Who’s watching you? Power, personalization and on-line compliance. Adam Joinson Institute of...

Post on 16-Jan-2016

221 views 0 download

Tags:

Transcript of Who’s watching you? Power, personalization and on-line compliance. Adam Joinson Institute of...

Who’s watching you? Power, personalization and on-line compliance.

Adam JoinsonInstitute of Educational Technology,

The Open University

Acknowledgements: Ulf Reips, Alan Woodley, Tom Buchanan.

CSI: Miami

“You have zero privacy anyway ... Get over it” (Scott McNealy, CEO, SUN Microsystems, 1999)

• What are the main threats to privacy?– Personalization, commodification, ubiquity, data-mining

• Implications for online behaviour, specifically compliance to survey requests

Personalization and privacy

• Personalized online experiences have some (questionable) benefits – e.g. special offers, e-learning profiles

• But, vast quantities of personal data collected in the name of personalization

• Used extensively in survey methodology – Difficult to personalize and maintain privacy..

Commodification

• How much is your personal data worth?– Safety from terror attack?

– Entry to the USA?

– Access to the NY Times?

– Access to community?• Expressive vs. Informational privacy (DeCew, 1999)

– $2.50 reduction in shopping?

• Generally, we undervalue our personal information – 70% will give it away for a raffle.

Ubiquity

• Always on networks (phone, WLANs, bluetooth)

• RFID tags (products, passports)

• Camera phones

• Identity theft and authentication– Biometrics, live databases

– Number plate recognition

• Ubicomp aims to make life easier … which means seamless authentication.

Data-mining

• Storage is cheap• How long are the following kept for?

– Your Amazon purchase history?– Your google searches?– Your fingerprint, photo and personal information on entry to the

US?– (ever, until 2038, 75-100 years).

• Ever more sophisticated software• Amazon – profiling at who you send gifts to.• Social software, meta-tagging and semantic web pose new

issues

Privacy and online behavior• Internet solves the privacy-

intimacy paradox (Ben Ze’ev, 2003)

– Require privacy for intimacy, intimacy reduces privacy

• DeCew (1998): Expressive vs. Informational privacy

– More useful to talk about pseudonmyity?

• But, it’s not clear exactly how this will influence behaviour

– Buying embarrassing items online vs. in a shop

– Rejection risk and e-mail vs. a quick chat (Joinson, 2004)

– Why leave a data-trail?• Examining the micro-environment

of surveys.

Surveys, privacy and social influence • Surveys pose a privacy challenge for respondents

– Most surveys stress anonymity / confidentiality – for this reason.– Technological mediation and improved responses (Tourangeau, 2004)

• Personalization and status used widely to improve falling response rates (Dillman, 1992; 2000; Tourangeau, 2004)– But both fairly unstable predictors.– Reasons for this variability unknown .. – Status usually confounded with power– Anonymity reduced? (Andreasen, 1970)

• Powerful audience + identifiability to out-group = – Suppression of in-group norms (Reicher & Levine, 1994)– Compliance / conformity to request?– Resistance? (Levine, 2000) – by exaggerating non-punishable behaviours.

Overview of experiments

• PRESTO – Mass personalized e-mailing

– Automated web surveys

– Automated reporting

• Used for institutional research with panels– Open University students recruited

• Studying personalization, privacy, disclosure, power and response rates

• This paper: Initial 7 experiments.

• Manipulation checks difficult (but, new panel recruited to allow)

Experiment 1

• Stratified sample of 10,000 OU students (Panel 2)

• E-mail signed by vice-chancellor• Four salutations used • Signing onto panel = dependent variable

• Overall sign-up rate: 15.5% (22% Panel 1)

Experiment 1: Results– Dear Student 317 13.9%– Dear OU Student 316 13.7%– Dear John Doe 378 16.4%– Dear John 418 18.2%

• Chi-square = 24.39, df (3), p < .000. • Odds-ratios: Dear John vs. Dear Student (1.4, p

< .001). • Personalized salutation increased odds of response

by almost 40%

Overall model

• Logistic regression• Age, salutation, loyalty, gender • Overall model: Chi-Square = 199.96 (df =

6), p < .001– Age (Wald = 105.3, p < .000)– Salutation (Wald = 27.53, p < .000)– Gender (Wald = 19.58, p < 0.00)– Loyalty (Wald = 13.27, p < .000)

Experiment 2

• Staff survey – Professors – Cleaners.

• 4226 e-mail invitations sent February – March 2005.

• 74% Response Rate

• (from two reminders).

• Signed by pro-vice chancellor

• Quite a personalization effect…

Experiment 2: Results

• Dear John: 1738 (82% RR)

• Dear Colleague: 1391 (66% RR)– Effect across staff categories

• Reminder manipulation: ‘1700’ vs. ‘some’– No impact of personal responsibility (some

responses vs. over 1700 responses). – Salutation effect remained.

Why the strong effect?

• Mindless action in response to personal salutation?

• Reciprocal response to effort?• Increased sense of responsibility /

‘specialness’?• Strategic response?• More likely to be read?

Experiment Three:

• Tested mindless / likely to read hypotheses• Used Panel 1 (N = 2247). Recruited late 2002• A reverse replication of experiment one:

– Email from vice-chancellor (same word count, sign-off etc)

– Asking if they want to leave the panel– Same web form as signing on, but to sign off

• Salutation manipulated as per experiment one

Experiment Three: Results Salutation

Dear Student Dear Open

University

Student

Dear John

Doe

Dear John

Number of

responses

32 28 21 22

Response

rate (%)

5.7 5.0 3.7 3.9

Experiment three: Analyses

• Personalized vs. non-personalized combined

• Odds ratio = 1.416 (Chi-Square = 2.93, p = 0.05).

• Logistic regression: salutation (p = 0.06), age (ns) and gender (ns).

• Overall model: ns (p = .11)

Power?

• Personalized salutation might serve to reduce sense of privacy / anonymity (Andreason, 1970)

• Power / status may be crucial when anonymity removed (Levine, 2000)

• When identifiability is combined with high power / status (and possible sanction), then there

is a strong incentive to comply.

For instance…

Sender: Professor Fred Perry, PresidentTo: A.N.JoinsonDate: 6th Feb 2004

Dear Adam,

I’m writing to remind you to complete the latest version of the staff survey. The original invitation was sent two weeks ago.

Professor Fred Perry, President, The Open University

As opposed to…

Sender: F.Perry

To: A.N.Joinson

Date: 6th Feb 2004

Dear colleague,

I’m writing to remind you to complete the latest version of the staff survey. The original invitation was sent two weeks ago.

Fred Perry, The Open University

Experiment 5

• Another panel (n=2137)• Salutation manipulated (as per usual)• Power / status manipulated • Invitation signed by pro-vice chancellor: either

– <name> (Strategy and Planning) The Open University– Professor <name> Pro-vice chancellor, (Strategy and

Planning), The Open University

• Manipulation placed at top and bottom of e-mail body (but not e-mail address).

Experiment 5: Results (response rates)

Dear John Dear John

Doe

Dear Student

High power 190 (53.4) 154 (43.3) 150 (42.1)

Neutral power 166 (46.6) 158 (44.4) 143 (40.1)

100110

120130

140150

160170

180190

200

Dear John Dear John Doe Dear Student

High Power

Neutral Power

Experiment 5: Analyses• Salutation (Chi-Square (df = 2) = 8.92, p < .02) • Power (Chi-Square (df =1) = 1.25, p = .23, ns).

• Logistic regression (Method: Stepwise Forward Conditional). DV = response rate– Power*Salutation interaction significant (SE = 0.27, Wald = 9.3,

df = 1, p<.01)

• Effect size of salutation by power:– high power (X = 10.97 (df = 2), Contingency co-efficient = 0.10, p

< .01) – low power (X = 3.24 (df = 2), Contingency co-efficient = 0.05, p

= .2, ns).

Experiments 6 and 7• Examine the impact of different

personalization techniques on self-disclosure to a sensitive question

• Two types of non-disclosure:• Good (I don’t want to say) vs• Bad (Submission of no

response)

• Study 6: Power and Personalized salutation

• Study 7: Personalized URL vs. Lon-on procedure

Experiment 6

• Another panel (combined 3544 people) • 1,617 (45.6%) response rate• 2 x 2 design

– Power of sponsor (high vs. neutral) • In e-mail invitation and front sheet• Title and job given (or not)

– Salutation (Dear John vs. Presto panel member)

• DV - Salary disclosure and response rates.

Response Rates (raw & %)

Dear John Dear Presto panel member

High power 432 (48.8) 401 (45.3)

Neutral power 391 (44.1) 393 (44.4)

Power: (2 = 2.71, df = 1, p = .053, Odds Ratio = 1.12)Salutation: (2 = 0.96, df = 1, p = .17, ns, Odds Ratio = 1.07).

Salutation / High power: (2 = 2.18, df = 1, p = .077, Odds Ratio = 1.15). Salutation / Low power: (2 = 0.01, df = 1, p > .9, Odds Ratio = 0.99).

Non-disclosure and salutation

‘I prefer not to answer’

Disclosed salary

Did not submit a selection

Personalized High Power

48 (10.5%) 376 (82.6%) 31 (6.8%)

Personalized Low Power

46 (10.9%) 340 (80.6%) 36 (8.5%)

Impersonal High Power

39 (9.1%) 352 (82.9%) 38 (8.9%)

Impersonal Low Power

37 (8.6%) 343 (79.8%) 50 (11.6%)

Total 1411 (87.3) 170 (10.5) 36 (2.2)

Salutation: (2 = 4.47, df = 2, p = 0.05)Power: (2 = 2.80, df = 2, p = 0.10)

Summary

• Personalized salutation increases response rates when combined with high power

• This has two effects– Increased ‘good’ non-disclosure via ‘don’t want to say’– Reduced ‘bad’ non-disclosure via submission of no

option

• Difference is the type of non-disclosure, not non-disclosure itself

• Evidence of a privacy bind?

Privacy bind?

• Completion of surveys poses a privacy problem for people when it’s sensitive information

• Use of anonymity mitigates this, but also enables ‘poor responding’ (via skipped questions)

• When it’s a powerful source, and personalized salutation, the participant is in a bind between compliance and privacy concerns

• This doesn’t exist for non-personalized, low power senders.

• The provision of ‘don’t want to say’ options provides a way out of this bind…compliant resistance?

Experiment 7

• Question: Confirming the role of identifiability?• Authentication method (Log-on vs. Encoded

URL)• Presto panel (1144 people) 633 women and 507

men (data missing for 4 participants), • Mean age of 43.6 years (SD = 10.44)• DV - Salary disclosure.

Disclosure

Disclosed salary ‘I prefer not to say’

Encoded URL 587 (97.0%) 18 (3.0%)

Log-on 488 (94.4%) 29 (5.6%)

(2 = 4.18, df = 1, p = .041, Odds Ratio = 1.94).

General Summary

• Personalization removes anonymity– Compliance behavior in surveys– Reduced disclosure (in a nice way), greater social desirability

• Loss of informational privacy also reduces expressive privacy– Unless the informational privacy resides elsewhere?– Pseudonymity rather than anonymity (Identity dissimulation)?

• The issue is who is watching – power critical, not just removal of anonymity – And inevitably, trust.

Thank you!

adam@joinson.com

http://www.joinson.com(slides next week)

http://www.prisd.net (privacy project)