

    these two

    factors.

    Clearly,

    the research

    results do not

    permit any

    firm

    conclusions

    to

    be

    drawn. The

    specific

    objective

    of

    this

    study

    is to

    examine the work

    orientation

    values)

    and

    the determinants

    f work

    satisfaction

    among

    workers

    in

    upper-level

    and

    lower-level

    occupations,

    in order to

    hopefully

    shed

    some

    light

    on

    this

    issue.

    It

    should be

    noted,

    however,

    thatthe

    study

    is

    primarily

    oncerned

    with assessing the relative importanceof intrinsicand extrinsic rewardsas sources of

    work

    satisfaction.

    Work-related

    alues are

    analyzed

    only

    in

    terms

    of

    their

    relationship

    o

    this

    concern.

    METHODS

    Sample

    The

    data for this

    study

    were

    obtained from

    a

    large

    midwestern

    metropolitan

    area

    during

    1979 and

    1980. Six

    diverse

    organizations

    participated

    n

    the

    study.

    The

    organizations

    includeda

    university,

    five

    elementary

    schools from the same

    district,

    a

    plastics

    factory,

    an

    order-processing

    irm,

    a

    hospital,

    and

    a

    large

    law-enforcement

    agency.

    Within each

    organization,

    simple

    randomor

    stratified

    andom

    sampling

    procedures

    were used

    to ensure

    an

    adequate

    representation

    f

    workers from

    all

    major

    occupational

    groups

    that

    agreed

    to

    participate

    n

    the

    study.2

    The total

    sample

    consistedof

    1,385

    full-time

    employees

    representing

    a

    variety

    of

    occupations.

    The

    respondents

    were classified

    into five

    occupational

    categories:

    (a)

    professional,

    consisting

    of

    university

    aculty, registered

    nurses,

    and

    elementary

    school

    teachers

    (N

    =

    349); (b)

    managerial,

    ncluding

    police

    administrators,

    ducational

    admin-

    istrators,

    and

    factorysupervisors

    N

    =

    235); (c)

    clerical,

    consisting

    of secretaries

    (N

    =

    112);

    (d)

    service,

    consisting

    of

    police

    officers

    (N

    =

    440);

    and

    (e)

    blue-collar,

    consisting

    of

    factory

    workers

    N

    =

    249).

    Although

    this

    categorization

    ombinessome workersfrom

    different

    organiations,

    an earlier

    analysis

    of the data

    revealed

    negligible

    differences

    in

    work

    satisfaction or work

    characteristicsbetween workers of

    the same

    occupational

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    6/22

    The Relative

    mportance

    f Intrinsic nd

    Extrinsic

    ewards

    369

    category

    but

    different

    organizations.

    Thus,

    the

    occupationalcategories

    have been

    treated

    as homogeneous.

    The

    questionnaire

    administered o

    the

    employees

    in

    this

    sample

    included sections on

    demographic

    characteristics,

    overall

    work

    satisfaction,

    work

    rewards

    and work

    values.

    The overall

    response

    rate

    was

    74

    percent.

    Measures

    Work

    Satisfaction

    Overall

    work

    satisfaction s

    defined

    as

    the worker's

    affective

    response

    to the total

    work

    situation.

    Using

    a four

    point Likert-typeformat,

    this scale

    was

    constructed

    from three

    commonly

    used

    global

    items which were

    slightly

    revised

    for the

    present

    study.

    The

    items

    are as follows:

    Generally

    peaking,

    am

    satisfied

    with

    this

    ob.

    If I

    hadthe

    opportunity

    o start

    over

    again,

    I

    would

    choose he

    same

    ype

    of work

    I

    presently

    o.

    Taking

    nto

    consideration

    ll

    things

    about

    my

    job,

    I am

    very

    satisfied.

    The

    reliability

    of

    this

    scale was assessed

    by

    Cronbach's

    alpha

    which

    yielded

    a

    reliability

    coefficient

    of

    .772.

    Concerningvalidity,

    a factor

    analysis

    of

    the

    items,

    along

    with

    those

    of several other work

    dimensions,

    revealed that the items formeda

    clearly

    distinctfactor.

    Intrinsic

    Task

    Rewards

    Three

    intrinsic

    rewards associated

    with the task

    were selected

    for the

    analysis.

    They

    are

    task

    autonomy,

    task

    significance,

    and

    task involvement.

    Task

    autonomy

    refers

    to the

    degree

    of

    self-direction

    in

    task

    performance.

    Task

    significance

    refers

    to the

    degree

    to

    which the

    task is

    perceived

    as a

    significant

    contribution

    to

    the

    work

    process.

    Task

    involvement

    refers

    to the

    degree

    to

    which the

    task

    is

    considered

    nteresting

    and

    rewarding

    in

    itself.

    The

    scales used to

    measure

    these

    three

    factors

    were constructed

    by

    the

    author

    andreported n a previousarticle(Mottaz, 1981). Eachscale consists of seven four-point

    Likert-type

    items.

    Cronbach's

    alpha produced

    a

    reliability

    coefficient

    of .917

    for the

    autonomy

    scale,

    .790

    for the task

    significance

    scale,

    and .875

    for the task

    involvement

    scale.

    Validity

    was evaluated

    through

    factor

    analysis

    which

    yielded

    distinct factors.

    Extrinsic

    ocial

    and

    Organizational

    Rewards

    Six

    extrinsic

    rewards

    associated

    with

    the work context

    were

    included in the

    analysis.

    The items

    used to

    measure these factors

    were drawn

    from several

    widely

    used

    scales

    reported

    in

    Robinson, Athanasiou,

    and Head

    (1969)

    and

    slightly

    modified for

    present

    purposes.

    Each scale consists of four to

    six

    four-point

    Likert-type

    tems. Social

    rewards

    include two

    factors:

    (1)

    supervisory

    assistance-the

    degree

    to

    which

    supervisors

    are

    perceived

    as

    supportive

    and

    helpful

    in

    job

    matters;

    and

    (2)

    colleague

    assistance-the

    degree

    to which

    colleagues

    are

    perceived

    as

    supportive

    and

    helpful.

    Cronbach's

    alpha

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    7/22

    370 THE

    SOCIOLOGICAL

    UARTERLY

    ol.

    26/No.

    3/1985

    yieldedreliability

    oefficients

    of

    .822 and

    .821 for

    the

    two

    scales

    respectively.

    Organizational

    rewards ncludefour factors:(1) adequateworkingconditions the extentto which there

    are

    adequate

    resources,

    supplies,

    equipment,

    time,

    and

    the

    like to do a

    good

    job;

    (2)

    pay

    equity-the

    extent to which

    workersfeel

    their

    salary

    is

    comparable

    o

    others

    performing

    the

    same or similar

    obs;

    (3)

    promotional

    pportunity-the

    extent

    to which the

    job

    provides

    opportunity

    or

    advancement;

    and

    (4)

    adequate ringe

    benefits-the

    degree

    to

    which

    the

    worker eels

    the

    pension

    plan,

    medical

    coverage,

    and the

    like are sufficient.

    The

    reliability

    coefficients

    reported

    for the four

    scales

    are

    .712,

    .832, .815,

    and .732

    respectively.

    Validity

    was

    assessed

    through

    factors

    analysis

    which,

    again, yielded

    distinct

    factors.

    It should be

    noted that

    these measures of

    work rewards

    do

    not

    represent

    objective

    properties

    of the

    workplace,

    but

    rather

    he individuals

    perception

    of

    them.

    As Hackman

    and Lawler (1971) have pointedout, it is not the objective state of these characteristics

    that

    affects

    work

    attitudes,

    but

    how

    they

    are

    experienced

    by

    the

    worker.

    Moreover,

    measuresof

    perceived

    rewards

    represent

    evaluative

    udgments

    on

    the

    part

    of the workers

    (e.g.,

    the

    pay

    is

    good,

    the work is

    interesting),

    and hence

    can be viewed as

    measuresof

    facet

    satisfaction.

    Work

    Values

    Respondents

    were asked

    to

    rate each of

    the nine work rewards

    discussed

    above

    in

    terms

    of how

    important

    t

    was to

    them.

    Responses ranged

    from

    1

    to

    10. The

    higher

    the

    score, the greaterthe importance.

    Demographic

    Characteristics

    Five

    demographic

    factors

    possibly

    related to work satisfaction

    were included

    in

    the

    research.

    These factors

    included

    sex,

    age,

    educational

    evel,

    family

    income,

    and

    length

    of

    time

    on

    the

    job.

    PROCEDURE

    Earlier

    n

    the

    article,

    t

    was

    suggested

    hat

    the

    variouswork

    rewards orm three

    conceptually

    distinct clusters. Since the presentresearch is based on this conceptualdistinctionit is

    necessary

    o

    demonstrate

    he

    empirical

    ndependence

    of the

    three

    clusters.

    For his

    purpose,

    the

    nine

    work

    rewards

    were

    subjected

    to

    factor

    analysis employing principle

    component

    solution

    and varimax

    rotation.

    The results

    appear

    n

    Table

    1.

    The

    data

    in

    Table

    1

    show

    that the

    factor

    analysis

    produced

    hree factors

    corresponding

    to the

    expectations

    discussed

    above.3 Intrinsic ask rewards

    oad

    significantly

    high

    on

    the

    first

    factor,

    extrinsic

    organizational

    rewards on

    the second

    factor,

    and

    extrinsic

    social

    rewards

    on the third.

    Thus,

    the results

    strongly

    suggest

    that the three clusters

    of work

    rewards are

    empirically

    independent,

    each

    tapping

    a

    relatively

    distinct

    area

    of work.

    On the

    basis

    of

    these

    findings,

    three

    scales labeled

    intrinsictask

    rewards,

    extrinsic

    organizationalewards,andextrinsicsocial rewardswerecreated or use in the forthcoming

    analysis.

    This was achieved

    by

    summing

    each

    respondent's

    scores on each individual

    factor

    within a

    given

    cluster and

    dividing

    by

    the number

    of

    factors

    in

    that cluster.

    While

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    8/22

    The

    Relative

    mportance

    f

    Intrinsic

    nd Extrinsic

    ewards

    371

    Table

    1

    Results of a Factor

    Analysis

    of the Nine Work

    Rewards

    Used

    in the

    Study

    Factors

    Work

    Characteristics I

    II

    III

    Task

    Autonomy

    .883*

    .022 .079

    Task

    Significance

    .643

    .082

    .293

    Task

    Involvement

    .840

    .093

    .256

    Working

    Conditions

    -.138

    .600

    .279

    Pay

    .157

    .862

    -

    .017

    Promotions

    .256

    .632

    .205

    FringeBenefits -.066 .734 .096

    Co-workers

    .089

    .049 .796

    Supervisors

    .084 .292 .739

    Note:

    *

    Loadings

    f

    .40 or

    greater

    reconsidered

    ignificant.

    N

    =

    1,385)

    this

    procedure

    may

    mask

    the

    contributionsof each

    individual

    factor,

    the

    objective

    of this

    study

    is

    to

    assess

    the

    relative

    impact

    of

    intrinsic and extrinsic rewards

    as clusters or

    sets

    on

    overall work

    satisfaction.

    The

    intercorrelations

    mong

    the nine work

    value

    items

    were

    then

    subjected

    to

    factor

    analysisemployingprinciplecomponentsolution and varimaxrotation.The datain Table

    2 show

    that

    the

    factor

    analysis

    produced

    three

    values

    factors

    that

    correspond

    with the

    three

    types

    of

    work

    rewards

    previously

    discussed.

    Therefore,

    three

    scales

    labeled intrinsic

    task

    values,

    extrinsic

    organizational

    alues,

    and extrinsic

    social values were

    formed.

    This

    was

    accomplished

    in the

    same

    manner as the reward

    scales.

    Since the

    forthcoming

    analysis

    is

    heavily

    dependent

    upon

    the

    use

    of

    multiple

    regression

    techniques,

    zero-order

    correlation

    coefficients were

    calculated

    among

    the

    potential

    in-

    Table

    2

    Results

    of a

    Factor

    Analysis

    of the Nine Work-

    Related

    Values

    Used

    in

    the

    Study

    Factors

    Work-RelatedValues

    I

    II

    III

    Task

    Autonomy

    .608* .149 .192

    Task

    Significance

    .692

    .085 .156

    Task

    Involvement

    .776

    .127

    .134

    Working

    Conditions

    .027

    .608 .181

    Pay

    .161

    .778

    .221

    Promotions

    .124

    .698 .188

    Fringe

    Benefits

    -

    .099

    .761 .042

    Co-workers .010 .072 .701

    Supervisors

    .146 .102

    .734

    Note:

    *

    Loadings

    f .40 or

    greater

    reconsidered

    ignificant.

    N

    =

    1,385)

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    9/22

    372

    THE

    SOCIOLOGICAL

    UARTERLY

    ol.

    26/No.

    3/1985

    dependent

    variables in

    order to test for

    multicollinerarity.Any

    large

    correlations

    among

    these variables would tend to make the results unreliable. The results revealed that

    multicollinearity

    was

    a serious

    problem

    n two

    cases.

    First,

    age

    and

    years

    employed

    were

    highly

    correlated

    r

    =

    .73).

    Age

    was selected

    for the

    analysis

    because

    previous

    research

    has

    found this

    variable to be

    important

    n

    studies

    of

    work satisfaction

    (Glenn,

    Taylor,

    and

    Weaver,

    1977).

    Second,

    as

    might

    be

    expected,

    education

    and

    occupational

    evel

    were

    highly

    correlated

    r

    =

    .71).

    In

    orderto deal

    with

    this

    problem,

    the

    forthcoming

    analysis

    was

    conducted for each

    occupational

    evel

    separately.

    This

    procedure

    would

    seem to

    be

    justified

    since the

    purpose

    of

    the

    study

    is

    to

    assess the

    work orientationand

    sources

    of

    work

    satisfaction

    in

    different

    occupational

    groups.

    Finally,

    some caution

    is warranted n

    interpreting

    he

    research

    results

    in

    the

    sections

    to follow.

    Survey

    data based on

    self-reports

    always

    involves some risk of

    response

    bias

    or

    social

    desirability

    bias

    (Phillips,

    1973;

    Orpen,

    1976).

    This

    refers

    to the

    tendency

    for

    people

    to

    agree

    with

    statements that are

    socially

    desirable

    or

    disagree

    with

    statements

    that

    are

    socially

    undesirable.

    For

    example,

    some

    workers

    may

    be reluctant

    to

    say

    they

    are

    dissatisfied with work

    or that their

    job

    is not

    very

    interesting.

    Response

    bias

    affecting

    two or

    more variables

    n

    the

    same

    way

    will

    tend

    to inflate

    the

    relationship

    between

    these

    variables and hence

    distort the results.

    The

    possibility

    of

    a

    differential

    response

    bias across

    occupational

    evels

    may

    exist with

    regard

    to some

    key

    research variables. For

    example,

    it

    may

    be

    that workers

    in

    upper

    level

    occupations

    are more

    inclined,

    than

    workers

    in

    lower

    level

    occupations,

    to

    report

    that

    they

    are more

    intrinsically

    rewarded or more satisfied with work than

    they

    really

    are,

    simply

    because

    they

    feel

    they

    should be.

    This

    response

    bias

    would tend

    to

    produce

    spurious

    relationshipsamong

    the

    variables

    nvolved.

    In

    the

    present

    study

    efforts

    have

    been

    made

    to

    minimize

    response

    bias

    by:

    (1)

    assuring

    anonymity;

    2)

    stressing

    hatthereare no

    right

    or

    wrong

    answers;

    3)

    the use

    of

    multiple

    item

    scales;

    (4)

    the

    use

    of

    direct and

    indirect

    statements;

    5)

    wording

    items

    in

    a

    simple

    and

    straightforward

    manner;

    and

    (6)

    random

    ordering

    of items.

    Moreover,

    the distribution

    of

    raw

    scores

    on

    the work

    satisfaction,

    reward,

    and

    value

    scales

    did not

    appear

    to be

    highly

    skewed

    in

    either a

    positive

    or

    negative

    direction.

    Nevertheless,

    one

    cannot

    be

    certain

    that the data

    is free

    of

    response

    bias.

    A

    potentially

    more serious

    methodological problem

    is

    sample

    selection bias. This

    refers

    to

    the

    systematic

    exclusion of

    a

    subset of

    the

    population

    (Berk,

    1983).

    According

    to

    Berk,

    a

    large

    bias

    may jeopardize

    both

    internaland

    external

    validity. Sample

    selection

    bias

    tends to occur when

    the

    population

    under

    study

    is

    not

    closed. Since the

    U.S.

    work

    force

    is a

    relatively

    open population

    with

    people

    continually

    moving

    in

    and

    out,

    sample

    selection bias

    characterizesmost research

    in

    the area of

    work. Glen

    (1981)

    argues

    that

    this is

    especially problematic

    n

    the

    study

    of

    work

    satisfaction.

    Some

    degree

    of

    sample

    selection

    bias

    may

    exist

    in

    the

    present

    analysis

    because all

    of

    the

    workers

    sampled

    are

    employed

    in a

    limited number

    of

    occupations,

    and

    the

    method

    of recruitmentnto

    (and

    exclusion

    from)

    those

    occupations

    may

    havecreated

    relationships

    among

    variables

    quite

    unlikethosefound n a different

    ample

    of

    occupations.

    Unfortunately,

    it

    is

    not

    possible

    to determine

    the size of this bias. This

    imposes

    limitations on what

    one

    can

    infer from

    the

    data. One

    way

    of

    reducing

    this

    problem

    would be to

    perform

    the

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    10/22

    The Relative

    mportance

    f Intrinsic

    nd

    Extrinsic

    ewards

    373

    analysis

    on

    a

    national

    sample

    of

    workers.

    However,

    even

    this

    procedure

    would

    not

    completely

    eliminate

    sample

    selection bias. In

    sum,

    given

    the

    possibility

    of some

    degree

    of both

    response

    bias

    and

    sample

    selection bias

    the

    forthcoming

    results

    must be

    viewed

    with

    caution

    and

    any

    conclusions

    must be considered

    tentative.

    RESULTS

    Overall Work

    Satisfaction

    Table 3

    presents

    the

    mean

    scores,

    standard

    deviations,

    and

    one-way

    analysis

    of

    variance

    results for

    overall

    work satisfaction

    by

    occupational group.

    As

    might

    be

    expected,

    the

    findings

    show thatwork satisfaction ncreases

    significantly

    romblue-collar o

    professional

    occupations.

    Additionally,

    the

    workers in

    the

    sample report

    can

    best

    be

    described as

    moderate to

    modestly

    high

    levels

    of

    work

    satisfaction.

    Work-RelatedValues

    Using

    multiple

    regression

    techniques,

    an

    exploratoryanalysis

    was conducted

    in

    order

    to

    assess the effects

    of the various

    background

    actors and

    occupational

    evel on the

    three

    types

    of

    work values

    (data

    not

    shown).

    The results indicated

    that

    occupational

    evel

    was

    by

    far the

    most

    powerful

    predictor

    of all three

    types

    of

    work-related

    values.4

    Table 4

    presentsthe meanscores andone-way analysisof varianceresultsforwork-relatedvalues

    both

    between and

    within

    occupational

    groups.

    An

    inspection

    of

    Table

    4

    reveals

    that

    a

    very

    similar

    hierarchy

    of work-relatedvalues

    emerges

    within each

    occupational

    group.

    Specifically,

    the

    findings

    suggest

    that workers

    in

    both

    upper-level

    and

    lower-level

    occupations

    assign

    the

    greatest

    mportance

    o

    intrinsic

    task

    rewards,

    followed

    by

    extrinsic

    social and

    organizational

    rewards,

    respectively.

    At

    the

    same

    time,

    the

    data indicate

    that some

    importantoccupational

    differences

    exist.

    It

    appears

    hat workers

    n

    upper-level

    occupations

    end to

    assign

    significantlygreaterweight

    to

    intrinsic

    task

    rewards

    than

    their

    counterparts

    n

    lower-level

    occupations.

    In

    contrast,

    the

    latter

    end

    to

    place

    significantly

    greater mportance

    on

    extrinsic

    organizational

    ewards

    thanthe former. No distinguishablepatternemerges for the valuationof social rewards

    by

    occupational

    group.

    Table 3

    Means,

    Standard

    Deviations,

    and

    Analysis

    of VarianceResults

    For Work

    Satisfaction

    by

    OccupationalGroup

    Occupational

    Group

    Professional

    Managerial

    Clerical Services Blue-Collar

    F

    P

    Mean

    3.18

    3.06 2.99

    2.87

    2.58

    46.66

    .000

    SD .47 .54 .54 .53 .58

    N

    349

    235

    112 440 249

    The

    possible

    ange

    or mean

    cores s between

    and4.

    The

    higher

    he

    score

    he

    higher

    he

    work atisfaction.

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    11/22

    Table

    4

    Mean

    Scores

    and

    Analysis

    of

    Variance Results

    for

    Work-

    Values

    Both

    Within and

    Between

    Occupational

    Grou

    Professional Managerial

    Clerical

    Service

    Work-RelatedValues

    (N

    =

    349)

    (N

    =

    235) (N

    =

    112) (N

    =

    440)

    Intrinsic

    Rewards 7.76* 6.87 6.57 6.22

    Extrinsic

    Organizational

    Rewards 3.82 4.01

    4.17

    4.47

    Extrinsic Social

    Rewards 5.39 5.49

    5.80

    5.35

    F 140.26 43.30 33.23

    72.23

    P

    .000 .000 .000

    .000

    The

    possible ange

    or

    mean cores

    s

    between

    1

    and 10.

    The

    higher

    he

    score,

    he

    greater

    he

    importance.

    w

    -ph

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    12/22

  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    13/22

    Table 5

    Multiple

    Regression

    of

    Background

    and Work

    Rewards

    on

    Work Satisfactio

    Occupational

    Group

    Professional Managerial

    Clerical

    (N

    =

    349) (N

    =

    235) (N

    =

    112)

    Characteristics B b

    B b

    B b

    Sex .013

    .012 .022

    .035

    - -

    Age

    .045

    .002

    .076

    .005 .075 .004

    Education -.110

    -

    .061 .043

    .032

    -

    .003

    -

    .002

    Income

    .040

    .014 .041

    .019 -.059

    -

    .027

    Intrinsic Rewards

    .505

    .610*

    .568

    .679*

    .489

    .619*

    Extrinsic

    Organizational

    Rewards .004 .004

    .053

    .045 .144

    .176*

    Extrinsic

    Social

    Rewards

    .375

    .381

    .266

    .356* .344

    .421

    R2

    =

    .610 R2

    =

    .631 R2

    =

    .691

    F =

    60.19*

    F

    =

    55.40*

    F

    =

    39.09*

    Note:

    *

    Significant

    t the .05

    level

    or

    better.

    twa

    u,

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    14/22

    Table

    6

    Mean

    Scores

    and

    Analysis

    of

    Variance Results For

    Work

    Sa

    and

    Availability

    of

    Work

    Rewards

    by

    OccupationalGr

    Occupational Group

    Professional Managerial

    Clerical

    Service

    (N

    =

    349)

    (N

    =

    235) (N

    =

    112) (N

    =

    440)

    Work Satisfaction

    3.18 3.06

    2.99

    2.87

    Intrinsic Rewards

    3.21 3.00

    2.93 2.86

    Extrinsic

    Rewards-Organization

    2.32

    2.35

    2.46 2.22

    Extrinsic

    Rewards-Social

    3.06 2.87

    3.07

    2.76

    Note:

    The

    possible ange

    or

    mean cores s between

    1

    and

    4. The

    higher

    he

    score,

    he

    greater

    he

    valueof the variable.

    4

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    15/22

    378

    THE

    SOCIOLOGICAL

    UARTERLY

    ol. 26/No.

    3/1985

    Extrinsic social

    rewards

    also

    emerge

    as a

    fairly

    powerful

    predictor.

    The data

    in Table

    5

    suggest

    thatthis factorhas a

    significant

    and

    positive

    impact

    on work satisfaction n all

    occupational

    groups.

    This

    finding

    should

    not

    be

    too

    surprising.

    After

    all,

    it seems self-

    evident

    that

    friendly

    and

    supportive

    relationships

    with co-workers

    and

    supervisors

    would

    be an

    important

    source

    of

    satisfaction in

    any

    type

    of

    work

    setting.

    Finally,

    the

    results

    reported

    n

    Table 5

    suggest

    that

    extrinsic

    organizational

    rewards

    have a

    significant

    and

    positive

    effect,

    but

    only

    in lower-level

    occupations.

    It

    appears

    hat

    clerical,

    service,

    and

    blue-collar

    workers are more sensitive

    to

    pay,

    fringe

    benefits,

    promotions,

    and

    working

    conditions n

    their

    overall

    assessment

    of work than

    professional

    or

    managerial

    workers.

    This

    finding

    ends

    to

    support

    he

    position

    hatextrinsic

    organizational

    rewards are

    a

    stronger

    predictor

    of

    work

    satisfaction in

    lower-level than

    in

    upper-level

    occupations.

    It is

    important

    to note,

    however,

    that in each of the three lower-level

    occupations, organizational

    ewards

    account for a

    considerably

    smaller

    proportion

    of

    the

    variance

    in

    overall

    work

    satisfaction than

    either the intrinsic

    or social rewards.

    In

    sum,

    the results

    seem to

    suggest

    that

    ntrinsic

    ewards

    arethe

    most

    critical

    determinant

    of

    work

    satisfaction

    across all

    occupational

    groups,

    accounting

    for

    a

    greaterproportion

    of

    the

    explained

    variance than both

    extrinsic

    rewards

    combined.

    Regarding

    extrinsic

    rewards,

    social

    rewards

    appear

    to

    be a

    fairly strong

    predictor

    in

    each

    group,

    while

    organizational

    rewards seem to

    have

    a

    much

    smaller effect

    and

    emerge

    as

    a

    significant

    predictor

    in

    lower-level

    occupations exclusively.

    Thus,

    there

    appears

    to be a

    strong

    correspondence

    between work-related

    values and the determinants

    of work

    satisfaction.

    This

    finding

    provides

    some

    support

    for the interactionist

    argument

    that workersassess

    their

    jobs

    in

    terms of

    what

    they

    consider

    to be

    important

    n work.

    On

    the

    basis of

    the

    findingspresented

    above,

    one would

    expect

    occupations

    hat

    provide

    greater

    opportunities

    or

    intrinsic rewards

    to

    be characterized

    by

    higher

    levels

    of

    work

    satisfaction. The

    data in

    Table 6

    appear

    to

    support

    his notion.

    The

    results

    suggest

    that

    the

    availability

    of

    intrinsic

    rewards

    has a

    strong

    inear

    relationship

    with

    work

    satisfaction,

    while the

    availability

    of

    organizational

    and

    social rewards

    have a

    considerably

    weaker,

    nonlinear

    relationship.

    As

    might

    be

    expected,

    workers in

    higher-level

    occupations

    tend

    to

    have

    greater

    access to

    intrinsic

    rewards and

    report

    higher

    levels

    of work satisfaction

    than

    workers

    in

    lower-level

    occupations.

    INTERPRETATION

    F

    RESULTS

    The

    foregoing analysis

    indicatesthatthe

    determinants

    f work

    satisfactionare

    very

    similar

    across

    occupational

    groups.

    Nevertheless,

    some distinct

    differences exist.

    The

    critical

    difference is that

    extrinsic

    organizational

    rewards

    emerge

    as

    a

    significant predictor

    of

    work

    satisfaction in

    lower-level

    occupations

    but

    not in

    upper-level

    occupations.

    The

    rationale

    behind

    this

    finding

    s

    not

    entirely

    clear. One

    possible

    explanation

    s

    that

    t

    reflects

    a

    differential

    n

    opportunities

    or

    organizational

    ewardsbetween

    occupations.

    It

    may

    be

    argued

    hat

    lower-level

    workers,

    in contrast o

    higher-level

    workers,

    have lower

    incomes,

    less benefits, lower status, etc. and hence are less satisfiedwith this areaof work. As a

    result,

    lower-level workers are more

    responsive

    to increases

    in such factors as

    pay

    or

    promotions

    in their

    overall assessment of work.

    Support

    for

    this

    argument

    s not found

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    16/22

    The Relative

    mportance

    f

    Intrinsic

    nd Extrinsic

    ewards

    379

    in

    the

    present

    data.

    No

    significant

    difference was

    found between

    workers in

    higher

    and

    lower-level

    occupations

    in

    regard

    to satisfactionwith

    organizational

    rewards

    (F

    =

    .14,

    P

    =

    .712).

    Another

    possibility

    is that

    the

    importance

    of

    organizational

    rewards

    as a source

    of

    satisfaction

    among

    lower-level

    workers

    represents

    an accommodation

    on their

    part

    to

    an

    intrinsically

    mpoverished

    work

    situation

    Argyris,

    1957;

    Gruenberg,

    1980).

    Specifically,

    it

    may

    be that

    organizational

    ewards end to increase

    n

    salience

    among

    workers

    n lower-

    level

    occupations

    as a

    result of

    limited

    opportunities

    or

    intrinsically

    rewarding

    work.

    Unfortunately,

    the

    nature

    of

    the

    present

    research

    does

    not

    permit

    a definitive

    test of this

    argument.

    Nevertheless,

    it is

    possible

    to make a

    tentative assessment

    using

    the

    data

    at

    hand.

    Data

    pertinent

    o

    this

    argument

    are

    presented

    n Table 7.

    The results show that the

    availability

    of intrinsicrewardsis much

    greater

    in

    higher-

    level

    occupations

    han in

    lower-level

    occupations.

    Additionally,

    n

    higher-level

    occupations

    the

    importance

    assigned

    to intrinsic

    rewards,

    while

    initially

    fairly

    high,

    tends

    to increase

    among

    workers who

    have been at

    their

    jobs

    for

    longer periods

    of

    time.

    Organizational

    rewards are

    considerably

    less

    important

    for these

    same

    workers

    and

    no variation is

    observed with

    regard

    o

    job

    longevity.

    At the same

    time,

    a

    much

    different

    pattern

    merges

    for

    workers in

    lower-level

    occupations.

    The

    importance given

    to intrinsic

    rewards

    is

    moderately

    high

    initially

    but

    then decreases

    considerably

    among

    workers

    with

    greater

    ob

    longevity,

    while

    organizational

    ewardsshow a

    significant

    ncrease.

    This does

    not

    mean,

    however,

    that

    lower-level

    workers

    do not continue

    to value

    intrinsically

    rewarding

    work.

    On the

    contrary,

    the data in Table 7 show that the mean score for the

    importance

    of

    intrinsic

    rewards

    is

    greater

    than the mean score

    for the

    importance

    of

    organizational

    rewards

    irrespective

    of

    job longevity.

    These

    findings suggest

    that

    the

    importance

    of

    organizational

    rewards

    as

    a

    source

    of work satisfaction

    in

    lower-level

    occupations

    is

    a

    function of

    the

    availability

    of

    intrinsic

    rewards.

    Thus,

    the results

    provide

    conditional

    support

    for

    the

    accommodation

    hypothesis

    and further

    emphasize

    the

    importance

    of

    intrinsicrewards

    or all

    classes

    of workers.

    Nevertheless,

    additional

    esearch

    s

    necessary.

    It

    may

    be that

    longitudinal

    researchor cohort

    analysis

    can

    provide

    useful

    supplementary

    information

    regarding

    he

    validity

    of

    this

    hypothesis.

    It

    is

    possible

    that other

    demographic

    actors

    such as

    age

    or

    education,

    which tend

    to

    be related to both

    job

    tenure and work

    values,

    may

    accountfor these

    findings. Again

    it

    is not

    possible

    to

    make

    a definitive

    choice

    among

    these alternative

    xplanations,

    but

    some

    clues can be

    obtained

    from

    the data at hand. Partialcorrelation

    echniques

    were used to

    assess the

    relationship

    between

    job

    tenureand work values

    among

    workers

    n

    lower-level

    occupations.

    Controlling

    for

    the

    effects of

    age

    and

    education,

    the correlations

    between

    job

    tenure and

    intrinsic

    and

    extrinsic values were

    -

    .12 and .26

    respectively.

    Although

    these

    coefficients are

    quite

    modest,

    they

    are

    statistically significant,

    and are

    consistent

    with the results

    in

    Table

    7.

    Of

    course,

    these results

    are tentative

    to

    say

    the least.

    As a

    word of

    caution,

    the results of

    the

    present

    research should

    not be

    interpreted

    o

    mean that

    organizational

    rewards

    are of little or no concern

    to

    workers;rather,

    it

    seems

    self-evident that these factors are an essential

    aspect

    of work satisfaction.

    Few,

    if

    any,

    workers

    would

    continue

    to

    work

    on

    job

    for

    very long

    if

    organizational

    rewards were

    completely

    inadequate.

    Moreover,

    several researchershave indicatedthatdeficiencies

    in

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    17/22

    Table

    7

    Mean

    Scores and

    Analysis

    of

    Variance Results For the

    of

    Intrinsic Rewards

    and

    the

    Importance

    of Intrinsi

    Organizational

    Rewards

    By Occupation

    and Job

    Lon

    Job

    Longevity

    (years)

    Occupational

    Rewards 0-3 3-10 1

    Professional

    and

    Managerial

    Occupations

    Availability

    of Intrinsic

    Rewards

    3.06*

    3.10

    Importanceof Intrinsic Rewards 7.03** 7.31

    Importance

    of

    Organizational

    Rewards

    4.44**

    4.73

    (N

    =

    139) (N

    =

    153) (N

    Clerical, Service,

    and Blue-Collar

    Occupations

    Availability

    of Intrinsic Rewards 2.69

    2.75

    Importance

    of

    Intrinsic

    Rewards 6.44 6.24

    Importance

    of

    Organizational

    Rewards 5.04

    5.22

    (N

    =

    278) (N

    =

    302) (N

    Notes:

    *

    The

    possible

    ange

    or mean

    cores

    s

    between and

    4. The

    higher

    he score

    he

    greater

    he

    availabi

    **

    The

    possible

    ange

    or mean

    cores

    s

    between

    1

    and

    10. The

    higher

    he score he

    greater

    he

    import

    0

    (JJ

    C

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    18/22

    The

    Relative

    mportance

    f

    Intrinsic

    nd Extrinsic

    ewards

    381

    organizational

    ewardscan

    impede

    task

    performance

    nd

    hence reduce

    he

    level

    of

    intrinsic

    returns and work satisfaction(Locke, 1976; Oldham, 1976). The present findings and

    those

    of severalother

    recent studies

    seem to

    suggest

    thatall classes

    of workers

    are

    presently

    demanding something

    more

    from

    their

    work than the traditional

    rewards

    of

    good

    pay,

    safe and

    comfortable

    conditions,

    and

    opportunities

    or

    getting

    ahead.

    In

    addition

    o these

    traditional

    rewards

    they

    appear

    to

    be

    looking

    for

    autonomy,

    meaning,

    and

    challenge

    in

    work. This

    suggests

    that

    organizational

    rewards

    have an

    upper

    limit in

    terms

    of their

    effect

    on

    work

    satisfaction. At

    the

    same

    time,

    few

    would

    deny

    their basic

    importance.

    DISCUSSION

    Clearly, the most significantfindingof this studyis the strongrelationshipbetweenwork

    satisfactionand

    intrinsically

    ewarding

    work.

    Workers,

    rrespective

    of

    occupational

    group,

    tend

    to

    assign greater

    weight

    to

    intrinsic

    rewards

    than to either

    social or

    organizational

    rewards

    n

    their

    overallassessment f work.

    These

    findings

    would seem to be of

    considerable

    practical importance

    or

    those

    designing

    programs

    o

    increase

    worker

    satisfaction.

    In

    a

    general

    sense,

    strategies

    to

    improve

    the

    quality

    of

    life in the

    workplace

    have been

    based on one

    of

    two

    opposing

    perspectives

    of human nature

    and

    motivation,

    which

    McGregor

    (1960)

    calls

    Theory

    X

    and

    Theory

    Y.

    Theory

    X

    is

    based on the

    assumption

    that work is

    an

    unpleasantnecessity,

    and therefore

    people

    dislike

    to

    work and

    will avoid

    it

    if

    possible.

    Workersare

    motivated

    to

    do

    a

    good

    job by

    extrinsic rewards.

    Theory

    Y,

    in contrast,assumes thatpeople like to work, enjoy doing a good job, and aremotivated

    primarily

    by

    intrinsic rewards.

    In

    most

    cases,

    attempts

    at

    improving

    the

    workplace,

    by

    both

    management

    and labor

    unions,

    have

    been

    extrinsically

    rientated.

    Traditionally,

    hese

    efforts,

    especially

    as

    applied

    to

    lower-level

    occupations,

    have

    been based on

    the

    cynical

    view of

    work and the

    carrot-

    and-stick

    approach

    characteristicof

    Theory

    X. One

    strategy

    used

    to

    improve

    inherently

    unpleasant

    work

    has been to reduce

    it

    by

    having

    a shorter

    work

    week,

    more

    holidays

    and

    longer

    vacations.

    Another

    strategy

    has been to increase

    economic

    incentives

    by providing

    higher

    wages,

    overtime

    pay,

    pension

    plans,

    medical

    plans,

    and

    bonuses.

    Efforts

    have

    also

    been directed

    at

    improvingworking

    conditionswith

    respect

    to

    such

    features

    as

    safety,

    noise, and cleanliness. On the social side, humanrelationstrainingfor supervisorsand

    a host

    of

    employee-centeredprograms

    have been utilized.

    Unfortunately,

    the available

    data

    suggest

    that

    increases

    in

    extrinsic rewards

    such

    as

    these are

    not

    likely

    to

    raise

    the

    level of work

    satisfaction

    significantly.

    The

    results of

    the

    present study

    are much

    more in line with

    those who

    advocate

    job

    redesign

    as

    a

    means

    of

    enhancing

    work satisfaction.

    This

    strategy,

    congruent

    with

    Theory

    Y,

    emphasizes

    the

    potential

    richnessof the work

    experience

    and the

    importance

    f intrinsic

    rewards.Job

    redesign nvolving ob enlargement

    nd

    ob

    enrichment

    s aimedat

    constructing

    more

    meaningful,

    challenging,

    and

    interesting obs,

    and

    thereby

    increasing

    the

    level

    of

    intrinsicreturns. The

    weight

    of

    the researchhas indicated

    thatsuch

    programs

    have been

    generallysuccessful in terms of increasedwork satisfactionandalso productivity.Taking

    into

    account the

    findings

    of this

    and

    other recent studies

    regarding

    the

    importance

    of

    intrinsic

    rewards

    among

    workersand the

    problems

    associated

    with work

    dissatisfaction,

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    19/22

    382

    THE

    SOCIOLOGICAL

    UARTERLY

    ol. 26/No.

    3/1985

    job

    redesign

    programs

    warrant

    erious

    consideration

    by

    those

    concerned

    with

    improving

    the

    quality

    of life in the

    workplace.

    In

    conclusion,

    the

    findings

    of

    this

    study

    mustbe viewed as tentative.

    Additional

    research

    on a wider

    variety

    of

    occupations

    is

    required

    o

    conclusively

    demonstrate heir

    validity.

    Furthermore,

    since the data are

    based on

    verbal

    reports,

    one cannot be sure that the

    measured

    relationships

    are not

    somewhat

    inflated due to

    response

    bias,

    although

    efforts

    have been made

    to

    avoid this

    problem.

    Sample

    selection

    bias due to the

    local nature

    of

    the

    sample may

    also be a

    problem.

    However,

    this bias

    would

    appear

    to be

    relatively

    small as

    the

    findings

    of

    this

    study

    are

    very

    similar to other

    studies based

    on

    a national

    sample

    (Cooper

    et

    al.,

    1979;

    Gruenberg,

    1980).

    Finally,

    longitudinal

    data

    or

    cohort

    analysis

    may

    be

    a

    useful

    supplement

    to the

    existing

    cross-sectional data

    by providing

    additionalnformationor

    testing

    the accommodation

    ypothesis.

    Nevertheless, hisresearch

    has

    attempted

    o

    provide

    additional

    nsight

    into

    the natureand

    sources

    of

    work satisfaction

    and some

    suggestions

    for future

    research.

    NOTES

    1.

    For an

    excellent

    review

    of the effects of

    task rewardson

    work

    satisfaction,

    see

    Gruneberg

    1979).

    2.

    Of the

    various

    occupationalgroups

    represented

    n

    the

    six

    organizations,

    only

    hospital

    administrators

    nd

    physicians

    declined

    to

    participate

    n the

    study.

    3. A

    factor

    analysis

    of all

    the individual

    reward

    items

    produced

    ten

    factors,

    but

    only

    the

    first nine were

    interpreted.These factorscorresponded o the nine workrewarddimensionspreviouslydiscussed. Nine scales

    were

    then

    formed

    from

    the

    corresponding

    tems. The

    intercorrelations

    mong

    the nine

    work

    rewards scales

    were

    then

    factor-analyzed

    n

    an

    attempt

    to

    identify any higher-order

    dimensions,

    which

    produced

    the

    results

    in

    Table

    1.

    4. Of

    course,

    one

    should

    not conclude that

    occupation

    n

    itself is

    the

    only

    factor

    effecting

    work

    values,

    as

    all

    the

    variables

    relevant to

    this issue

    have not

    been

    controlled.

    5.

    When the

    same

    regression

    was

    calculated

    for

    the entire

    sample

    both

    age

    and education

    emerged

    as

    significantpredictors.

    The

    data

    suggested

    that

    work

    satisfaction

    s

    positively

    related

    to

    age

    but

    inversely

    related

    to

    education.

    Nevertheless,

    the

    four

    background

    characteristics

    accounted

    for

    less than four

    percent

    of the

    explained

    variance.

    REFERENCES

    Argyris,

    Chris

    1957

    Personality

    and

    Organization.

    New

    York:

    Harper

    Torchbooks.

    Aronowitz,

    Stanley

    1973

    False

    Promises: The

    Shaping

    of American

    Working

    Class

    Consciousness.

    New

    York:

    McGraw-Hill.

    Bacharach,

    Samuel B. and

    Michael

    Aiken

    1979 The

    impact

    of

    alienation,

    meaninglessness,

    nd

    meritocracy

    n

    supervisor

    nd subordinateatisfaction.

    Social

    Forces

    57:

    853-870.

    Berk,

    Richard

    A.

    1983 An

    introduction o

    sample

    selection bias

    in

    sociological

    data. American

    Sociological

    Review

    48:

    386-398.

    Blauner,Robert

    1964 Alienation

    and

    Freedom.

    Chicago:

    University

    of

    Chicago

    Press.

    Campbell,

    A.,

    P.

    E.

    Converse,

    and

    W.

    L.

    Rogers

    1976

    The

    Quality

    of

    American

    Life:

    Perceptions,

    Evaluations,

    and

    Satisfactions,

    New York:Russell

    Sage.

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    20/22

    The

    Relative

    mportance

    f

    Intrinsic

    nd Extrinsic ewards

    383

    Centers,

    R. and D. E.

    Bugental

    1966 Intrinsicand extrinsic motivationsamong differentaspects of the workingpopulation. Journalof

    Applied

    Psychology

    50:

    193-197.

    Cooper,

    M.

    R.,

    B.

    S.

    Morgan,

    P. M.

    Foley,

    and

    L.

    B.

    Kaplan

    1979

    Changing

    employee

    values:

    deepening

    discontent?

    HarvardBusiness

    Review

    24:

    117-125.

    DeBoer,

    Connie

    1978 The

    polls:

    attitudes oward work.

    Public

    Opinion

    Quarterly

    42: 414-423.

    Fournet,

    Glenn

    P.,

    M. K.

    Distefano, Jr.,

    and

    Margaret

    W.

    Pryer

    1966

    Job

    satisfaction: ssues and

    problems.

    Personnel

    Psychology

    19:

    165-183.

    Friedlander,

    Frank

    1965

    Comparative

    work

    value

    systems.

    Personnel

    Psychology

    18:

    1-20.

    Friedman,

    E. A.

    and R. J.

    Havighurst

    1962 Work and

    retirement.

    Pp.

    41-55 in

    S.

    Nosow and

    W. H. Form

    (eds.),

    Man,

    Work,

    and

    Society.

    New York:Basic Books.

    Glenn,

    Norval D.

    1981

    Age,

    birth

    cohorts,

    and

    drinking:

    an

    illustration

    f

    the hazardsof

    inferring

    effects from cohort data.

    Journalof

    Gerontology

    36:

    362-369.

    Glenn,

    Norval

    and

    Charles

    N.

    Weaver

    1982a

    Further

    vidence

    on

    education

    and

    job

    satisfaction. Social Forces

    61:

    46-55.

    1982b

    Enjoyment

    of work

    by

    full-time workers

    in the United

    states, 1966,

    and 1980. Public

    Opinion

    Quarterly

    46:

    459-470.

    Glenn,

    Norval

    D.,

    Patricia A.

    Taylor,

    and

    Charles

    N. Weaver

    1977

    Age

    and

    job

    satisfaction

    among

    males

    and females: a

    multivariate,

    multisurvey

    survey.

    Journal

    of

    Applied

    Psychology

    62:

    189-193.

    Golembiewski,

    Robert B.

    1977 Testing some stereotypes about the sexes in organizations:differential satisfaction with work?

    Human

    Resource

    Management

    16: 30-32.

    Goldthrope,

    John

    H.,

    David

    Lockwood,

    Frank

    Bechofer,

    and Jennifer

    Platt

    1969 The

    Affluent Worker n

    the Class

    Structure.London:

    CambridgeUniversity

    Press.

    Gruenberg,

    Barry

    1980

    The

    happy

    worker: an

    analysis

    of

    educational and

    occupational

    differences

    in determinantsof

    job

    satisfaction.

    American

    Journalof

    Sociology

    86: 247-271.

    Gruneberg,

    Michael M.

    1979

    Understanding

    Job

    Satisfaction. New

    York:

    Wiley.

    Hackman,

    RichardJ.

    and Edward E.

    Lawler III

    1971

    Employee

    reactions to

    job

    characteristics. Journalof

    Applied

    Psychology Monography

    55: 259-

    286.

    Hackman, RichardJ. and Greg R. Oldham

    1975

    Development

    of

    the

    job

    diagnostic

    survey.

    Journalof

    Applied

    Psychology

    60: 159-170.

    1979

    Work

    Redesign. Reading,

    MA:

    Addison-Wesley.

    Herzberg,

    Frederick

    1966 Work

    and the

    Nature of Man.

    Cleveland,

    OH: World.

    Kalleberg,

    Arne L.

    1977

    Work

    values and

    job

    rewards:a

    theory

    of

    job

    satisfaction. American

    Sociological

    Review 42: 124-

    143.

    Katz,

    Ralph

    1978 Job

    longevity

    as a

    situational actor in

    job

    satisfaction.

    AdministrativeScience

    Quarterly

    23: 204-

    223.

    Katz,

    Ralph

    and John

    Van Maanen

    1977 The loci of work satisfaction: ob interaction,and policy. Human Relations 30: 469-486.

    Katzell,

    Raymond

    A.

    1979

    Changing

    attitudes

    oward

    work.

    Pp.

    35-57 in

    C.

    Kerr

    and J. M. Rosow

    (eds.),

    Work

    n

    America,

    New York:

    Van Nostrand.

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 4106221.pdf

    21/22

    384

    THE

    SOCIOLOGICAL

    UARTERLY

    ol. 26/No.

    3/1985

    King,

    Michael,

    Michael

    A.

    Murry,

    and

    Tom

    Atkinson

    1982

    Background,

    personality,

    job

    characteristics,

    and

    satisfaction

    with work in

    a national

    sample.

    Human

    Relations 35:

    119-133.

    Kirsch,

    Barbara

    A.

    and

    Joseph

    J.

    Lengermann

    1971

    An

    empirical

    test of Robert

    Baluner's ideas on

    alienation

    in work

    as

    applied

    to

    different

    type

    jobs

    in a

    white-collar

    setting.

    Sociology

    and Social Research

    56:

    180-194.

    Locke,

    Edwin A.

    1969 What is

    job

    satisfaction?

    Organizational

    Behavior

    and Human

    Performance

    4:

    309-336.

    1973 Satisfiers nd

    dissatisfiers

    mong

    white-collar

    ndblue-collar

    mployees.

    Journal f

    Applied

    Psychology

    58: 67-76.

    1976 The natureand

    consequences

    of

    job

    satisfaction.

    Pp.

    1297-1349

    in M.

    D.

    Dunnette

    ed.),

    Handbook

    of Industrialand

    Organizational

    Psychology.

    Chicago:

    Rand

    McNally.

    McGregor,

    Douglas

    1960

    The

    Human

    Side

    of

    Enterprise.

    New York:

    McGraw-Hill.

    Morse,

    Nancy

    and Robert

    Weiss

    1955 The

    function

    and

    meaning

    of

    work

    and the

    job.

    American

    Sociological

    Review

    20:

    191-198.

    Mottaz,

    Clifford

    1981 Some

    determinants

    of

    work

    alienation. The

    Sociological Quarterly

    22:

    515-529.

    1983

    Alienation

    among

    police

    officers. Journalof

    Police

    Science and

    Administration

    11: 23-30.

    Oldham,

    Greg

    R.

    1976 Job

    characteristics

    and internal

    motivation:

    the

    moderating

    effect

    of

    interpersonal

    and

    individual

    variables. Human Relations

    29:

    559-569.

    Orpen, Christopher

    1976 Social

    desirability

    s

    a moderator f

    the

    relationship

    etween

    ob

    satisfaction

    nd

    personal

    adjustment.

    In

    Michael

    Gruneberg

    ed.),

    Job

    Satisfaction.

    New York:

    Halsted.

    Phillips,

    Derek L.

    1973

    Abandoning

    Method.

    San Francisco:

    Jossey-Bass.

    Pierce,

    Jon L.

    and Randall B.

    Dunham

    1976 Task

    design:

    a

    literature

    review.

    Academy

    of

    Management

    Review

    1:

    83-97.

    Quinn,

    Robert P. and

    GrahamL. Staines

    1979 The

    1977

    Quality

    of

    Employment

    Survey.

    Ann

    Arbor,

    MI: Institute

    for

    Social Research.

    Robinson,

    J.

    P.,

    R.

    Athanasiou,

    and K.

    Head

    1969

    Measures

    of

    Occupational

    Attitudes

    and

    Occupational

    Characteristics.

    Ann

    Arbor,

    MI:

    University

    of

    Michigan

    Press.

    Ronan,

    W. W.

    1970 Relative

    importance

    of

    job

    characteristics.

    Journalof

    Applied

    Psychology

    54:

    192-200.

    Seeman,

    Melvin

    1967

    On the

    personal

    consequence

    of

    alienation

    n

    work.

    American

    Sociological

    Review

    32: 273-285.

    Seybolt,

    John

    W. and

    Leopold

    Gruenfeld

    1976 The

    discriminant

    alidity

    of

    work

    alienation

    and work satisfactionmeasures. Journal

    f

    Occupational

    Psychology

    49:

    193-202.

    Sheppard,

    Harold L.

    and

    Neal

    Q.

    Herrick

    1972 Where Have All

    the Robots

    Gone?

    New

    York:

    The Free Press.

    Simonds,

    Rolin H. and

    John N. Orife

    1976 Workerbehavior

    versus enrichment

    heory.

    AdministrativeScience

    Quarterly

    20: 606-612.

    Sims,

    Henry

    P.

    and

    Andrew D.

    Szilagyi

    1976

    Job

    characteristic

    elationships:

    ndividual

    and structuralmoderators.

    Organizational

    Behavior

    and

    Human

    Performance 17:

    211-230.

    Smith, P.,

    L.

    Kendall,

    and C. Hulin

    1969 The

    Measurementof

    Satisfaction in

    Work and

    Retirement.

    Chicago:

    Rand

    McNally.

    Tausky,

    Curt

    1969

    Meanings

    of

    work

    among

    blue-collar men.

    Pacific

    Sociological

    Review

    12:

    49-55.

    This content downloaded from 111.68.106.133 on Tue, 18 Jun 2013 05:26:02 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10