1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a...

download 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation of Paranoid Processes-Pergamon Press (1975)_Part7

of 10

Transcript of 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a...

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    1/10

    The Central Processes of the Model 57

    remember

    del stmt, to

    which interviewer

    is about to

    respond

    hoosedel

    D

    fchange

    * | I subject J

    Fig. 5.5.

    the algorithm), the program will refuse to d iscuss Mafia topics at all since

    it is too upset to talk about this most sensitive area.

    To make some of these operations more intelligible, let us consider

    interview examples. Suppose at some point in the interview the doctor

    asks a standard

    first interview

    question as follows:

    (5) Dr.DO YOU EVER HAVE THE FEELING YOU ARE

    BEING WATCHED?

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    2/10

    58 Artificial Paranoia

    Flg. 5.6 Directed graph of flare concepts.

    If this is the first reference to the delusional net, FEAR will increase

    greatly and the linguistic response will be:

    (6) Pt.YOU KNOW, THEY KNOW ME.

    In making this response, the model must expect from the interviewer a

    number of typical questions of the wh-type as well as rejoinder state

    ments. The use of

    they

    by the interviewerinhis response to the m odel's

    output is assumed to be an anaphoric reference to the they PARRY is

    talking about. Although it is likely the interviewer will react to the m odel's

    output of (6), the algorithm must be prepared for the possibility that he

    will change the topic. Hence if the interviewer at this point asks some

    non-sequitur question such as:

    (7) Dr.HOW LONG HAVE YOU BEEN IN HOSPITAL?

    the program recognizes that no reference to the delusional topic has been

    made and answers the question just as though it had been asked in any

    other context. This ability to deal with input in a flexible manner is

    important because of the many contingencies that can occur in psychiat

    ric dialogues.

    If the topic is changed abruptly in this way by an interviewer, the

    algorithm remembers that it has output its

    first

    delusional statement of

    (6).

    When the interviewer makes another neutral delusional reference, the

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    3/10

    The Central Processes of the Model 59

    next line of the delusional story will be output, e.g.,

    8

    Pt.THE MAFIA REALLY K N O W ABOUT ME.

    The ability to answer typical wh- and how questions depends on how

    much conceptual information is contained in the delusional belief being

    addressed. For example, suppose PARRY replied as in (6):

    (6) Pt.YOU KNOW, THEY KNOW ME.

    and the interviewer then asked:

    (9) Dr.WHERE DO THEY KNOW ABOUT YOU?

    If the expectancy-anaphoras contain no where, then a question about

    location cannot be answered. In this default situation, the algorithm

    recognizes the anaphoric they, know and you. Hence it knows at

    least that the topic has not been changed so it outputs the next statement

    in the delusional story:

    (9) pt.THEY KNOW WHO I AM.

    and again anticipates questions and rejoinders pertaining to this state

    ment.

    In constructing the data-base of beliefs, we tried to pack as much

    information in each belief as any reasonable (like ourselves) inter

    viewer might request. However, one cannot anticipate everything and

    when some unanticipated information is requested , another relevant reply

    must be substituted. This heuristic may seem less than perfect but there is

    little else to do when the model simply lacks the pertinent information. B y

    the way, humans do this also.

    When the interviewer shows interest in the delusional story, PARRY

    continues to output assertions appropriate to the dialogue. However,

    when the interviewer expresses doubt or disbelief about the delusions,

    ANGER and FEAR increase and the interviewer becomes questioned as

    in:

    10)

    Pt.YOU DON T BELIEVE

    ME,

    DO YOU?

    Such an output expression attempts to prompt the dialogue toward the

    relation between the interviewer and the model, which will be described

    later (see p. 65).

    If no delusional reference at all is detected by this procedure, the

    algorithm attempts the next function that searches for certain types of

    references to the

    self

    (See Fig. 5.7.)

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    4/10

    60 Artificial Paran oia

    Self References

    Since

    the

    main concern

    of a

    psychiatric interview consists

    of the

    beliefs, feelings, state s,andactionsofthe patient,themodel mustbeable

    to answer

    a

    large number

    of

    questions about

    its Self.

    If

    the

    input

    is

    recognized

    as a

    question

    and no

    topic

    is

    currently under

    discussion

    and the

    question refers

    to the Self,

    then

    it is

    assumed

    temporarily that it will refer only to a main self-topic. These mainor

    intro duc tory self-topics (age, sex, marriage, health, family, occupation,

    hospital stay, etc.)inturn hav e subtopicstovarying depths .Forexam ple,

    suppose

    the

    interviewer asks:

    (11) Dr.HOW

    DO YOU

    LIKE

    THE

    HOSPITAL?

    Since hospital

    is a

    main intro du ctor y topic with several levels

    of

    subtopics,the algorithm answers the question with:

    1 2 Pt.ISHOULDN T HAVE COME HERE.

    and then anticipates

    a

    variety

    of

    likely questions such

    as

    W hat brought

    you

    to the

    hospital? ,

    How

    long have

    you

    been

    in the

    hospital? ,

    How

    do

    you get

    along with

    the

    other patients? ,

    etc.

    Each

    of

    these questions

    brings

    up

    further topics, some

    of

    which represent

    a

    continuation

    of the

    main topic hospital, butothers of which represent a shift to another

    main introductory topic,

    e.g.,

    other patien ts. Since many

    of the

    inputs

    of

    the

    interviewer consist

    of

    ellipses

    or

    fragments,

    the

    algorithm assum es

    them

    to

    refer

    to the

    topic

    or

    subtopic under discussion.

    If

    some topic

    is

    being discussed,thealgorithm checks firstfor anew m ain topic, thenfor a

    follow-upto the last subtopic, then (unlessthesubtopicisitself a main

    topic,as forexample other patie nts in theabove)for afollow-upto the

    last main topic. Thus continuity and coherence in the dialogue are

    maintained.

    If some meaning cannot

    be

    extracted from

    the

    question

    but it is

    recognized

    at

    least that

    a

    question

    is

    being asked,

    a

    procedure

    is

    called

    that attempts

    to

    handle certain common miscellaneous questions that

    are

    difficult

    to

    categorize. These include

    the

    space-time orientation questions

    ( What

    day is

    this? )

    and

    everyday information

    ( Who is

    president? )

    asked

    by

    psychiatrists

    in a

    mental-status examination

    to

    test

    a

    pa tient's

    awareness

    and

    orientation. Some quantitative

    how

    questions

    ( how

    many,

    how

    often,

    how

    long )

    are

    recognized here. Since

    any

    adjective

    or

    adverb

    can

    follow

    a

    how,

    one of the

    limitations

    of the

    model

    is its

    inability

    to

    handle

    all of

    them satisfactorily because

    the

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    5/10

    The Central Processes of the Model

    61

    IreturrNy

    f a l s e / ^

    ^^wit l i^S .

    direct r e f ^ i yS

    s ^ o s e l f ? ^ ^

    N

    TR E P LY

    [\

    .suspicious y

    W i e s t i o n /

    ^ X w i t h S . v

    ^ direct ref >

    S J o s e l f ? ^

    N*

    yreturrK

    false X

    . a u e s t i o n ? ^ a

    N |

    NORMAL 1

    REPLY

    return false}

    J defensive ]

    reply J

    ^^

    'ANSWERA

    answer |

    suspiciously

    Fig. 5.7.

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    6/10

    62 Artificial Parano ia

    relevant information is lacking in the data-base. If absolutely no clues are

    recognized in the question, the algorithm is forced to output

    noncommit

    tal reply such as:

    (13) Pt.WELL, I DON'T KNOW.

    This function also checks for statements about the self that are taken to

    be insulting or complimentary. Naturally the presence of a negator in the

    input reverses the meaning. Thus:

    (14) Dr.YOU DONT SEEM VERY ALERT.

    is classified as an insult whereas:

    (15) Dr.YOU ARE RIGHT.

    is considered complimentary and benevolent.

    Among the introductory self-topics are those that constitute sensitive

    areas, e.g., sex, religion, and family. If the interviewer refers to one of

    these areas, the value of ANGER increases sharply and a response is

    selected from one of the lists categorized as hostile, defensive,

    personal, or guarded, depending on the level of MISTRUST at the

    moment. For example, if the interviewer asks a question about PARRY'S

    sex life, it first replies with:

    (16) Pt.MY SEX LIFE IS MY OWN BUSINESS.

    If the interviewer persists or even later tries to ask about sex, the model

    will respond with a hostile reply, such as:

    17 Pt.DO YOU KNOW WHAT YOU ARE DOING?

    The particular sensitive areas in the model are part of the initial

    conditions specific for this hypothetical patient. Of course, these topics

    are commonly found to be sensitive areas in human patients.

    The model operates sequentially trying one major process after

    another. If it has come this far (i.e., having tested for special reactions,

    delusional references, and self-references without recognizing anything in

    the input pertinent to these procedures), it proceeds to the next process,

    which handles flare references. (See Figs. 5.8 and 5.9.)

    Flare References

    The data-base contains a directed graph of concepts involved in the

    model's stories. PARRY has small stories to tell about horseracing,

    gambling, bookies, etc. The major concepts of these stories are termed

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    7/10

    The Central Processes of the Model 63

    flare conce pts since they activate stories that are differently weighted

    in the graph. (See Fig. 5.6.)

    In the strong version of the mod el, the concept M afia is given the

    highest weight, while in the weak version the concept R ac ke ts is most

    heavily weighted. In both versions H or se s has the lowest weight. The

    weights are assigned to the concepts and not individual words or

    word-groups denoting the concepts.

    The graph is directed in the sense that reference to horseracing elicits

    the first line of a story about horseracing . When a story is ended, a prom pt

    is given to the interviewer to discuss the next story in the graph, which

    involves bo ok ies . The model strives to tell its stories under approp riate

    conditions and leads the interviewer along paths of increasing delusional

    relevance. Much depends on whether the interviewer follows these leads

    benev olently and reacts to the prom pts.

    The first step in this procedure is to scan the input for a flare concept

    having the highest weight. Thus if a flare concept is already under

    discussion, a weaker new flare will be disrega rded. If th e flare concept is

    one in a story that has already been partially told, then a prom pt is offered

    regarding the next story-node in the graph.

    If a question is asked about the events of a story, the model tries to

    answer it. Also the model is sensitive to whether the interviewer is

    showing interest in the story or whether he tries to change the subject or

    expresses a negative attitude, such as

    disbelief.

    If the interviewer indicates a positive attitude toward the story, then

    benevolence is recognized and the variables of ANGER, FEAR, and

    MISTRUST decrease slightly after each I O pair. ANG ER decreases

    more rapidly than FEAR while MISTRUST, being a more stable variable

    once it has risen, decreases least.

    If n o flare conc epts a re recognized in the inpu t, the model next tries to

    detect if a reference is being made to the relation betw een th e interviewer

    and the model. In an interview interaction there exist two situations, the

    one being talked about and the one the participants are in at the moment.

    Sometimes the latter situation becomes the former, i.e. the one talked

    about.

    Interviewer-Interviewee Relations

    As described in Chapter 4, the algorithm must be ready to handle input

    referring to the relation between interviewer and model. The simplest

    cases are exemplified by expressions such as:

    (18) Dr.I UNDERSTAND YOU.

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    8/10

    e

    flareref

    I

    D

    FLAREMECH

    respond to

    flare and

    delete as flare

    FLAREMECH

    respond to

    flare and

    delete as

    flare

    FLARE REPLY

    suspicious

    question

    - * silence j

    LEAD ON

    ANSFLARE

    answer

    RESPFLARE

    respond

    I CHECK TOPICI

    | check

    to see whether

    interviewer is

    changing topic

    ask leading

    question about

    next higher

    flare

    Fig. 5.8.

    64

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    9/10

    checkflare

    D

    _/TY.

    set new flare =

    this wo rd

    set result = true

    set global

    flare to new

    flare

    Fig. 5.9.

    65

  • 8/12/2019 1.Kenneth Mark Colby, Arnold P. Goldstein and Leonard Krasner (Auth.)-Artificial Paranoia. a Computer Simulation

    10/10

    66 Artificial Paran oia

    (19) Dr.YOU DO NOT TRUST ME.

    Those phrases in an expression that can appear between I and yo u or

    betw een yo u and m e we classified as representing a positive or

    negative attitude on the part of the interviewer. Thus expression (18) is

    taken to be positive whe reas

    19)

    is negative becau se, although it contains a

    positive verb, the verb is negated.

    If a positive attitude is expressed by the interviewer, FEAR and

    ANGER decrease. FEAR and ANGER increase depending on the con

    ceptualizations of the input. These attitudes of the interviewer, as

    interpreted by the model, are reflected in the values of the affect-

    variables.

    Associated in the data base with each type of attitude expression

    expected are lists of appropriate output expressions. Thus in reply to:

    (20) Dr.I UNDERSTAND YOU.

    the model would reply:

    (21) Pt. GLAD YOU DO.

    or

    (22) Pt.I APPRECIATE YOUR TRYING TO UNDERSTAND.

    or some equivalent expression depending on values of the affect-

    variables. When ANGER and FEAR are high, positive attitude expres

    sions are interpreted as insincerity and hence evoke hostile replies.

    The remainder of input expression types thus far not discussed are

    handled by a procedure for miscellaneous expressions.

    Miscellaneous Expressions

    This procedure deals with all those interviewer ex pressions from which

    no clear conceptualization can be formed. The only thing that can be

    determined is perhaps the sentence-type of the input. Presented with one

    of these ex pression s, if FE AR is extremely high PARR Y signs off without

    a farewell expression and cannot be contacted through further natural

    language input. If FEAR is high but not extreme, and the input is

    recognized as a question, the model chooses a reply from a list that brings

    up the attitude of the interviewer as in:

    (23) Pt.WHY DO YOU WANT TO KNOW?